* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Loudness, Its Definition, Measurement and
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
OCTOBER, ][933 J. A. S. A. VOLUME V Loudness, Its Definition, Measurement and Calculation H^RVE¾FLETCHER^St) W. A. MVNSO•, Bell TelephoneLaboratories (ReceivedAugust 28, 1933) INTRODUCTION ear receiving it, and the physiological and psychologicalconditionsof the listener. In most engineeringproblemswe are interested mainly in describe the magnitude of an auditory senthe effect upon a typical observer who is in a sation. Although we use the terms "very loud," typical condition for listening. "loud," "moderately loud," "soft" and "very In a paper during 1921 one of us suggested soft," corresponding to the musical notations using the number of decibelsabove threshold as a if, f, mf, p, and pp, to define the magnitude, it is measure of loudnessand some experimental data evident that these terms are not at all precise were presentedon this basis. As more data were and depend upon the experience, the auditory accumulated it was evident that such a basis for acuity, and the customs of the persons using defining loudnessmust be abandoned. them. If loudness depended only upon the inIn 1924 in a paper by Steinberg and Fletcher1 tensity of the soundwave producingthe loudness, somedata were given which showedthe effectsof then measurements of the physical intensity eliminating certain frequency bands upon the would definitely determine the loudnessas sensed loudnessof the sound. By using such data as a by a typical individual and therefore could be basis, a mathematical formula was given for used as a precisemeans of defining it. However, calculating the loudness lossesof a sound being no such simple relation exists. transmitted to the ear, due to changes in the The magnitude of an auditory sensation, that transmissionsystem. The formula was limited in is, the loudness of the sound, is probably deits application to the particular soundsstudied, pendent upon the total number of nerve impulses namely, speechand a soundwhich was generated that reach the brain per second along the by an electrical buzzer and called the test tone. auditory tract. It is evident that these auditory In 1925 Steinberg2 developed a formula for phenomena are dependent not alone upon the calculating the loudnessof any complex sound. intensity of the sound but also upon their The results computed by this formula agreed physical composition. For example, if a person with the data which were then available. Howlistened to a flute and then to a bassdrum placed ever, as more data have accumulated it has been at such distancesthat the soundscoming from found to be inadequate. Since that time conthe two instruments are judged to be equally siderably more information concerning the loud, then, the intensity of the sound at the ear mechanism of hearing has been discovered and produced by the bass drum would be many the technique in making loudnessmeasurements times that producedby the flute. has advanced. Also more powerful methods for OUDNESS isapsychological term Used to If the composition of the sound, that is, its producing complex tones of any known compowave form, is held constant, but its intensity at sition are now available. For these reasons and the ear of the listener varied, then the loudness because of the demand for a loudness formula of produced will be the same for the same intensity general application, especially in connection with only if the same or an equivalent ear is receiving noise measurements, the whole subject was the sound and also only if the listener is in the reviewed by the Bell Telephone Laboratories and same psychologicaland physiologicalconditions, with reference to fatigue, attention, alertness, • H. Fletcher and J. C. Steinberg, Loudnessof a Co•nplex etc. Therefore, in order to determine the loudness Sound, Phys. Rev. 24, 306 (1924). produced, it is necessaryto define the intensity • J. C. Steinberg, The Loudnessof a Sound and Its of the sound,its physical composition, the kind of Physical Stimulus, Phys. Rev. 26, 507 (1925). 82 LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT AND CALCULATION 83 centimeter. In a plane or spherical progressive sound wave in air, this intensity correspondsto a root-mean-squarepressurep givenby the formula the work reported in the present paper undertaken. This work has resulted in better experimental methods for determining the loudness level of any sustained complex sound and a formula which gives calculated results in agreement with the great variety of loudness data wherep is expressedin bars,I[ is the heightof the which are now available. barometer in centimeters, and 7' is the absolute p=o.ooo207[-(tœ/76)(273/T)•:' (2) temperature. At a temperature of 20øC and a pressureof 76 cm of Hg, p = 0.000204 bar. DEFINITIONS The subject matter which follows necessitates Intensity level of terms which have often The intensity level of a soundis the numberof been applied in very inexact ways in the past. db above the reference intensity. Becauseof the increasein interest and activity in this field, it became desirable to obtain a general Reference tone agreement concerningthe meaning of the terms A planeor sphericalsoundwave havingonly a which are most frequently used. The following single frequency of 1000 cyclesper secondshall definitions are taken from recent proposals of be usedas the referencefor loudnesscomparisons. the use of a number the sectional committee on Acoustical Measure- ments and Terminology of the American Standards Association and the terms have been used with these meaningsthroughout the paper. Note: One practical way to obtain a plane or sphericalwave is to use a small source,and to have the head of the observer at least one meter distant from the source, with the external conditions such titat reflected waves are negligible as Sound intensity The sound intensity of a sound field in a specifieddirection at a point is the sound energy transmitted per unit of time in the specified direction through a unit area normal to this direction at the point. In the case of a plane or spherical free pro- comparedwith the original wave at the head of the observer. Loudness level The loudnesslevel of any sound shall be the intensity level of the equally loud referencetone at the position where the listener's head is to be gressivewave having the effectivesoundpressure placed. P (bars), the velocity of propagationc (cm per sec.) in a medium of density p (grams per cubic Manner of listening to the sound cm), the intensity in the direction of propagation In observing the loudness of the reference is given by sound, the observer shall face the source, which J=P2/pc (ergsper sec.per sq. cm). (1) This same relation can often be used in practice with sufficientaccuracyto calculatethe intensity at a point near the sourcewith only a pressure measurement. In more complicated sound fields the results given by this relation may differ greatly from the actual intensity. When dealing with a plane or a spherical progressive •vave it will be understood that the should be small, and listen with both ears at a position so that the distancefrom the sourceto a line joining the two ears is one meter. The value of the intensity level of the equally loud reference..sound depends upon the manner of listening to the unknown sound and also to the standard of reference.The manner of listening to the unknown sound may be considered as part of the characteristics of that sound. The manner of listening to the reference sound is as specified intensity is taken in the direction of propagation above. of the wave. Loudness has been briefly Reference intensity defined as the magnitude of an auditory sensation, and more The reference intensity for intensity level will be said about this later, but it will be seen comparisonsshall be 10 la watts per square from the above definitions that the loudness level 84 HARVEY FLETCHER of any sound is obtained by adjusting the intensity level of the reference tone until it sounds equally loud as judged by a typical listener. The only way of determining a typical listener is to use a number of observers who have normal hearing to make the judgment tests. The typical listener, as used in this sense,would then give the sameresultsas the averageobtained by a large number of suchobservers. A pure tone having a frequencyof 1000 cycles per secondwas chosenfor the referencetone for the followingreasons:(1) it is simpleto define, (2) it is sometimes used as a standard of reference for pitch, (3) its use makes the mathematical formulae more simple, (4) its range of auditory intensities (from the threshold of hearing to the threshold of feeling) is as large and usually larger than for any other type of sound, and (5) its frequency is in the mid-range of audible frequencies. There has been considerable discussion con- cerning the choice of the reference or zero for loudnesslevels. In many ways the threshold of hearing intensity for a 1000-cycle tone seemsa logicalchoice.However, variationsin this threshold intensity arisedependingupon the individual, his age, the manner of listening, the method of presentingthe tone to the listener, etc. For this reason no attempt was made to choose the reference intensity as equal to the average threshold of a given group listening in a prescribedway. Rather, an intensity of the reference tone in air of 10-• watts per square centimeter was chosen as the reference intensity because it AND W. A. MUNSON tone is the same as its loudness level L and is given by 10 log Jrq- 100, (3) where -/r is its sound intensity in microwatts per squarecentimeter. The intensity level of any other soundis given by /• = 10 log J+ 100, (4) where J is its sound intensity, but the loudness level of such a soundis a complicatedfunction of the intensitiesand frequenciesof its components. However, it will be seen from the experimental data given later that for a considerablerange of frequenciesand intensitiesthe intensity level and loudness level for pure tones are approximately equal. With the reference levels adopted here, all values of loudness level which are positive indicate a sound which can be heard by the referenceobserver and those which are negative indicate a sound which cannot be heard by such an observer. It is frequently more convenient to use two matched head receivers for introducing the reference tone into the two ears. This can be done provided they are calibrated against the condition described above. This consists in finding by a series of listening tests by a number of observersthe electrical power W• in the receivers which producesthe same loudnessas a level 31 of the reference tone. The intensity level •,. of an open air reference tone equivalent to that producedin the receiver for any other power W, in the receivers is then given by was a simple number which was convenient as a referencefor computation work, and at the same •=/•q-10 log (W•/W•). (5) time it is in the range of threshold measurements Or, since the intensity level •,. of the reference obtained when listening in the standard method describedabove. This referenceintensity corre- tone is its loudness level L, we have spondsto the threshold intensity of an observer L= 10 log W•+C,., (6) who might be designated a reference observer. An examination of a large series of measure- where C• is a constant of the receivers. ments on the threshold of hearing indicates that In determining loudnesslevels by comparison such a reference observer has a hearing which is with a reference tone there are two general classes slightly more acute than the average of a large of sound for which