Download Electrophysiological correlates of looking at paintings and its

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts
no text concepts found
Transcript
Biological Psychology 93 (2013) 246–254
Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect
Biological Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho
Electrophysiological correlates of looking at paintings and its association
with art expertise
C.Y. Pang a , M. Nadal b , J.S. Müller-Paul c , R. Rosenberg d , C. Klein e,f,∗
a
Institute for Psychology, University of Leipzig, Germany
Department of Psychology, University of the Balearic Islands, Spain
Department of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, Austria
d
Department of History of Art, University of Vienna, Austria
e
School of Psychology, Bangor University, United Kingdom
f
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Freiburg, Germany
b
c
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 22 July 2011
Accepted 31 October 2012
Available online 15 November 2012
Keywords:
EEG
ERP
Expertise
Visual art
a b s t r a c t
This study investigated the electrocortical correlates of art expertise, as defined by a newly developed,
content-valid and internally consistent 23-item art expertise questionnaire in N = 27 participants that
varied in their degree of art expertise. Participants viewed each 50 paintings, filtering-distorted versions
of these paintings and plain colour stimuli under free-viewing conditions whilst the EEG was recorded
from 64 channels. Results revealed P3b-/LPC-like bilateral posterior event-related potentials (ERP) that
were larger over the right hemisphere than over the left hemisphere. Art expertise correlated negatively
with the amplitude of the ERP responses to paintings and control stimuli. We conclude that art expertise
is associated with reduced ERP responses to visual stimuli in general that can be considered to reflect
increased neural efficiency due to extensive practice in the contemplation of visual art.
© 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Visual art is one of the most widespread forms of artistic
expression, and is created in one form or another by virtually all
human cultures. Ever since Fechner’s (1876) seminal work, the
omnipresence of art in people’s daily lives has stimulated the
empirical investigation of this anthropologic universal (Averill
et al., 1998; Silvia, 2005). Current psychological models underscore
the role of the complex interaction of mnemonic–cognitive and
evaluative–affective processes in the appreciation of art (Leder
et al., 2004). Researchers have attempted in recent years to characterise some of the attributes of art that contribute to aesthetic
appraisals such as “liking” (e.g., Jacobsen and Höfel, 2002, 2003;
Landau et al., 2006; Locher et al., 1999; Nodine et al., 1993), as well
as the influence of distortions in the composition of paintings on
participants’ aesthetic valuation (Locher et al., 1999; Vartanian and
Goel, 2004). Furthermore, neuro-aesthetic research has begun to
unveil the complex patterns of neural activation that accompany
aesthetic experience (Ramachandran and Hirstein, 1999; Nadal
and Pearce, 2011), including its perceptual (Brown et al., 2006;
∗ Corresponding author at: School of Psychology, University of Wales, Bangor, The
Brigantia Building, Penrallt Road, Bangor, Gwynedd LL57 2AS, Wales, UK.
Tel.: +44 01248 38 8351; fax: +44 01248 38 2599.
E-mail address: [email protected] (C. Klein).
0301-0511/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.10.013
Calvo-Merino et al., 2010a; Cela-Conde et al., 2009; Cupchik et al.,
2009; Koelsch et al., 2006; Lacey et al., 2011; Vartanian and Goel,
2004), cognitive-evaluative (Cela-Conde et al., 2004; Cupchik et al.,
2009; Jacobsen et al., 2006) and motivational (Blood and Zatorre,
2001; Brown et al., 2004; Cupchik et al., 2009; Ishai et al., 2007;
Kawabata and Zeki, 2004; Kirk et al., 2009a; Koelsch et al., 2006;
Salimpoor et al., 2011; Vartanian and Goel, 2004) components.
While many people across many cultures undoubtedly share a
liking for visual art, empirical studies have identified a number of
systematic inter-individual differences in people’s appreciation of
art that can be related to theoretical models of aesthetic experience.