measurements are desired: group. For those who have been thinking in terms of microwatts it is easy to remember that (1) those which are steady, such as a musical tone, or the hum from machinery, (2) those which are varying in loudnesssuch as the noise watt per square centimeter. When using these from the street, conversational speech, music, definitions the intensity level 3,. of the reference etc. In this paper we have confinedour discussion this reference level is 100 db below one micro- LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT to sourceswhich are steady and the method of specifyingsuch sourceswill now be given. A steady soundcan be representedby a finite number of pure tones called components. Since changes in phase produce only second order effectsupon the loudnesslevel it is only necessary to specify the magnitude and frequency of the components? The magnitudes of the components at the listening position where the loudness level is desired are given by the intensity levels •, 32, '" 3•, '" •3• of each component at that position. In case the sound is conducted to the ears by telephone receivers or AND CALCULATION 85 componenttoward the total loudnesssensation depends not only upon the properties of this component but also upon the properties of the other components in the combination. The answer to the problem of finding a method of calculating the loudnesslevel lies in determining the nature of the ear and brain as measuring instruments in evaluating the magnitude of an auditory sensation. One can readily estimate roughly the magnitude of an auditory sensation;for example, one can tell whether the sound is soft or loud. There have been many theories to account for this change in loudness.One that seemsvery reaqonbe known such that if this component were able to us is that the loudnessexperienced is acting separately it would produce the same dependent upon the total number of nerve loudnessfor typical observersas a tone of the impulsesper secondgoing to the brain along all same pitch coming from a sourceat one meter's the fibres that are excited. Although such an distanceand producingan intensity level of/•s-. assumptionis not necessaryfor deriving the In addition to the frequency and magnitude of formulafor calculatingloudnessit aidsin making the components of a sound it is necessary to the meaning of the quantities involved more know the position and orientation of the head definite. with respectto the source,and alsowhether one Let usconsitler,then, a complextone having n or two ears are used in listening. The monaural componentseach of which is specifiedby a value type of listening is important in telephone use of intensity level • and of frequencyft,. Let N be and the binaural type when listeningdirectly to a a number which measuresthe magnitude of the tubes, then a value [1• for each component must sound source in air. Unless otherwise stated, the auditory sensation produced when a typical individual listens to a pure tone. Since by condition where the listener faces the source and definitionthemagnitudeof an auditorysensation is uses both ears, or uses head telephone receivers theloudness, thenN is theloudness of this simple tone. Loudness as used here must not be confused which produce an equivalent result. with loudness level.The latter is measuredby the FORMULATION OF THE EMPIRICAL TIlEOR'/ FOR intensity of the equally loud referencetone and is CALCULATING THE LOUDNESS LEVEL OF expressedin decibels while the former will be discussionand data which follow apply to the A STEADY COMPLEX TONE expressedin units related to loudness levels in a manner to be: developed. If we accept the complextone is the sum of the intensitiesof the assumptionmentioned above, N is proportional individual components. Similarly, in finding a to the number of nerve impulses per second method of calculating the loudness level of a reaching the brain along all the excited nerve complex tone one would naturally try to find fibers when the typical observer listens to a simple tone. numbers which could be related to each comLet the dependencyof the loudnessN uponthe ponent in such a way that the sum of such frequency f and the intensity • for a simpletone numbers will be related in the same way to the be represented by equally loud reference tone. Such efforts have failed because the amount contributed by any N = S(f, 3), (7) It is well known that the intensity of a * Recent work by ChaDin and Firestone indicates that at very high levels these second order effects become large and c•annotbe neglected. a K. E. Chltpin and F. A. Firestone, Interferenceof Sub- jettire liarmonies,J. Aeons.Soc.Am. 4, 1•6A (1933). where G is a function a hich is determined by any pair of valuesoff and•1.For the referencetone,f is 1000 and/S i• equal to the loudnesslevel L, so a determinationof the relationexpressed in Eq. 86 HARVEY FLETCHER AND W. A. MUNSON (7) for the reference tone gives the desired This transformationlookssimplebut it is a very important one since instead of having to deIf now a simple tone is put into combination termine a different function for every comwith other simple tones to form a complex tone, ponent, we now have to determine a single relation between loudness and loudness level. its loudness contribution, that is, its contribution functiondependingonly upon the propertiesof toward the total sensation, ;;'ill in general be the reference somewhat less because of the interference tone and as stated above this of the function is the relationshipbetween loudnessand other components. For example, if the other loudnesslevel. And sincethe frequencyis always components are much louder and in the same 1000 this function is dependentonly upon the frequencyregion the loudnessof the simpletone singlevariable, the intensity level. in such a combination will be zero. Let 1-b be This formula has no practical value unlesswe the fractional reduction in loudness because of can determine b• and G in terms of quantities which can be obtained by physical measurecontribution of this component toward the loud- ments. It ;;,ill be shown that experimental measnessof the complextone. It will be seenthat b by urementsof the loudnesslevels L and Le upon definition always remains between 0 and unity. simple and complextones of a properly chosen It depends not only upon the frequency and structure have yielded resultswhich have enabled intensity of the simple tone under discussionbut us to find the dependenceof b and G upon the also upon the frequenciesand intensities of the frequenciesand intensities of the components. other components.It will be shown later that this When b and G are known, then the more general dependencecan be determinedfrom experimental function G(f, •) can be obtained from Eq. (9), measurements. and the experimentalvaluesof L•. corresponding The subscript k will be used when f and • tore and t36. correspondto the frequencyand intensity level of the kth componentof the complextone, and the DETERMINATION OF THE RELATION BETWEEN Lk, ft AND /• subscriptr usedwhenf is 1000cyclesper second. The "loudness level" L by definition, is the Thisreiation canbeobtained fromexperiintensity level of the reference tone when it is mental measurements of the loudness levels of adjusted so it and the complex tone sound pare tones. Such measurements;;'ere made by equally loud. Then Kingsbury• which covereda rangein frequency and intensity limited by instrumentalities then Nr= G(1000,L) -- E =E (8) available. Using the experimentaltechnique k 1 k--1 describedin AppendixA, we have againobtained the loudness levels of pure tones, this time Now let the referencetone be adjusted so that it coveringpractically the ;;-holeaudible range.* soundsequally loud successivelyto simple tones All of the data on loudness levelsbothfor pure correspondingin frequency and intensity to each and alsocomplextonestaken in our laboratory componentof the complextone. which are discussedin this paper have been Designate the experimental values thus detaken with telephone receivers on the ears. It termined as L•, L•, Ls, ßßßL•., ß- ßL•. Then from has been explained previously how telephone the definition of these values receivers may be used to introduce the reference its being in such a combination. Then bN is the N• = G(1000, L•) = G(f•,/•), (9) tone into the ears at known loudness levels to obtain the loudnesslevelsof other soundsby a sincefor a singletone beis unity. On substituting loudness balance. If the receivers are also used the values from (9) into (8) there results the for producing the sounds whose loudness levels fundamental equation for calculating the loud- are being determined, then an additional calinessof a complex tone • B. A. Kingsbury,A Direct Comparison of lite LmMness of Pure Tones,Phys. Rev. 29, 588 (1927). G(1000,L) = Y•.b•G(1000,L•). (10) * See AppendixB for a comparisonwith t(ingsbury's results. LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT •ND CALCUL•,TION 87 bration, which will be explained later, is neces- trolled, it is obviously impractical to measure sary if it is desiredto know the intensity levelsof directly the threshold level by using a large the sounds. group of obser•-ershaving normal hearing. For The experimental data for determining the most purposesit is •nore convenient to measure relation between Lk and fk are given in Table I in the intensity levels fi•, t•, .-- •, etc., directly terms of voltage levels. (Voltage level = 20 log l', rather than have them related in any way to the where [' is the ean.f. across the receivers in threshold of hearing. In order to reduce the data in Table I to those volts.) The pairs of values in each double column give the voltage levels of the referencetone and which one would obtain if the observers were the pure tone having the frequency indicated at listeningto a freewave and facing the source,we the top of the column when the two tonescoming must obtain a field calibration of the telephone from the head receivers were judged to be receivers used in the loudnesscomparisons.The equally loud when usingthe techniquedescribed calibration for the referencetone frequency has in Appendix A. For example, in the second been explained previously and the equation column it will be seenthat for the 125-cycletone /• =/•+ 10 log (W,/gh) (5) when the voltage is +9.8 db above 1 volt then the voltage level for the reference tone must be derived for the relation between the intensity 4.4 db below 1 volt for equality of loudness.The of the referencetone and the electrical power bottom set of numbers in each column gives the threshold values for this group of observers. Each voltage level in Table I is the median of 297 observations representing the combined results of eleven observers. The method of in the receivers. The calibration consisted of findingby meansof loudnessbalancesa power in the receiverswhich producesa tone equal in loudness to that of a free wave having an intensity level/•1. obtaining these is explainedin Appendix A also. For soundsother than the 1000-cyclereference The standard deviation was computed and it was tone a relation similar to Eq. (5) can be derived, found to he somewhat larger for tests in which namely, the tone differed most in frequency from the •=01'-}-•0 log (W/W1), reference tone. The probable error of the combined result as computed in the usual way was where • and [[h are correspondingvalues found between 1 and 2 db. Since deviations of any one from loudness balances for each frequency or observer's results from his own average are less complex wave form of interest. If, as is usually than the deviations of his average from the assumed, a linear relation exists between • and 