Leder et al. (2004) comprehensive model of aesthetic appreciation
includes a number of component processes that refer to longterm memory, and hence implicit or explicit knowledge, in which
individuals obviously differ. One of these sources of systematic
inter-individual differences is expertise. It has been shown that art
experts and non-experts differ in various aspects of art perception,
including the way they explore and perceptually organise paintings
(Hekkert and van Wieringen, 1996; Kristjanson and Antes, 1989;
Schmidt et al., 1989), the way they process complexity (Reber et al.,
2004), and their aesthetic preferences and judgements (Hekkert
and van Wieringen, 1996; McWhinnie, 1968; Nodine et al., 1993).
The investigation of expertise is relevant for the study of art,
and beyond, because expertise and the accumulated effects of
long-term training are accompanied by changes in brain processes
involved in domain-relevant tasks (Gauthier et al., 2010) and can
C.Y. Pang et al. / Biological Psychology 93 (2013) 246–254
thus serve as a “natural model” of neural plasticity (Bukach et al.,
2006; Münte et al., 2002; Rossion et al., 2004, 2007; Scott et al.,
2008; Tanaka and Curran, 2001). In addition, systematic experimental task modifications during the contemplation of paintings
inspired by explicit theoretical models of aesthetic experience such
as Leder et al. (2004) model, could empirically link the effects of
training and expertise to the various components of the complex
amalgam of cognitive, evaluative and affective processes that characterise aesthetic experience.
It is thus reasonable to expect art experts to differ from
non-experts in certain aspects of brain activity while processing
art-related stimuli and performing art-related tasks. Such differences have indeed been shown with various neuroscience
techniques (Solso, 2001), including the EEG. A study in solving
chess problems, for instance, has revealed that experts as compared
to non-experts, exhibited higher EEG synchronisation in general,
including higher delta band synchrony in more posterior cortical
regions, as well as stronger right hemispheric dominance (Volke
et al., 2002). Volke and colleagues’ (1999, as cited in Volke et al.,
2002) work showed that increased EEG coherence correlated with
simple, well-mastered, and largely automatically performed tasks,
whereas decreased coherence correlated with difficult and poorly
mastered tasks. Bhattacharya and Petsche (2002) found higher EEG
phase synchronisation, particularly in the delta and gamma bands
in the right hemisphere and in more posterior brain regions, when
artists were asked to imagine a painting after viewing it. These
results are in accordance with the aforementioned findings of Volke
et al. (2002) study, and suggests the interesting possibility that contemplating paintings may be simpler for art experts, who, owing to
their greater efficiency in solving tasks in their particular domain,
might rely on more well-mastered and effortless cognitive and perceptual processes.
However, considering the art expertise literature, it is interesting to note that virtually all of the aforementioned studies defined
art “experts” according to their formal education background or
working experience in fine art or art history, and “non-experts”
as people who have equivalent educational level in areas unrelated to art (such as psychology undergraduates) but have no
formal art training. Such differentiations between (art) experts
and non-experts render art expertise a (quasi-) categorical variable
(e.g., artists and non-artists, Bhattacharya and Petsche, 2002, 2005;
Kristjanson and Antes, 1989; art-trained and untrained, Nodine
et al., 1993; experts and non-experts, Hekkert and van Wieringen,
1996; experts and novices, Schmidt et al., 1989). Such classifications, however, can be considered as the results of an artificial
dichotomisation of an otherwise continuous quantitative variable:
the degree of study or practice. Smith and Smith’s (2006) Aesthetic
Fluency Scale and Chatterjee et al. (2010) Art Experience Questionnaire, which yield a continuous measure for participants’ art
expertise, constitute attempts to overcome this drawback. Silvia’s
(2007) results suggest that such quantitative measures represent
valid means to measure the degree of expertise, and avoid other
problems derived from defining expertise based solely on the
amount of experience or work (Farrington-Darby and Wilson, 2006;
Shanteau et al., 2003).
Based on these considerations, the present study pursued the
following aims and hypotheses. First, considering aesthetic experience as the product of a complex amalgam of cognitive, evaluative
and motivational processes (possibly) rising during the free and
effortless viewing of paintings for pleasure (Kant’s “Wohlgefallen”),
we realised a free viewing condition without the addition of certain
cognitive, evaluative or motivationally relevant tasks and assured
participants attention for the presented stimuli by pre-announcing
questions about the stimuli at the end of the session. Second, we
compared individuals with varying degrees of art training and
experience with art with regard to their electro-cortical correlates
247
during the contemplation of art. Here, we predict lower electrocortical activity in those with greater art expertise as an expression
of increased neural efficiency due to practice. Third, in order to
tackle the specificity of neural responses to visual art, we employed
two types of control stimuli that differ from visual art either by
“dissolving” its constituting elements by filtering, or by realising
visual stimulation in one of its simplest manners as coloured plain
visual fields. While the original paintings contain all visual elements that contribute to their aesthetic value, with the filtering we
eliminated recognisable visual elements but preserved the overall form and colour palette. It has been shown that the processing
of stimuli in which the compositional structure is changed differs
from the processing of paintings with “intact” compositional structure (e.g., Locher et al., 1999; Vartanian and Goel, 2004). Plain colour
screens were used as a third experimental stimulus type, as plain
colour stimuli lack even the faintest information regarding form
and pattern. For this comparison we therefore predict a right-sided
predominance of electro-cortical activity and greater amplitudes
for paintings than for control stimuli (see also next point). Fourth,
referring to the aforementioned expertise-related EEG literature
as well as further studies using visual stimuli that are relevant
here (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Keil et al., 2002), we focus our analyses on the ERP components at posterior locations as our “region
of interest”. These ERP components can be expected to resemble
the P300 and the Late Positive Complex (LPC) and are thus relevant
for models of aesthetic experience that stress the importance of
memory processes (such as recognition and long-term memory) in
the appreciation of visual art (Leder et al., 2004). These processes
should be most pronounced in response to the original paintings
with their recognisable compositional elements and structure. In
addition to these theoretical rationales of focusing on posterior
LPC-like potentials, this topographical focus minimises the potential confounds of proper EEG activity with residual eye movement
artefacts due to volume conduction to negligible.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
On the basis of a recently developed art expertise questionnaire that focuses
of elements of formal art and musical education (see Appendix 1), N = 27 participants (mean age: 24.48 ± 5.60, range: 19–40 years; 17 females; 22 right-handed;
visual art expertise sum score: 27.13 ± 11.76, range: 9.0–48.75) were selected from
a larger screening sample with N = 346 undergraduate students. The questionnaire
consisted of 44 closed-ended questions, in which the first 23 were related to visual
art education and the remaining ones to education in music. Of these, questions
9, 17 and 24 were not included in the computation of the art expertise sum score
as these are nominal variables without numerical information. For the other 20
art-related questions, the answer scores were summed up. This questionnaire differs from similar measures (see Section 1) in covering more areas of art expertise
(formal art education, art preferences and specific interests) in greater detail (also
covering rather elaborated skills such as specific art analysis techniques to differentiate individuals in the upper expertise range). Questions about education in art
and the time contributed to visual art contemplation weighted the most. An expert
in the history of art (RR) and an expert in the study of art expertise (MN) have considered these items as content valid for the construct of art expertise. The internal
consistency of the 22 questions which contributed to the visual art expertise scores
was very high (Cronbach’s alpha = .906). Participants in both groups were native or
at least proficient English speakers (25 European, 1 Indian, 1 South-American). All
of the participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants gave
informed written consent before they participated in questionnaire screening and
EEG recording sessions.