10 average of the group, it would be necessaryto log W, then determinations of/• and I,V1 at one increase the size of the group if values more level are sufficient and it follows that a change in representative of the average normal ear were the power level of _X decibels will produce a desired. The data shown in Table I can be reduced to the number of decibels above threshold if we corresponding change of ,• decibels in the intensity of the sound generated. Obviously the receivers must not be overloaded or this as- accept the values of this crew as the reference sumption will not be valid. Rather than depend threshold values. However, we have already upon the existenceof a linear relation between adopted a value for the 1000-cyclereferencezero. and 10 log 1V with no confirming data, the As will be shown, our crew obtained a threshold for the reference tone which is 3 db above the reference level chosen. It is not only more convenient but also more reliable to relate the data to a calibration of the receivers in terms of physical measurements of receivers used in this investigation were calibrated at two widely separatedlevels. Referring again to Table I, the data are expressed in terms of voltage levels instead of power levels. If, as was the case with our receivers, the electrical impedance is essentially a the sound intensity rather than to the threshold constant, Eq. (11) can be put in the form: values. Except in experimental work where the intensity of the sound can be definitely con02) log (17 lr,) T^BLE I. Voltagelevels(db) for loudnessequality. Refer- 62 Refer- 250 Refer- 500 Refer- 2000 Refer- 4000 Refer- 5650 Refer- 8000 Refer- 11,300 Refer- 16,00• ence c.p.s. ence c.p.s. ence c.p.s. ence c.p.s. ence c.p.s. ence c.p.s. ence c.p.s. ence c.p.s. ence c.p.s. ence c.p.s. --12.2 --17.2 --19.2 --15.7 --21.2 + + + + + -- 4.4 --10.2 --13.3 --18.6 --23.2 + + ---- -- 2.9 -- 3.7 -- 5.2 -- 6.7 --12.2 + + + --- -- 2.2 -- 4.2 -- 6.2 -- 7.2 --12.2 + q---- -- 2.2 -- 1.7 -- 3.2 --18.2 --21.2 -- 1.2 -- 1.5 -- 1.3 --13.4 --17.2 -- 1.7 -- 1.2 --23.2 --24.7 --44.7 -- 0.7 -- 1.5 --17.3 --16.7 --35.2 -- 3.7 -- 7.2 --28.2 --30.2 --53.2 + 0.8 -- 1.2 --19.2 --19.2 --38.2 --10.9 --12.2 --26.2 --27.1 --46.2 -- 4.3 --12.0 --24.0 --24.3 --38.3 --20.2 --22.2 --38.2 --38.7 --55.2 + 1.7 + 1.8 --20.2 --20.3 --34.2 --50.2 --66.2 --77.2 --85.2 q- 1.: --13.: --28.: --38.: --56.2 --67.2 --68.7 --97.2 --15.2 --20.2 --20.3 --30.3 --88.8 --46.5 --90.2 --68.3 --43.7 --63.7 --64.2 --83.2 --42.2 --61.0 --61.2 --80.2 --61.2 --64.2 --78.2 --78.2 --57.5' --57.3 --77.3 --80.2 --83.7 --78.0 --81.7 --77.3 -- 108.1 -- 102.6 --108.3 --101.7 --27.2 --32.2 --33.2 --41.2 --35.4 9.8 5.8 2.8 2.6 0.8 -- 0.2 -- 7.2 -- 7.2 --10.2 --10.4 --27.9 --31.0 --35.2 --40.7 --66.6 --108.1 --39.8 --109.3 --113.1 --42.4 --108.3 --38.5 --113.1 125 Refer- 9.8 7.9 0.8 3.2 5.2 --12.3 --14.2 --15.2 --23.6 --35.0 --25.5 --32.2 --32.2 --52.5 --72.9 6.6 5.7 5.8 2.2 2.2 --18.3 --22.2 --23.2 --40.4 --56.3 --21.2 --21.7 --32.2 --34.2 --41.7 --22.3 --21.9 --30.2 --31.2 --41.9 --108.3 --39.5 --108.1 --62.8 --108.1 --86.7 --108.3--60.7 --108.3 --63.5 --113.1 5.8 6.8 2.0 2.3 8.2 --22.2 --40.2 --41.2 --42.2 --59.2 --108.3 --18.3 --35.2 --35.3 --35.4• --54.2 --47.7 --63.2 --65.2 --77.7 --80.2 --105.2 --108.3 --35.1 --54.2 --54.5 --72.2 --72.5 --54.7 --72.2 --72.7 --85.2 --92.7 --39.1 --58.2 --58.1 --71.2 --78.1 --48.2 --64.2 --70.2 --76.2 --82.6 --38.2 --52.2 --56.2 --72.2 --76.2 --55.7 --72.2 --78.7 --88.2 --90.7 --34.2 --52.2 --58.1 --72.2 --77.1 --104.6 99.7 --108.3--109.0 --108.3--105.7 --108.3--101.9 --108.3--108.1!--108.3 --93.7 --86.4 --109.3 -- 103.4 -- 109.3 --108.9 -- 109.3 --102.0 --109.3 -- 99.3 --109.3 --103.1 --109.3 --86.3 --113.1 --103.(1 --113.1 --111.4 --113.1 --108.1 --113.1 --102.3 --113.1 --106.• --113.1 --94.6 --93.7 --109.3 --57.: TABLE II. Field calibrationof telephonereceivers. Frequency c.p.s. Voltage level (20 log V0 Intensity level (•t) C• =•20 log V• Thresholdvoltage level (20 log Vo) Thresholdintensity level (•5o) Co=t•o- 20 log Vo Diff. = C•- Co 60 -- 13.0 +79.3 92.3 -48.0 +49.3 97.3 -5.0 120 --26.2 +71.0 97.2 --61.8 +33.7 95.5 1.7 240 480 960 1920 3850 7800 10,500 --38.5 --47.0 --48.2 --42.3 --36.3 --34.0 5400 --39.1 --32.4 -- +67.4 +63.8 +65.3 +64.0 +62.2 +65.5 +74.0 +78.6 +75.0 105.9 110.8 113.5 106.3 98.5 99.5 113.1 -- 86.2 -- 105.4 -- 110.7 -- 109.0 -- 104.0 --97.1 -- 100.5 +19.7 +8.4 +5.4 --0.9 --4.2 +2.7 +10.6 105.9 0 113.8 --3.0 116.1 --2.6 108.1 --1.8 99.8 --1.3 99.8 --0.3 Ilia +2.0 111.0 --102.0 +16.1 118.1 --7.1 15,000 6.4 81.4 -- 74.0 +22.0 96.0 --14.6 > LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT AND CALCULATION 89 fi = 20 log 1'+ C or tl=20 log V+C, (13) can be applied to our receiverswith considerable where V is the voltage acrossthe receiversand C confidence. is a constant The constant C determined at the high level was determined with greater accuracy than at the threshold. For this reasononly the values for the higher level were used for the calibration curve. Also in these tests only four receiverswere used while in the loudnesstests eight receivers of the receivers to be determined from a calibration giving correspondingvalues of th and 20 log V•. The calibration will now be described. By usingthe soundstageand the techniqueof measuring field pressures described by Sivian and White • and by using the technique for making loudness measurements described in Appendix A, the following measurements were made. An electrical voltage V• was placed across were used. The difference between the efficiency of the former four and the latter eight receivers was determined by measurementson an artificial ear. The figuresgiven in Table II were corrected by this difference.The resultingcalibrationcurve level producedwas the same at each frequency. is that given in Fig. 1. It should be pointed out the two head receivers The observer listened such that the loudness to the tone in these head receivers and then after 1« seconds silence listened to the tone from the loud speaker producing a free wave of the same frequency. The voltage level acrossthe loud speaker necessary to produce a tone equally loud to the tone from the head receivers was obtained using the procedure described in Appendix A. The free wave intensity level fi• correspondingto this voltage level was measured in the manner described in Sivian and White's paper. Threshold values both for the head receivers and the loud speaker were also observed. In these tests eleven observers were used. The results obtained are given in Table II. In the second rmv values of 20 FIG. 1. Field calibration of loudness balance receivers.* (Calibration made at L=60 db.) here that such a calibration curve on a single individual would show considerable deviations from this aw.•rage curve. These deviations are real, that is, they are due to the sizesand shapes log V•, the voltage level, are given. The intensity of the ear canals. levels, fi•, of the free wave which sounded We can now express the data in Table I in equally loud are given in the third row. In the terms of field intensity levels. To do this, the fourth row the values of the constant C, the data in each double column were plotted and a calibration we are seeking,are given. The voltage smooth curve drawn through the observed level added to this constantgivesthe equivalent points. The resulting curves give the relation free wave intensity level. In the fifth, sixth and between voltage levels of the pure tones for seventh rows, similar values are given which equality of loudness.From the calibration curve were bottom determined row the at the threshold differences in level. the In the constants of the receivers these levels are converted to intensity levels by a simple shift in the axes of determined at the two levels are given. The fact coordinates. Since the intensity level of the that the difference is no larger than the probable reference tone is by definition the "loudness error is very significant. It means that through- level," these shifted curves will represent the out this wide range there is a linear relationship between the equivalent field intensity levels, •, * The ordinatesrepresentthe intensity level in db of a and the voltage levels, 20 log V, so that the free wave in air which, when listened to with both ears in formula (13) the standard manner, is as loud as a tone of the same • L. J. Sivian and S. D. White, Minimum Audible Sound Fields, J. Acous. Soc. Am. 4, 288 (1933). frequency heard from the two head receivers used in the tests when an e.m.f. of one volt is applied to the receiver ternfinals. 90 HARVEY FLETCHER AND W. A. MUNSON loudnesslevel of pure tonesin terms of intensity Table I except for the threshold values. The levels. The resulting curves for the ten tones results of separate determinations by the crew tested are given in Figs. 2A to 2J. Each point on used in these loudness tests at different times are these curves correspondsto a pair of values in given by the circles.The points representedby (*) are the valuesadoptedby Sivian and White. It will be seen that most of the threshold points are slightlyabovethe zerowe have chosen.This meansthat our zerocorresponds to the thresholds of observerswho are slightly more acute than the average. From these curves the loudness level contours can be drawn. The first set of loudness level contoursare plotted with levels above reference threshold as ordinates. For example, the zero loudness level contour corresponds to points where the curves of Figs. 2A to 2J intersect the abscissa axis. The number of db above these points is plotted as the ordinate in the loudness level contours shown in Fig. 3. From a con- 40 o 100 120 0 20 40 60 iNTENSiTY LEVEL-DB i 4C 80 •00 120 O , 4ooo• TONE FIG. 3. Loudness level contours. sideration of the nature of the hearing mechanism we believe that o 2o 40 6o 6o O Ioo •2o o 2o 40 INTœN•TYLEVEL-DB 60 •o 1oo 12o 60 80 too 120 H 11300 TJNE 0 20 40 60 ! 80 Ioo 120 0 INTEN$4TY LEVEL-DB 20 40 j FIG. 2. (A to J) loudnesslevels of pure tones. these curves should be smooth. These curves, therefore, represent the best set of smooth curves which we could draw through the observed points. After the smoothing process, the curves in Figs. 2A to 2J xverethen adjusted to correspond.The curvesshownin thesefiguresare such adjusted curves. In Fig. 4 a similar set of loudnesslevel contours is shown using intensity levels as ordinates. There are good reasonss for believing that the peculiar shape of these contours for frequencies above 1000 c.p.s. is due to diffraction around the head of the observer as he faces the source of sound. It was for this reason that the smoothing LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT AND CALCULATION 91 processwas done with the curves plotted with the level above threshold as the ordinate. From these:loudnesslevel contours, the curves shownin Figs. 5A and 5B were obtained.They show the loudnesslevel rs. intensity level with frequency as a parameter. They are convenient to use for calculation purposes. Fro. 4. Loudness It is interesting to note that through a large part of the practicalrangefor tonesof frequencies froIn 300 c.p.s. to 4000 c.p.s. the loudnesslevel is approximately equal to the intensity level. From these curves, it is possibleto obtain any value of L• in terms of •a and f•.:. On Fig. 4 the 120-db loudnesslevel contour has been marked "Feeling." The data publishedby R. R. Riesz• on the threshold of feeling indicate that this contour is very closeto the feeling point throughout the frequency range where data have level contours. 