2.2. Apparatus
The experiment took place in sound-attenuated and electrically shielded EEG
lab at Bangor University. BrainAmps DC amplifiers (sampling rate: 500 Hz, online
band pass: DC-250 Hz; resolution: 0.1 ␮V, Brain Products, UK) and EasyCaps (Falk
Minow Systems, Munich) were used to record the EEG from 64 positions of the international 10-10 system (American Electroencephalographic Society, 1991), including
F9 and F10 near the outer canthi for EOG measurement. The reference electrode and
ground electrode were placed at Cz and AF4, respectively. Electrode impedances
were kept below 5 k. The EEG was recorded on a Pentium 3 PC using Brain Vision
248
C.Y. Pang et al. / Biological Psychology 93 (2013) 246–254
Fig. 1. Examples of different genres of art (portrait, genre work, landscape and still life) are shown in their original (left) and filtered (right) versions.
Bronzino, “Untitled (portrait of Bia Medici)”. Available at http://zh.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Angelo Bronzino 037.jpg, de Hooch, “Mutter an der Wiege”. Available at
http://fr.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fichier:Pieter de Hooch 016.jpg, Corinth, “Ostern am Walchensee”. Available at http://it.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Lovis Corinth 007.jpg, Chardin,
“Der Wasserbehälter”. Available at http://bg.wikipedia.org/wiki/%D0%A4%D0%B0%D0%B9%D0%BB:Jean-Baptiste Sim%C3%A9on Chardin 009.jpg.
Recorder Version 1.04 (Brain Products, UK). The experimental stimuli were edited
by Adobe Photoshop CS3. They were presented with the “Presentation” software
(Neurobehavioral Systems, USA) on a 21 -TFT monitor in 1024 × 768 pixels resolution. BESA Version 5.1.6 by MEGIS Software was then used to perform ocular artefact
correction, EEG data segmentation, and all further EEG analysis. BPlot Version 1.4.0.9
(MEGIS Software) was used to generate EEG data visualisations and maps. Statistical
analysis was conducted using SPSS 13.0 for Windows.
sets. Each set consisted of 50 stimuli, and these 3 sets of stimuli were presented to
all participants in a single fixed order.
In order to minimise the confounding influence of differential familiarity with
the paintings in the two groups only relatively unknown paintings were used. Familiarity with the paintings in their original form was assessed at the end of the lab
session (see below).
2.4. Procedure
2.3. Stimuli
Fifty representational Western paintings of high professional standards in four
different categories (14 portraits, 11 landscapes, 18 genre works, and 7 still lives,
see Fig. 1) were presented under two stimulus types: in their original appearance
and filtered. The original stimuli were created by resizing paintings into maximum dimension which fitted in a 1024 × 768 pixels pure black background without
distorting the original dimension (Fig. 1). The filtered paintings were created by
applying the four filters: median noise, Gaussian blur, mosaic, or glass distortion
(Adobe Photoshop CS3). Depending on the features of the paintings, different filters
were applied until the structure and details of the paintings were dissolved but the
overall forms were retained. We did not apply the same filter to every painting in
order to avoid specific effect related to the filter. The plain colour stimulus type was
created by filling 50 different randomly selected colours on the 1024 × 768 pixels
background. Each stimulus type included 50 different stimuli, thus a total of 150
stimuli were used in the study. These 150 stimuli were randomly divided into 3
At the beginning of the session, the laboratory was shown to the participants and
the equipment and the different tasks were explained. If a participant agreed to take
part in the experiment, s/he gave her/his written consent and testing could begin.
The electrode cap was mounted, electrodes were attached and electrode impedances
were brought to below 5 k. Next, participants were prompted with arrows pointing
to the left, right, up or down as well as with a schematic picture of an eye to produce
horizontal and vertical saccades to visually-marked positions outside the monitor or
eye blinks (“calibration”). The proper (quasi-) experiment consisted of two separate
parts, the order of which was counterbalanced, namely presentation of (a) art stimuli
(35 min duration) and (b) music stimuli (35 min duration). Results of the music study
will be reported elsewhere.