120 been taken. DETERMINATION OF TIlE LOUDNESS FUNCTION G In the section "Formulation of the Empirical Theory for Calculatingthe Loudnessof a Steady Complex Tone," the fundamental equation for calculating the loudnesslevel of a complex tone was derived, namely, G(1000, L) = Z b,:G(1000,Zt:). (10) k=l tf the type of complextone can be chosenso that b•.is unity and also so that the values of L• for each compommt are equal, then the fundamental equation for calculating loudnessbecomes ioo G(L) = nG(L•), where n is the number of components. Since we are always dealing in this sectionwith G(1000, L) or G(1000, L;:), the 1000 is left out in the above nomenclature.. If experimental measurementsot' L correspondingto values of Lt. are taken for a tone fulfilling the above conditions throughout the audible :range, the function G can be determined. If we accept the thory that, when two simple tones widely separated in frequency, act upon the ear: the nerve terminals stimulated by • •o o -I-020-•O (14) 0 10 •O 30 40 50 INTENSITY •0 70 •0 90 100 110 LEVœL-D8 FIG. 5. (A and B) loudnesslevels of pure tones. 120 • R. R. Riesz, The Relationship BetweenLoudnessand the Minimum PerceptibleIncrementof Intensity, J. Acous.Soc. Am. 4, 211 (1933). 92 HARVEY FLETCHER AND W. A. MUNSON In this curve the ordinates give the loudness levels when one ear is used while the abscissae give the correspondingloudnesslevels for the same intensity level of the tone when both ears are usedfor listening. If binaural versesmonaural loudness data actually fit into this scheme of calculationthesepointsshouldbe representedby G(y)= «S(x). o-looo 2000 AND Any one of these curves which was accurately determined would be sufficient to completely determine o 20 40 LOUDNESS 60 LEVEL 80 OF EACH lOO 12o the function G. For example,considerthe curve for two tones. It is evident that it is only necessaryto deal with 14o relative COMPONENT-OB Fro. 6. Complextonesbavlng componentswidely separated in frequency. values of G so that we can choose one value arbitrarily. The value of G(0) was chosen equal to unity. Therefore, G(0) = 1, each are at different portions of the basilar membrane,then we would expectthe interference of the loudnessof one upon that of the other would be negligible. Consequently, for such a combinationb is unity. Measurementswere made upon two such tones, the two componentsbeing equally loud, the first having frequenciesof 1000 and 2000 cycles and the second,frequenciesof 125 and 1000 cycles. The observed points are shown along the secondcurve from the top of G(yo)= 2G(O)= 2 where y0 correspondsto x = 0, G(y• = 2G(x•) = 2a(yo) = 4 where yl correspondsto x• =y0, G(y•.)= 2G(x2)= 2G(y•) = 8 where y2 correspondsto x•=y•, G(yk) = 2G(x•) = 2G(yk_•)= 2 wherey• correspondsto x• = y•_•. Fig. 6. The abscissae give the loudnesslevel Lk of In this way a set of valuesfor G can be obtained. each componentand the ordinates the loudness A smooth curve connecting all such calculated level L of the two components combined. The equation G(y)=2G(x) should represent these data. Similar measurements were made with a complextone having 10 components,all equally loud. The method of generating such tones is describedin Appendix C. The results are shown by the points along the top curve of Fig. 6. The equation G(y)=lOG(x) should representthese data except at high levels where bk is not unity. moo There is probably a complete separation be- tween stimulated patchesof nerve endingswhen the first component is introduced into one ear and the second component into the other ear. In this casethe same or different frequenciescan be used. Since it is easier to make • •o õ 20 loudness balances when the same kind of sound is used, measurements were made (1) with 125-cycle tones (2) with 1000-cycle tones and (3) with 4000-cycletones.The resultsare shownon Fig. 7. / 0 20 40 60 LOUDNESS LEVEL, •0 100 120 140 BOTH EARS-DB FIG. 7. Relation between loudness levels listening with one ear and with both ears. LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMEN'[' TABLE III. AND CALCULATION 93 Valuesof G(L•). L 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 -- 10 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 0.015 1.00 13.9 97.5 360 975 2200 4350 7950 17100 38000 88000 215000 556000 0.025 1.40 17.2 113 405 1060 2350 4640 8510 18400 41500 97000 235000 609000 0.04 1.90 21.4 131 455 1155 2510 4950 9130 19800 45000 106000 260000 668000 0.06 2.51 26.6 151 505 1250 2680 5250 9850 21400 49000 116000 288000 732000 0.09 3.40 32.6 173 555 1360 2880 5560 10600 23100 53000 126000 316000 800000 0.14 4.43 39.3 197 615 1500 3080 5870 11400 25000 57000 138000 346000 875000 (I.22 5.70 4;'.5 222 675 1640 3310 6240 12400 27200 62000 150000 380000 956000 0.32 7.08 57.5 252 740 1780 3560 6620 13500 29600 67500 164000 418000 1047000 0.45 9.00 69.5 287 810 1920 3820 7020 14600 32200 74000 180000 460000 1150000 0.70 11.2 82.5 324 890 2070 4070 7440 15800 35000 81000 197000 506000 1266000 points will enable one to find any value of G(x) to give a to•e which can be heard. When the for a given value of x. In a similar way setsof componentsare all in the high pitch range and all equally loud, each componentmay be from values can be obtained from the other two experimental curves. Instead of using any one of 6 to 8 db below the threshold the curves alone the values of G were chosen to tion will still be audible. When they are all in the low pitch range they may be only 2 or 3 db below the threshold.The closeness of packingof the components also influences the threshold. For example, if the ten components are all within a 100-cycle band each one may be down 10 db. It will be shownthat the formula proposed best fit all three sets of data, taking into account the fact that the observedpoints for the 10-tone data might be low at the higher levels where b would be less than unity. The values for the function which were finally adopted are given in Table III. From these values the three solid curves of Figs. 6 and 7 were calculated by the equations and the cmnbina- above can be made to take care of these vari- ations in the 'threshold. There is still another method which might be S(y) = -}G(x). usedfor determining this loudnessfunction G(L), provided one's judgment as to the magnitude of The fit of the three sets of data is sufficiently an auditory sensation can be relied upon. If a good, we think, to justify the point of view taken person were asked to judge when the loudnessof in developingthe formula. The calculated points a sound was reduced to one-half it might be for the 10-component tones agree with the expected that he would base his judgment on observed ones when the proper value of b•, is the experience of the decreasein loudnesswhen introduced into the formula. In this connection going from the condition of listening with both it is important to emphasizethat in calculating ears to that of listening with one ear. Or, if the the loudnesslevel of a complex tone under the magnitude of the sensation is the number of condition of listening with one ear instead of nerve dischargesreaching the brain per second, two, a factor of 21must be placed in front of then when this has decreased to one-half, he the summationof Eq. (10). This will be explained might be able to say that the loudnesshas dein greater detail later. The values of G for nega- creased one-half. tive values of L were chosenafter consideringall In any case, if it is assumed that an observer the data on the thresholdvaluesof the complex can judge when the magnitude of the auditory tones studied. These data will be given with the sensation, that is, the loudness, is reduced to S(y) = 10G(x), G(y): 2G(x), other loudness data on complex tones. It is interesting to note here that the threshold data show that 10 pure tones, which are below the one-half, then the value of the loudness function G can be computed from such measurements. Several different research workers have made threshold when soundedseparately, will combine such measurements. The measurements are some- 94 HARVEY FLETCHER AND W. A. MUNSON what in conflict at the present time so that they did not in any way influence the choice of the calculated and observed results of data taken by loudness function. taken from Tables la, lb, 2a, 2b, 3a and 3b of their paper. The calculation is very simple. Rather we used the loudness function given in Table III to calculate what such observations should give. A comparison of the calculated and observed results is given below. In Table IV is shown a comparison of Ham and Parkinson. 7 The observed values were From the the number loudness 350 cycles Fractional reduction in loudness S L G Cal. % Obs. % 74.0 70.4 67.7 64.0 59.0 54.0 44.0 34.0 85 82 79 75 70 65 53 41 25,000 19,800 15,800 11,400 7,950 5,870 2,680 1,100 100 79 63 46 32 24 11 4 100.0 83.0 67.0 49.0 35.0 26.0 15.0 8.0 59.5 57.7 55.0 49.0 44.0 39.0 34.0 71 69 66 59 47 41 8,510 7,440 6,240 4,070 2,680 1,780 1,060 100 87 73 48 31 21 12 I00.0 92.0 77.0 57.0 38.0 25.0 13.0 24.0 29 53 86.0 82.4 79.7 76.0 71.0 66.0 56.0 46.0 86 82 80 76 71 66 56 46 56.0 54.2 56 54 324 1000 cycles 27,200 19,800 17,100 12,400 8,510 6,420 3,310 1,640 4 6.0 i00.0 68.0 53.0 41.0 26.0 20.0 13.0 8.0 100 87 76 100.0 93.4 74.6 51.5 52 3,310 2,880 2,510 48.8 49 2,070 62 55.0 46.0 41.0 46 41 1,640 1,060 49 32 40.9 24.5 36.0 36 20 10.8 74.0 70.4 69 64 2500 cycles 7,440 5,560 67.7 64.0 59.0 54.0 62 58 53 48 4,950 3,820 2,680 1,920 67 51 36 26 68.1 44.0 34.0 39 30 890 360 12 5 13.0 6.7 44.0 42.2 39.5 36.8 34.0 29.0 24.0 39 37 36 33 30 26 21 890 740 675 505 360 222 113 100 83 76 57 41 25 13 675 i00 75 above threshold is determined from S the reduction in the loudness function for the correspondingvalues of L. The agreement between observed and calculated results is re- markably good. However, the agreement with the data of Laird, Taylor and Wille is very poor, as is shown by Table V. The calculation was made only for the 1024-cycle tone. The observed data were taken from Table VII of the paper by Laird, Taylor and Wille. a As shown in Table V the calculation of the level for one-fourth re- duction in loudness agrees better with the observed data correspondingto one-half reduction in loudness. T^BLE V. Comparisonof calculatedand observed fractional loudness(Laird, Taylor and Wille). Cal. level Original loudness level 100 73 63 46 31 24 12 6 L curvesof Fig. 3. The fractional reduction is just the fractional T^nLE IV. Comparisonof calculatedand observed fractional loudness(Ham and Parkinson). of decibels level 100 90 80 7O 60 50 40 30 20 10 Level for « loudness reduction Cal. Obs. 92 82 71 58 50 42 33 25 16 7 76.O 68.0 60.0 49.5 40.5 31.0 21.0 14.9 6.5 5.0 for ¬ loudness reduction 84 73 60 48 41 34 27 20 13 4 Firestone and Geiger reported some prelimi~ nary values which were in closer agreement with those obtained by Parkinson and Ham, but their completed paper has not yet been published.ø 100.0 Because of the lackof agreement of observed 86.4 data of this sort we concluded that it could not 49.5 beusedforinfluencing thechoice of thevalues 32.8 23.3 100.0 94.6 82.2 61.1 46.0 27.8 14.9 of the loudness function adopted and shown in 7 L. B. Ham and J. S. Parkinson,Loudnessand Intensity Relations, J. Acous. Soc. Am. 3, 511 (1932). • Laird, Taylor and Wille, The Apparent Reduction in Loudness,J. Acous. Soc. Am. 3, 393 (1932). 