During the visual art study, each stimulus was presented only once according
to the following procedure. At the beginning of each trial, a white fixation cross
appeared in the middle of the screen for 6.0 s, followed by an art stimulus that
was presented for 6.0 s. After stimulus offset, the fixation point appeared again and
C.Y. Pang et al. / Biological Psychology 93 (2013) 246–254
249
Fig. 2. Grand average ERP curves displaying the relationship between art expertise and ERP amplitudes at parietal leads and for the three experimental conditions separately.
The sample is artificially dichotomised in low-scorers versus high-scorers.
remained there for another 6.0 s before the next trial. Participants were asked to
carefully attend to the stimuli as they would be asked about them at the end of the
session. After the experiment, participants were shown the paintings used as stimuli
and asked to report if they had ever seen the paintings before the experiment. After
this task, participants were asked to fill in the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory
(Oldfield, 1971) and to indicate their age.
2.5. Primary and statistical data analysis
EEG data analysis was accomplished with BESA and BPlot (MEGIS software,
Munich). In the first data analysis step, the MSEC method (Berg and Scherg, 1994)
was applied to the “calibration” data. The MSEC method differs from traditional EOG
correction methods in that eye activity is described by components that are empirically determined separately for each individual. The components are treated like
dipole sources which model the EEG activity. While both eye and cortical activity
are assumed to be recorded by all applied channels, albeit to a different degrees, the
topography of eye versus cortical activity, as described by source vectors, differs. The
eye source vectors were determined empirically by applying a Principal Component
Analysis (PCA) on co-variances of all time points of the calibration data. While the
resulting source vector defines the topography of the eye activity, the source waveform describes the magnitude of the source over time. In order to correct the EEG
for eye movement artefacts, the source wave rather than the EOG signal is subtracted from each EEG channels in proportions which are defined by the respective
source component. Three types of eye movement were corrected: vertical and horizontal eye movements, and blinks. In addition, the source waveform identifies the
eye activity. By averaging these waveforms along with the EEG, an estimate of the
time-locked eye activity is obtained. The efficacy of the eye activity correction was
checked by visual inspection, and trials still containing artefacts were rejected.
A trial was defined as the interval starting 1000 ms before and ending 6000 ms
after stimulus onset, thus including a 1000 ms pre-stimulus baseline. After segmentation, the raw data were re-referenced to the common average reference
and 0.1–150.0 Hz zero phase band-pass filtered (both slopes set to 12 db/Octave).
Presentation stimulus triggers were used to differentiate the three experimental
stimulus types: original painting, modified painting, and plain colour. Accepted trials of the same stimulus type were averaged for each participant. Finally, grand
averages were then created for high-scorers and low-scorers from the individual
averages. Individual and average waveforms and topographical maps were visually
inspected to identify the ERP components following stimulus onset as well as their
latency ranges and topographies. The latency ranges were (a) 245–390 ms and (b)
500–525 ms. For the first component, peaks were found based on the P6 electrode
within the latency range 245–390 ms due to great variation among the latencies
of the peak for each participant. The mean amplitudes of the ERP during 25 ms
around the peak for each stimulus type were used for statistical analysis. Eight electrode channels (P3, P4, P5, P6, PO3, PO4, PO7 and PO8) covered the main activity
in these latency ranges and were for this reason used for statistical analysis. The
topographical maps are based on the BESA standard montage of 81 channels.
Repeated-measures ANCOVAs were conducted with the SAS “Proc GLM” with
Type III sums of squares using the amplitude of the 8 selected channels as dependent measures and “EXPERTISE” (the visual art expertise sum score, see above) as
predictor in the regression part of the ANCOVA as well as “LATENCY” (245–390 ms;
500–525 ms), “STIMULUS TYPE” (original painting; filtered painting; plain colour),
“HEMISPHERE” (left- versus right-sided electrodes), and “ELECTRODE” (P3/P4;
P5/P6; PO3/PO4; PO7/PO8) as the within-subject factors in the ANOVA part of the
ANCOVA. Although “EXPERTISE” has been considered as a continuous variable both
conceptually and in the ANCOVA, in order to display the relationship between art
expertise and ERP activity, the following two types of topographical maps were created. Firstly, as the dichotomising of an otherwise continuous variable, the entire
sample was split up into two groups (henceforth called “high-scorers” and “lowscorers” for the sake of brevity and to indicate that these participants were quite
highly experienced or little experienced, respectively) in order to show the proper
ERP topographies in those scoring high versus low in art expertise (see Figs. 2 and 3).