9 This paper is now available. P. H. Geiger and F. A. Firestone, The Estimation of Fractional Loudness,J. Acous. Soc. Am. 5, 25 (1933). LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMEN'[' Table III. It is to be hoped that more data of this type will be taken until there is a better agreement between observed results of different observers. It should be emphasized here that changesof the level above threshold corresponding to any fixed increase or decreasein loudness will, according to the theory outlined in this paper, depend upon the frequency of the tone when using pure tones, or upon its structure when using complex tones. DETERMINATION OF THE FORMULA FOR AND CAI. CUI•ATION 95 which is unity. It is thus seen that the fundamental of a series of tones will always have a value of b• equal to unity. For the casewhen the maskingcomponentand the kth component have the same loudness,the function representing b• will be considerably simplified, particularly if it were also found to be independentof f• and only dependent upon the differenos between fk and j;,•. From the theory of hearing one xvould expect that this would be approximately true for the following reasons: CALCULATING bk The distance in millimeters between the po- Having now determined the function G for all sitions of maximum response on the basilar values of L or Lk we can proceedto find methods membrane for the two components is more of calculatingb•. Its value is evidently dependent nearly proportional to differences in pitch than upon the frequencyand intensity of all the other components present as well as upon the component being considered.For practical computations, simplifying assumptions can be .made. In most cases the reduction of b• from unity is principally due to the adjacent component on the side of the lower pitch. This is due to the fact that a tone masks another tone of higher pitch very .much more than one of lower pitch. For example, in most casesa tone which is 100 cycles higher than the masking tone would be masked when it is reduced to differencesin frequency. However, the peaks are sharpest in the high frequency regionswhere the distances on the basilar membrane for a given/xf are smallest.Also, in the low frequency region where the distances for a given /xf are largest, these. peaks are broadest. These two factors tend to make the interference between two components having a fixed difference in frequency approximately the same regardlessof their position on the frequency scale. However, it would be extraordinary if these two factors 25 db below the level of the masking tone, whereas a tone 100 cycles lower in frequency will be masked only when it is reduced from 40 to 60 db below the level of the masking tone. It will therefore be assumed that the neighboring component on the side of lower pitch which causes the greatest masking will account for all the reduction in bk. Designating thi's component with the subscript m, meaning the masking component,then we have bx. expressed as a function of the following variables. bk= B(f•., f,,,, S•., S,•), (15) wheref is the frequencyand S is the level above threshold. For the case when the level of the kth componentis T db below the level of the masking component, where T is just sufficient for the component to be masked, then the value of b would be equal to zero. Also• it is reasonable to assumethat when the masking component is at a level somewhat less than T db below the kth component, the latter will have a value of b• FIG. 8. Loudnesslevelsof complextones having ten equally loud components 50 cycles apart. 96 HARVEY FLETCHER AND W. A. MUNSON just balanced. To test this point three complex tones having ten components with a common zXf of 50 cycles were tested for loudness. The first had frequencies of 50-100-150...500, the second 1400-1450... 3450...3900. The 1900, and the third 3400results of these tests are shown in Fig. 8. The abscissaegive the loudness level of each component and the ordinates the u 60 o• measured loudness level of the combined tone. ,.-d, so Similar results were obtained with a complex u,40 tone having ten componentsof equal loudness and a commonfrequencydifferenceof 100 cycles. J o FREQUENCYDIFFERENCE- 340 The results are shown in Fig. 9. It will be seen that although the points correspondingto the different frequency ranges lie approximately / upon the same curve through the middle range, 0_, ø o"P x there are consistentdeparturesat both the high and low intensities.If we choosethe frequencyof FIG. 10. Loudnesslevels of complextones having ten the components largely in the middle range then equally loud componentswith a fundamentalfrequency this factor b will be dependent only upon zXf of 1000 c.p.s. > and Lk. To determine the value of b for this range in 56 cycles per second.The fundamental for each terms of 6f and Lk, a seriesof loudnessmeasure- tonewascloseto 1000cycles.The ten-component ments was made upon complex tones having ten toneshaving frequencieswhich are multiplesof components with a common difference in fre- 530 was included in this series. The results of quency zXf and all having a common loudness loudnessbalancesare shownby the points in level L•. The valuesof zXfwere 340, 230, 112 and Fig. 10. By taking all the data as a whole, the curves were consideredto give the best fit. The values of b were calculated from these curves as follows: Accordingto the assumptions made above,the componentof lowest pitch in the seriesof componentsalwayshas a value of b•.equal to unity. Thereforefor the seriesof 10components having a common loudness level L•, the value of L is related to Lk by G(L) = (1+9b•)G(LD or by solving for b•., b•= (1/9)[-G(L)/G(Lk)- 1]. (16) The values of b•.can be computed from this equation from the observed values of L and L• 20 -10 0 10 20 L0U0NES$ 30 LEVEL 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 OF EACH COMPONENT-DB FzG.9. Loudnesslevelsof complextoneshaving ten equally loud components 100 cycles apart. by usingthe valuesof G given in Table III. Because of the difficulty in obtaining accurate valuesof L and L• suchcomputedvaluesof bk will be rather inaccurate.Consequently,considerablefreedomis left in choosinga simple LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT 5.0 AND CALCULATION 97 agreewith thesetwo setsof data, Q was made to depend upon x=fi+30 4.0 log f-95 instead of L•. It was found when using this function of/5 and f as an abscissaand the same ordinates as in Fig. 10, a value of Q was obtained which gives just as good a fit for the data of Fig. 10 and also gives a better fit for the data of Figs. 8 and 9. Other much more complicated factors were tried 3.0 X 2.0 1.0 to make 0 0 20 40 LOUONE$1• 60 LEVEL 80 100 120 OF COMPONENT'OB FIG. 11. Loudness factor Q. Table formula which will represent the results. When the values of b/, derived in this way were plotted with bk as ordinates and zXfas abscissaeand Lk as a variable parameter then the resulting graphs were a series of straight lines going through the common point (-250, 0) but having slopes depending upon L•,. Consequently the following formula bk= [-(250+/xf)/lOOO-]Q(L•.) (17) will representthe results.The quantity Af is the common difference in frequency between the components,Lk the loudnesslevel of each component, and Q a function depending upon Lk. The results indicated that Q could be represented by the curve in Fig. 11. Also the condition must be imposed upon this equation that b is always taken as unity whenever the calculation gives values greater than unity. The solid curves shown in Fig. 10 are actually calculated curves using these equations, so the comparison of these curves with the observed points gives an indication of how well this equation fits the data. For this seriesof tones Q could be made to depend upon fi• rather than L• and approximately the same results would be obtained since fi• and L• are nearly equal in this range of frequencies.However, for tones having low intensities and low frequencies, fik will be the observed and calculated results shownin thesetwo figurescomeinto better agreement but none were more satisfactory than the simpleprocedureoutlinedabove. For purposeof calculation the values of Q are tabulated in VI. •I'^]•L• VI. Valuesof Q(X). X 0 0 10 20 30 40 5.00 3.82 2.64 1.60 1.09 0.90 0.88 50 60 70 0.90 80 1.04 90 1.27 100 1.51 1 4.88 3.70 2.52 1.53 1.06 0.90 0.88 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 4.76 3.58 ;!.40 1.47 1.03 0.89 0.88 4.64 3.46 2.28 1.40 1.01 0.89 0.88 4.53 3.35 2.16 1.35 0.99 0.88 0.88 4.41 4.20 4.17 4.05 3.94 3.33 3.11 2.99 2.87 2.76 1.95 1.25 0.95 0.88 0.88 0.97 1.17 1.41 1.64 1.85 1.76 1.68 1.20 1.16 1.13 0.94 0.92 0.91 0.88 0.88 0.88 0.89 0.89 0.90 0.99 1.00 1.02 1.19 1.22 1.24 1.44 1.46 1.48 1.67 1.69 1.71 0.91 (}.92 0.93 1.06 1.08 1.10 1.29 1.31 1.34 1.53 1.55 1.58 Note: X=•t.+30 2.05 1.30 0.97 0.88 0.88 0.94 0.96 1.13 1.15 1.36 1.39 1.60 1.62 log fz.-95. There are reasonsbased upon the mechanicsof hearing for treating componentswhich are very closetogether by a separatemethod. When they are close together the combination must act as thoughthe energywere all in a singlecomponent, sincethe componentsact uponapproximatelythe same set of nerve terminals. For this reason it seemslogicalro combinethem by the energylaw and treat the combinationas a singlefrequency. That some such procedureis necessaryis shown from the absurdities into which one is led when one tries to make Eq. (17) applicable to all cases. For example, if 100 components were crowded into a 1000-cyclespaceabout a 1000-cycletone, then it is obvious that the combination should much larger than L7• and consequently Q will be sound about 20 db louder. But according to smaller and hence the calculated Eq. (10) to make this true for valuesof L• greater than 45, bkmust be chosenas 0.036. Similarly, for 10 tones thus crowded together L-L• must be loudness smaller. The results in Figs. 8 and 9 are just contrary to this. To make the calculated and observed results 98 HARVEY FLETCHER about 10 db. and therefore be=0.13 and then for two such tones L-L• must be 3 db and the cor- respondingvalue of b• = 0.26. These three values must belong to the same condition zXf=10. It is evident then that the formulae for b given by Eq. (17) will lead to very erroneousresults for AND W. A. MUNSON z•L = L•--L•. (20) Also let this differencebe T when L• is adjusted so that the masking componentjust masks the componentk. Then the function for calculating b must satisfy the following conditions: such components. b•=[-(250+z_Xf)/lOOO-]Q when ,XL=0, In order to cover suchcasesit was necessaryto b•=0 when•L=-T. group together all componentswithin a certain frequencyband and treat them as a singlecomAlso the following condition when L• is larger ponent. Since there was no definite criterion for than L•, must be satisfied,namely, be= 1 when determining accurately what theselimiting bands should be, several were tried and ones selected which gave the best agreement between computed and observed results. The following band widths were finally chosen: For frequencies below 2000 cycles, the band width is 100 cycles; for frequenciesbetween 2000 and 4000 cycles,the band width is 200 cycles;for frequenciesbetween 4000 and 8000 cycles, the band width is 400 cycles; and for frequenciesbetween 8000 and 16,000 cycles, the band width is 800 cycles. If there are k componentswithin one of these limiting bands, the intensity I taken for the equivalent single frequency component is given by I= E I• = • 10•mø. (18) A frequencymust be assignedto the combination. It seemsreasonableto assigna weighted value of f given by the equation f=•. f•,Ie/I=Z f•10a•/lø,/E 10ateø. (19) Only a small error will be introduced if the midfrequency of such bands be taken as the fre- 6L= some value somewhat smaller than +T. The value of T can be obtained from masking curves. An examination of these data indicates that to a good approximation the value of T is dependentupon the singlevariable f•.--2f,,. A curve showing the relation between T and this variable is shown in Fig. 12. It will be seen that I I for CASE WHœ• f)fm VALU•:S a•' af-fm-f -•fm FIG. 12. Valuesof the maskingT. for most practical casesthe value of T is 25. It cannot be claimed that the curve of Fig. 12 is an accurate representation of the masking data, but quency of an equivalent componentexcept for it is sufficientlyaccuratefor the purposeof loudthe band of lowest frequency. Below 125 cyclesit nesscalculation since rather large changesin T is important that the frequencyand intensity of will producea very slight changein the final caleach component be known, since in this region culated loudness level. Data were taken in an effort to determine how the loudnesslevel Le changesvery rapidly with both changes in intensity and frequency. However, if the intensity for this band is lower than that for other bands, it will contribute little to the total loudness so that only a small error will be introduced by a wrong choice of frequency for this function depended upon aXLbut it was not possibleto obtain sufficientaccuracyin the experimental results. The difference between the resultant loudness level when half the tones are down so as not to contribute to loudness and the band. when these are equal is not more than 4 or 5 db, This then gives a method of calculating be which is not much more than the observational when the adjacent componentsare equal in loud- errors in such results. ness.When they are not equal let us define the difference/XL by A series of tests were made with tones similar to those used to obtain the resultsshownin Figs. LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT AND CALCULATION 99 zXL=-25, the most probable value of T. For /xf= 100 and Q = 0.88 we will obtain the smallest was made in which every other component was value of b/• without applying the 2•L factor, down 10 db. Although these data were not usedin namely, 0.31. Then when using this factor as determining the function described above, it was given above, all values of b•,:will be unity for useful as a check on the final equations derived values of AL greater than 12 db. Several more complicated functions of zXLwere for calculating the loudnessof tones of this sort. The factor finally chosenfor representing the tried but no•e of them gave results showing a dependenceof bk upon /XLis 10 aL/v.This factor better agreement with the experimental values is unity for AL=0, fulfilling the first condition than the function chosen above. 8 and 9 except that every other component was down in loudness level 5 db. Also a second series mentioned above. It is 0.10 instead of zero for The formula for calculation of b•:then becomes b• = [(250+f• -f,•)/lOOO•lO(•-•"')/rO(3•:+30 where f• is the frequency of the component expressedin cyclesper second, f,,•is the frequencyof the maskingcomponent expressedin cycles per second, L•. is the loudnesslevel of the kth component when soundingalone, L• is the loudnesslevel of the masking tone, Q is a function depending upon the intensity level fi• and the frequencyf•; of each ponent and is given in Table VI as a function of x=fi•+30 logf•-95, T is the masking and is given by the curve of (21) whereb•.is given by Eq. (21). If the valuesoff• and • are measureddirectly then corresponding valuesof L• can be found from Fig. 5. Having these values, the masking component can be found either by inspection or better by trial in Eq. (21). That componentwhose values of L,•, f,• and T introduced into this equation gives the smallestvalue of b• is the maskingcomponent. The values of G and Q can be found from Tables III and VI from the correspondingvalues of L•-, ilk, and f•. If all thesevalues are now introduced into Eq. (10), the resulting value of the summation is.the loudnessof the complextone. The loudnesslevel L correspondingto it is found from Table Fig. 12. logf•- 95) I[I. If it is desired to know the loudness obtained if It is important to remember that b• can neverbe greater than unity so that all calculatedvalues greaterthan this mu.•tbereplacedwith valuesequal to unity. Also all components within the limiting frequencybandsmust be groupedtogetheras indicatedabove.It is very helpful to remember that any component for which the loudnesslevel is 12 db below the kth component, that is, the one for xvhichb is being calculated, need not be considered as possibly being the masking component. If all the components preceding the kth are in this classthen b• is unity. these limitations the formula for calculat- ing the loudnesslevel L of a steady complex tone having n components is G(L) = 5• b•G(L•), will be obtained if the summation indicated in Eq. (10) is divided by 2. Practically the same result will be obtained in most instances if the loudnesslevel L• for each component when listened to ;vith one ear instead of both ears is in- sertedin Eq. (10). (G(Lx.)for one ear listeningis equal to one--halfG(L•) for listening with both ears for the samevalue of the intensity level of the component.) If two complex tones are listened to, one in one ear and one in the other, it would be expected that the combined loudness would be the sum of the two loudness values calculated for RECAPITULATION With the typical listener used only one ear, the result (10) each ear as though no soundwere in the opposite ear, although this has not been confirmed by experimental trial. In fact, the loudness reduction factor b• has been derived from data taken with both ears only, so strictly speaking, its use is limited to this type of listening. 100 HARVEY FLETCHER AND To illustrate the method of using the formula the loudnessof two complex tones will be calcu- W. A. MUNSON sections,the results of a large number of tests are given here, including those from which the lated. The first may represent the hum from a formula was derived. In Tables VII to XIII, the dynamo. Its componentsare given in the table first column shows the frequency range over which the componentsof the tones were distribof computations. uted, the figuresbeing the frequenciesof the first Computations and last components. Several tones having two componentswere tested, but as the tables indicate, the majority of the tones had ten com1 60 50 3 3 1.0 ponents. Becauseof a misunderstandingin the 2 180 45 25 191 1.0 ZbeGle= 1009 designof the apparatus for generatingthe latter 3 300 40 30 360 1.0 4 540 30 27 252 1.0 L= 40 tones, a number of them contained eleven com5 1200 25 25 197 1.0 ponents, so for purposesof identification, these are placed in a separate group. In the second column of the tables, next to the frequency range The first step is to find from Fig. 5 the values of the tones, the frequency difference (•f) beof L• fromf, and •t•. Then the loudness valuesG, tween adjacent components is given. The reare found from Table III. Since the values of L mainder of the data pertains to the loudnesslevels are low'and the frequency separationfairly large, of the tones. Opposite L• are given the common one familiar with these functions would readily loudness levels to which all the componentsof the see that the values of b would be unity and a tone were adjusted for a particular test, and in computationwould verify it so that the sum of the G values gives the total loudness1009. This the next line the results of the test, that is, the corresponds to a loudness levelof 40. observedloudnesslevels (Lo•,,.), are given. Directly beneath each observed value, the calculated loudnesslevels (L•.) are shown. The three The second tone calculated is this same hum amplified30 db. It better illustratesthe useof the formula. Computations k f• • L• 1 60 2 180 3 300 4 540 5 1200 80 75 70 60 55 69 72 69 60 55 G• f,, L,•(301ogf•-95)Q 7440 9130 60 69 7440 180 72 4350 300 69 3080 540 60 ---28 --21 --13 -- 3 -0.91 0.91 0.94 0.89 b bXG 1.00 0.41 0.27 0.23 0.61 7440 3740 2010 1000 1880 loudness G= 16070 loudness level L = 79 db associated values of L•, Loh,., and L•.•t½.in each column representthe data for one completetest. For example, in Table VIII, the first tone is described as having ten components, and for the first test showneach componentwas adjusted to have a loudnesslevel (L•) of 67 db. The resultsof the test gave an observed loudnesslevel (Lo•,.) of 83 db for the ten componentsacting together, and the calculated loudnesslevel (L•.,lo.) of this same tone was 81 db. The probable error of the observed results in the tables is approximately 4-2 db. The loudness level of the combined tones is only 7 db abovethe loudness levelof the second component.If only one ear is usedin listening, the loudness of this tone is one-half, correspond- In the next seriesof data, adjacent components T•BLE VII. Two componenttones(AL=0). Loudnesslevels (db) Frequency range ing to a loudness levelof 70 db. L• Lo•,•. Leale. 83 87 87 63 68 68 43 47 47 23 28 28 2 2 4 000 Lt: Lon•. Le•c. 83 89 91 63 71 74 43 4o 52 23 28 28 --1 2 l 875 L& Lobs. ' Leale. 84 92 92 1000-1100 COMPARISONOF OBSERVED AND CALCULATED RESULTS OXi THE LOUDNESS LEVELS OF COl•IPLEX TONES ßIn order to show the agreementbetweenobserved loudnesslevels and levels calculated by meansof the formula developediv the preceding 1000-2000 125-1000 LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITI()N, MEASUREMENT TABLE VIII. CALCULATION Loudnesslevels (db) 50 500 Lk Lo•,•. L•d•. 67 83 81 54 68 72 33 47 53 21 38 39 11 2:0 2:4 -- 1 2 8 50 500 Lk Lobs. L•ale. 78 92 91 61 73 77 41 53 60 23 42 42 13 2:5 27 --1 2 8 L• 78 69 50 16 6 Lobs. L•::m•. 94 93 82 83 62 65 32 31 22 17 2 0 L• Lobs. Le=•. 57 68 73 37 50 52 20 34 36 3 2 5 84 95 100 64 83 83 43 59 68 24 41 47 2 2 12 84 94 100 L• Lo•s. 81 93 64 82 43 65 23 49 13 33 -4 Lcale. 98 83 68 45 27 0 1400 1895 1400-1895 L, Lobs. L•,=m,. 100-1000 100 1000 -1 83 63 43 23 95 79 59 43 2 L•.•d•. 99 82 68 45 9 L• Lo•,•. Leal½. 83 100 100 63 82 80 43 59 60 23 32 38 78 99 95 59 81 77 1100-3170 L• Lobs. L½•t0. 79 100 100 60 81 83 41 65 64 17 33 34 7 22 18 --4 2 3 260-2600 L• Lobs. Leale. 79 97 100 62 82 85 42 65 68 23 44 45 13 28 27 - 2 2 5 3100 3900 L• 530 5300 530 5300 - 7 2 0 53 43 25 82 61 43 17 -- 2 83 82 73 72 52 48 105 108 90 89 73 72 40 34 2 5 Lk Lobs. 61 89 41 69 21 45 --3 2 Lea•½. 89 70 42 4 In thefollowing setoftests(Tables XII and XIII) the differencein loudnesslevel of adjacent componentswas 10 db. The next data are the results of tests made on the complex tone generated by the Western Electric No. 3A audiometer. When analyzed, this tone was found to have the voltage level specXIV. 27 38 42 75 loudness level given opposite L•, and the even numbered components were 5 db lower. (Tables X and XI.) Table 48 62 65 100 101 had a difference in loudness level of 5 db, that is, in 24 44 47 Lob•. Lcale. the first, third, fifth, etc., components had the shown 43 63 68 When the r.m.s. 2 2 12 3 L• 1000 64 80 83 2 Lob•. 100 101 Ten componenttones(zXL=0). Frequency range trum AND voltage acrossthe receiversused was unity, that is, zero voltage level, then the separate componentshad the voltage levelsgiven in this table. Adding to the voltage levels the calibration constant for the receivers used in making the loud- nesstests giw•s the values of fi for zero voltage level acrossthe receivers.The values of • for any other voltage level are obtained by addition of the level desired. Tests were made on the audiometer tone with the same receivers* that were used with the other complex tones, but in addition, data were avail* See calibration shownin Fig. 1. 102 HARVEY FLETCHER AND W. TABLE IX. Elevencomponent tones(/•L=0). Frequency range 1000-2000 1000-2000 1150-2270 100 112 MUNSON TABLE XIII. Loudness levels (db) 100 A. Elevencomponenttones(•XL= 10 db). Frequency range af 57 L• 84 64 43 24 Loba. 97 83 65 43 Lc•le. 103 84 64 45 --1 2 7 57-627 Lk 84 64 43 24 Lobs. 99 82 65 42 Leale. 103 84 64 45 1 2 11 3420-4020 Lk Lobs. Lc•ic. 79 60 40 20 99 78 62 41 10 --5 25 2 98 81 61 40 23 Lk Lobs. Le•lc. 77 62 42 22 102 86 66 46 101 88 69 44 60 Loudnesslevels (db) L• Lob•. 80 88 L•lc. 90 L• Lobs. L•x•. 62 70 42 53 27 17 40 27 76 59 45 30 81 62 42 27 100 70 50 33 94 75 53 37 17 26 27 2 2 8 --4 2 0 TABLE XIV. Voltage level spectrum ofNo.3A 1 audiometer tone. 1120-4520 340 7 --7 2 20 19 - 1 Frequency Voltagelevel ----- 2.1 5.4 4.7 5.9 2128 2280 2432 2584 --11.4 -- 16.9 --14.1 -- 16.2 760 912 1064 1216 1368 1520 1672 1824 1976 --------- 4.6 6.8 6.0 8.1 7.6 9.1 10.0 9.9 14.1 2736 2880 3040 3192 3344 3496 3648 3800 3952 -- 17.4 -17.5 --20.0 --19.4 -- 22.7 --23.7 -- 25.6 -- 24.6 --26.8 TABLE X. Ten componenttones(•L = 5 db). Frequency range Loudnesslevels (db) 1725-2220 55 L• 82 62 43 27 Loba. 101 73 54 38 Lcalc. 95 76 56 40 17 30 30 --6 2 -1 55 L• Lobs. Lc•t•. 12 22 22 -2 1725-2220 80 62 42 22 94 66 50 33 93 76 54 35 2 4 Frequency Voltage level 152 304 456 608 given instead of voltage levels,so in utilizing it here, it was necessaryto assumethat the thresh- TABLEXI. Elevencomponent tones(AL = 5 db). old levels of the new and old tests were the same. Frequency range Loudnesslevels (db) 57 627 57 L• 79 Lobs. 91 Lc,,lc. 90 3420-4020 60 L• Loba. Lcfii. 76 95 89 61 41 73 56 76 59 26 41 43 16 28 28 61 77 75 25 33 36 15 25 26 42 55 54 1 2 8 Computations were made at the levels tested experimentally and a comparisonof observedand calculated results is shown in Table XV. -9 TABLE XV. 2 --4 A. Recent r.m.s. level Lobs. Leale. TABLE XII. Ten cmnponent tones(zXL= 10 db). Frequency range •f 1725-2220 55 1725-2220 55 Loudnesslevels (db) tests on No. 3A audiometer tone. volt. --38 --55 --59 --70 --75 --78 --80 --87 --89 --100 --102 95 85 79 61 56 41 42 28 22 2 2 89 74 71 57 49 44 40 :28 B. P•evious tests on No. 3A audiometer L• 79 59 40 19 9 --5 Lobs. Leale. 95 91 7l 73 54 51 33 31 22 17 2 --1 Lk 79 Lob•. 89 LcMc. 92 61 67 75 41 48 53 27 37 39 17 27 28 --1 2 4 r.m.s. volt. level Loba. LcMc. 25 7 4 tone. +10 118 9 103 --40 77 --49 69 --6(I 6l --69 50 --91 2 119 103 82 73 56 41 6 The agreement of observed and calculated resuits is poor for some of the tests, but the close agreement in the recent data at loxvlevels and in the previous data at high levels indicates that the able on tests made about six years ago using a observed results are not as accurate as could be differenttype of receiver.This latter type of re- desired. Because of the labor involved these tests ceiver was recalibrated (Fig. 13) and computa- have not been repeated. tions made for both the old and new tests. In the At the time the tests were made severalyears older set of data, levels above threshold were ago on the No. 3A audiometer tone, the reduc- LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT AND CALCULATION 103 ,: FiG. 14. (A to D) Iouduesslevel reduction tests on the No. 3A audiometer FIG. 13. Calibration of receivers for tests on the No. 3A audiometer tone tone. testing the method of measuringand calculating loudness levels. rio,1 in loudnesslevel which takes place when In view of the complexnature of the problem this computation method cannot be considered ternfined. As this can be readily calculatedwith fully developedin all its details and as more acthe formula developedhere, a comparisonof ob- curate data accumulatesit may be necessaryto served and calculated results will be shown. In change the formula for b. Also at the higher levels Fig. 14A, the ordinate is the reduction in loud- someattention must be given to phasedifferences ness level resulting when a No. 3A audiometer between the components.However, we feel that tone having a loudnesslevel of 42 db was changed the form of the equation is fundamentally correct by the insertion of a filter which eliminated all of and the loudness function, G, corresponds to the componentsabove or below the frequency in- somethingreal in the mechanismof hearing. The dicated on the abscissa. The observed data are the present values given for G may be modified plotted points and the smooth curvesare calcu- slightly, but we think that they will not be lated results. A similar comparison is shown in radically changed. Figs. 14B, C and D for other levels. A study of the loudness of complex sounds This completesthe data which are available on which are not steady, such as speechand sounds steady complextones. It is to be hoped that others of varying duration, is in progressat the present will find the field of sufficientimportanceto war- time and the resultswill be reported in a second rant obtaining additional data for improving and paper on this subject. certain components are eliminated was also de- APPENDIX A measurelncnt A. EXPERIMENTAL •IETHOD OF •IEASURING of the loudness leYel of a sound consists THE LOUDNEqS LEYEL OF A •TEADV •OUND method of averaf/e error, constant stimuli, etc., but also in of listeningalternately to the soundand to the 1000-cycle important experinIentaldetails suchas the control of noise reference tone and adjusting the latter until the two are conditions and fatigue effects. In some instancesuniqne equally loud. If the intensity level of the reference tone is L decibels when this condition is reached, the sound is devices have been used to facilitate a read_v comparison of sounds. One of these, the alternation phonolueter,•ø said to have introducesinto the comparisonimportant factors sut•has a loudness level of L decibels. When the characterof the soundbeingmeasureddiffersonly slightly froin that of the referencetone, the comparisonis easilyand the duration time of the sounds and the effect of transient quickly made, but for other soundsthe numerousfactors which enter into a judglnent of equality of loudnessbecome conditions. The merits of a particular method will delx-nd upon the circumstancesunder which it is to be used.The one to be described here was developed for an extensive important, series of laboratory tests. and an experimental method should be used which will yield results typical of the average normal ear To dctermine when two sounds are eqnally loud it iq and nornmlphysiologicaland psychological conditions. A variety of methodshave been proposedto accomplish •0 D. Mackenzie, RelativeSensitivityof theF•ar at Different this, differing not only in generalclassification,that is, the Levelsof Lo•td•tes•,Phys. Rev. 20, 331 (1922). 104 HARVEY FLETCHER AND necessaryto rely upon the judgment of an observer, and this involves of course, not only the ear mechanism, but also associatedmental processes,and effectively hnbeds the problem in a variety of psychological factors. Thesc difficulties are enhanced by the large variations found in the judgments of different observers, necessitating an investigation conducted on a statistical basis. The method of constant stimuli, wherein the observer listens to fixed levels of the two sounds and estimates which sound is the louder, seemedbest adapted to control the many factors involved,when usingseveralobserverssimultaneously.By meansof this method,an observer'spart in the test can be readily limited to an indication of his loudnessjudgment. This is essential as it was found that manipulation of apparatus controls, even though they were not calibrated, or participation in any way other than as a iudge of loudness values, introduced undesirable factors which were aggravated by continued use of the same observers over a long period of time. Control of fatigue, memory effects, and the associationof an observer'sjudgments with the results of the tests or with the iudgments of Other observerscould be rigidly maintained with this method, as will be seen from the detailed explanation of the experi- W. control the reference tone and the sounds to be whether the reference measured.Vacuum tube oscillatorswere usedto generate pure tones, and for complex tones and other sounds, suitablesourceswere substituted.By meansof the voltage measuringcircuit and the attenuator, the voltage level (voltagelevel= 20 log V) impressedupon the terminalsof the receivers, could be determined. For example, the or softer than the booth. The typical recording chart shown in Fig. 16 contains the results of three observerstesting a 12S-cycletone at three different levels. Two marks were used for recording 125 c.p.s. Pure Tone Test No. 4 Crew No. 1. 1000 c.p.s. Voltage Level (db) Obs. 4-6 4-2 -2 -6 -10 CK 4- + 4- 4- 4- 0 0 0 c.p.s. AS + + d- + 0 0 0 0 0 Volt. DH level= CK 4-9.8db AS DE[ CK AS DH 4+ 44+ 44- 4+ 44+ 4+ 0 + 40 + 40 0 + q0 + 0 0 0 + 0 40 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 --4 --8 --12 125 125 -14-18-22 -26 0 --16--20--24--28--32 CK 4- + 4- 4- 0 4- 4- 0 0 c.p.s. AS 4- 4- 4- 4- + 0 0 0 0 h+ 4+ + + + + + 4h+ + + 4+ hh+ + + 4+ h0 + + 0 0 44+ + + + 0 + 40 + 0 0 0 40 h0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 --15 --19 --23 --27 --31 --35 --39 --43 --47 h+ + 0 + + h+ + + 44- h+ 0 + + 0 h+ + 4+ 4- + 0 0 + 0 0 0 0 + + + 0 0 0 0 0 0 4- 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 + + 4- + 44- o 0 0 + 0 0 + + 0 + + 0 0 0 4- 0 0 0 DH CK AS DH CK AS DH 125 CK c.p.s. AS for loudness balances. tone was louder other sound and indicate their opinions by operating the switches.The levels were then changed and the procednre repeated. The results of the tests were recorded outside the Volt. level= -3.2db FIG. 15. Circuit MUNSON of one second.After a pause of one secondthis sequence was repeated, and then they were required to estimate mental procedure. The circuit shown in Fig. 15 was employed to generate and A. Volt. level= -14.2 db DH CK AS DH CK AS DH Fm. 16. Loudness balance 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 data sheet. attenuator, which was calibrated in decibels, was set so the observers'judgments,a cipherindicatingthe 125-cycle that the voltage measuringset indicated 1 volt was being tone to be the louder, and a plus sign denoting the reference impressed upon the receiver. Then the difference between tone to be the louder of the two. No equal judgments this settingand any other settingis the voltagelevel. To were permitted. The figures at the head of each column give the voltage level of the reference tone impressed upon the receivers, that is, the number of decibels from 1 volt, plus if above and minus if below, and thoseat the side are similar values for the tone being tested. Successivetests were chosenat random from the twenty-seven possible obtain the intensity level of the sound we must know the calibration of the receivers. The observers were seated in a sound-proof booth and were required only to listen and then operate a simple switch. These switcheswere provided at each position and were arranged so that the operations of one observer could not be seenby another. This was necessaryto prevent the judgmentsof oneobserverfrom influencingthoseof another observer.First they heardthe soundbeing tested,and immediately afterwards the referencetone, each for a period combinationsof levelsshown,thus reducingthe possibility of memory effects. The levels were selected so the observers listened to reference tones which were louder and softer than the tone being tested and the median of their judgments was taken as the point of equal loudness. LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT The data on this recordingchart, combinedx•ith a similar number of observationsby the rest of the crew, •ND C•LCULtkTION 105 gradually. Relays operating in thefeedback circuits of the vacuumtube oscillatorsand in the grid circuitsof anq)lifiers (a total of elevenobservers) are shownin graphicalform performedthis operation.The period of grox•thand 0.1 secondas ,own on the in Fig.17.Thearrowindicates themedian levelat which decaywas approximately typical oscillogram in Fig. I9. \Vith thesealex icesthe vo,'r^Gd L•&-' 7 :_-c__ 0 ! e'Ec FIG. 17. Percentof obser•ations estimating 1000-cycletone to be louder than 125-cycle tone. the 1000-c.xcle reference, in theopinionof thisgroupof observers, sounded equallyloudto the 12S-cycle tone. The testingmethodadoptedwasinfluenced by efforts to minimizefatigueeffects,both mentaland physical. Mentalfatigueandprobable changes in theattitudeof an DECAY FXG.19. Growth and decayof 1000-cyclereferencetone. observer duringthe progress of a longseriesof testswere detected by keeping a recordof the spreadof eachob- transient effects were reducedanti yet the sotrodsseemed server'sresttits.As longas the spreadwas normalit was assumed that the fatigue,if present,wassmall.The tests were conductedon a time schedulewhich limited the observersto five minutesof continuoustesting, during whichtimeapproximately fifteenobserYations weremade. The maximum number of observationspermitted in one day was 150. To avoid fatiguingthe ear the soundsto whichthe to start and stop instantaneously unlessattention was called to the effect. -k motor-driven commutator operated the relayswhichstartedand stoppedthe sountis in proper sequence, and sxGtched the receixers fromthe reference tone circuit to the sound under test. The customaryroutine measurements to insure the propervoltagelevelsimpressed uponthe receivers were madewith the measuringcircuit shownschematicallyin Fig. 15. Duringthe progress of the testsvoltagemeasureseqnence illustrated onFig.18.Theduration timeofeach mentswere made frequentlyand later correlatedwith observerslistened were of short duration and in the measurementsof the correspondingfield sound pressures. Thresholdmeasurements were madebeforeanti after the loudness tests.They weretakenon the samecircuit used for the loudnesstests (Fig. 15) by turning off the 1000- cycleoscillator andslox• ly attenuating theothertouebelow FIG. 18. Time sequence for loudness comparisons. thresholdand then raisingthe level until it again became audible.The observers signalledwhenthey couldno longer hearthe toneand thenagainwhenit wasjust amlible.The soundhadto belongenough to attainfull loudness andyet average ofthesetwoconditions wastakenasthethreshold. notsufficiently longto fatiguetheear.The refereuce tone An analysisof the harmonicsgeneratedby the refollowedthe x soundat a time intervalshortenoughto ceiversand other apparatuswas madeto be sureof the permita readycomparison, andyet not be subject to purityof the tonesreaching the ear.The receivers were fatigueby prolonging thestimulation withoutanadequate of theelectrodynamic typeand • erefoundto produceoverrestperiod.At highlevelsit wasfoundthat a tonere- tonesof the orderof 50 decibelsbelowthe fundamental.At quiresnearly0.3 second to reachfull loudness antiif the veryhighlexels,distortionfromthe filterswasgreater sustainedfor longerperiodsthan one second,thereis than from the receivers,but in all casesthe loudness level dangerof fatiguingthe ear.u of anyovertone was20decibels or morebelowthatof the To avoidtheobjectionable transients whichoccurx•hen fundamental.Experiencewith complextoneshas shown soundsare interruptedsuddenlyat high levels,the con- that under thesecomlitionsthe contribution of the overtrollingcircuitwasdesigned to startandstopthesounds tones to the total loudnessis insignificant. n G. v. Bekesy,Theoryof IIearing, Phys.Zeits.30, 115 (1929). The methodof measuringloudnesslevel which is de. scribedherehasbeenusedon a largevariety of soundsand foundto give satisfactoryrest,Its. 106 HARVEY APPENDIX B. FLETCHER AND COMPARISON OF DATA listened to the tones with both ears in the tests reported here, while a single receiver was used by Kingsbury. Also, it is intportant to remember that the level of the tonesusedin the experimentswasexpressed as the number of db above the averagethresholdcurrent obtained with a single receiver. For both of these reasons a direct comparison of the results cannot be made. However, in the course of our work two sets of experiments were made which give results that make it possibleto reduce Kingsbury's data so that it nray be compared directly with that reported in this paper. In the first set of experiments it was found that if a typical observerlistened with both ears and esti•nated that two tones, the referencetone and a tone of different frequency, appeared equally loud, then, nraking a similar comparisonusing one ear (the voltages on the receiver remaining unchanged)he would still estimate that the two tones were equally loud. The results upon which this TAnLE XVI. XVI. A. MUNSON ON 1HE LOUDNESS LEVELS OF PURE TONES A comparisonof the present loudnessdata with that reported previouslyby B. A. Kingsbury4 would be desirable and in the event of agreement, would lend support to the general application of the results as representativeof the average ear. It will be remembered that the observers conclusion is based are shown in Table W. when the soundsare comingdirectly to the earsfrom a free wave. This result is in agreement with the point of view adoptedin developingthe formula for calculatingloudness. When listeningwith oneinsteadof two ears,the loudnessof the referencetone and also that of the tone being compared are reducedto one-half. Consequently,if they were equally loud when listeningwith two earsthey mustbe equally loud when listening with one ear. The secondset of data is concerned with differences in the threshold when listening with one ear versuslistening with two ears. It is well knownthat for any individualthe two earshave different acuity. Consequently,when listening with both ears the threshold is determined principally by the better ear. The curve in Fig. 20 shows the difference in the •o so •oo zoo 500 •ooo zooo sooo ,oooo zoooo In the first F•o. 20. Differencein acuity betweenthe best ear and the average of both ears. Comparisonof one and two-ear loudness balances. A. Reference Frecluency, c.p.s. Voltage level difference* 62 --0.5 i25 0 tone voltage level 250 500 2000 4000 6000 8000 10,000 +1.0 --1.0 --0.5 --0.5 4-0.5 --3.0 --3.0 B. Other Ref. tone volt. level --32rib reference 62 c.p.s. Volt. level difference* tone levels 2000 c.p.s. Ref. tone Volt. level volt. level difference* -20 --34 --57 +0.5 +0.2 +2.0 -- 3 -22 --41 0.0 +0.3 --0.8 --68 --0.5 --60 --0.8 --7 ½ ) --6.2 threshold level between the average of the better of an observer'sears and the average of eli the ears. The. circles representdata taken on the observersusedin our loudness tests while the crosses represent data taken from an analysisof 80 audiogramsof personswith normal hearing. If the difference in acuity when listening with one ear rs. listeningwith two ears is determinedentirely by the better ear, then the curve shown gives this difference.However, someexperimentaltests which we made on one ear acuity vs. two ear acuity showed the latter to be slightly greater than for the better ear alone, but the small magnitudes involved and the difficulty of avoiding psychologicaleffects causeda probable error of the same order of magnitude as * Differences are in (lb, positive values indicating a higher voltage for the one ear balance. row are shown the frequenciesof the tones tested. Under these frcquenciea are ahown the differences in db of the voitage levels on the receivers obtained when listening by the two methods, the voltage level of the referencetone being constant at 32 db down frmn I volt. Under the caption "Other ReferenceTone Levels" similar figuresfor frequenciesof 62 c.p.s. and 2000 c.p.s. and for the levels of the referencetone indicated are given. It will be seen that these differences are well within the observational error. Consequently, the conclusion mentioned above seems to be justified. This is an important conclusionand although the data are confined to tests made with receivers on the ear it would be expectedthat a similar relation would hold Fro. 21. Loudnesslevels of pure tones--A comparisonwith Kingsbury's data. LOUDNESS, ITS DEFINITION, MEASUREMENT A\'D CALCULATION 107 the qualitybeingmeasured. At the higherfrequencies If now we add to the values for the level above threshold anamountcorresponding to thedifferwherelargedifferences are usuallypresent the acuityis givenby Kingsbury deternfinedentirely by the better ear. From valuesof the loudnessfunctionG, onecan readily calcnlatewhat the differencein acuity when usingone z,s. two earsshouldbe. Sucha calculationindicatesthat when the two earshavethesameacuity,thenwhenlisteningwith both earsthe thresholdvaluesare about 2 db lowerthan encesshownby the curveof Fig. 20, then the resulting valuesshouldbe directly comparableto our data on the basisof decibelsabove threshold. Comparisonsof his data on this basisaith thosereportedin this paperare shownin Fig. 21. The solidcontourlineqaredrawnthroughpoints taken from Table I and the dotted contour linc• taken data.It willbeseenthat thetxvosetsof whenlistening with oneear.Thissmalldifference would fromKingsbury's data are in goodagreementbetween100and 2000cycles above and belowthesepoints. We are nowin a positionto compare the dataof Kings- but divergesomexxhat are slightly greaterthan would be burywiththose shown in TableI. Thedatain TableI can The discrepancies fromexperimental errors,but mightbeexplained be convertedinto decibelsabovethresholdby subtracting expected accountfor the difficultyin t%ing to measureit. ofa slightamount ofnoise duringthreshold theaverage threshold valuein eachcolumn frmnanyother bythepresence deternfinations. number in the same column. APPENDIXC. OPTICALTONE (;ENER•,TOR OF COMPLEX. •V•,VEFORMS For the loudnesstests in which the referencetone was compared with a complex tonehavingcomponents of specified loudness levelsand frequencies, the toneswere listened to bymeans of headreceivers asbefore; thecircuit shownin Fig. 15 remaining the sameexcepting for the vacuumtube oscillatormarked "x Frequency."This was replaced by a complex tonegenerator dexiscdby E. C. Wenteof the Bell TelephoneLaboratories.The generator is shownschematicallyin Fig. 22. The desiredwave form wasaccuratelydrawn on a large scaleand then transferredphotographicalIsto the glass diskdesignated asD in thediagram. Thedisk,drivenbya motor,rotatedbetween the lampL anda photoelectric cell C, producing a fluctuating light sourcewhichwas directed by a suitable opticalsystem uponthe plateof the cell.The voltagegenerated wasalnplifiedandattenuated as in the case of the pure tones. FIG. 22. Schematic of optical tone generator. FIG. 23. Ten disk optical tone generator. 108 HARVEY FLETCHER The relative magnitudes of the components were of course fixed by the form of the wave inscribed upon the disk, but this was modified when desired, by the insertion of elements in the electrical circuit which gave the desired characteristic. Greater flexibility in the control of the amplitude of the componentswas obtained by inscribing each componenton a separate disk with a complete optical system and cell for each. Frequency and phase relations were maintained by mounting all of the disks on a single shaft. Such a generator having ten disks is shown in Fig. 23. AND W. A. MUNSON An analysis of the voltage output of the optical tone generators showed an average error for the amplitude of the componentsof about 4-0.5 db, which was probably the linfit of accuracyof the measuringinstrument. Undesired harmonicsdue to the disk being off center or inaccuracies in the wave form were removed by filters in the electrical circuit. All of the tests on complex tones describedin this paper were made with the optical tone generator excepting the audiometer, and two tone tests. For the latter tests, two vacuum tube oscillators were used as a source.