Secondly, for each ERP component and condition separately, we determined the correlation between the amplitude in each of the EEG channels and the art expertise
sum score. The topography of these correlations is shown in Fig. 4. After excluding
4 participants with poor data quality, data of 11 “high-scorers” (visual art expertise
sum score: 37.84 ± 6.15 (ranging from 29.00 to 48.75); 7 females, 9 right-handed,
mean age: 24.8 ± 6.0 years (19–39 years)) and 16 “low-scorers” (visual art expertise
sum score: 16.42 ± 3.90 (ranging from 9.00 to 22.25); 10 females, 13 right-handed,
mean age: 24.1 ± 5.5 years (19–40 years)) remained for these secondary analyses. The cut-off point for this dichotomisation was based on the expertise score
distribution of the larger screening sample, and the group difference in visual art
expertise was highly significant (t(15.495) = −10.230, p < .001). The number of paintings known to the participants was very low and did not differ between groups
(high-scorers: 1.8 ± 2.4; low-scorers: 2.1 ± 2.9; t(25) = 0.23, p = .82).
In addition to the proper test statistics (F-, t-values), significance levels and
Huynh–Feldt’s epsilon values are reported. Unless stated otherwise, the ANCOVA
results of the non-normalised data remained significant after data normalisation.
3. Results
Fig. 2 shows the temporal course for those eight electrodes
that were used for statistical analyses in art high-scorers and lowscorers for the three experimental stimulus types. As can be seen
in Fig. 2 (grand average curves) and Fig. 3 (grand average maps),
the experimental stimuli elicited ERP components that resemble
in terms of latency and topography the Late Positive Complex
(LPC) or P3b that is typically seen after the attentive processing
of visual stimuli. Plain colours elicited smaller amplitudes than
filtered paintings, which in turn elicited smaller amplitudes than
the original paintings. It can be seen in Figs. 2 and 3 that both the
250
C.Y. Pang et al. / Biological Psychology 93 (2013) 246–254
Fig. 3. Grand average ERP maps displaying the relationship between art expertise and ERP topographies as well as differences in ERP topographies for the three experimental
conditions and the two analysed components. The sample is artificially dichotomised in low-scorers versus high-scorers.
positive deflections and the differences between the three stimulus types in these deflections exhibited topographical maxima
at posterior positions. While Figs. 2 and 3 suggest a negative correlation between art expertise and ERP amplitudes, this negative
relationship is directly shown in Fig. 4. This figure reveals that for
the first ERP component, greater art expertise is associated with
lower ERP amplitudes at right-posterior leads. For the second ERP
component and the original and filtered paintings, the negative
ERP-expertise correlation is more symmetrical over the two
hemispheres.
C.Y. Pang et al. / Biological Psychology 93 (2013) 246–254
251
Fig. 4. Correlation maps showing the relationship between ERP amplitudes and the continuous art expertise sum score for the two components and the three conditions
separately. Note that these maps do not show the ERPs as such. Red colour represents negative ERP-expertise correlations, green colour positive ERP-expertise correlations.
The scale on the right side displays the size of the correlations. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version
of the article.)
The statistical analyses of the first ERP component revealed a significant STIMULUS TYPE × HEMISPHERE × ELECTRODE interaction
(F3.767,94.179 = 3.858, p = .007, ε = .628, eta2 = .134) that can be further
specified by its subordinate interaction and main effects (STIMULUS TYPE × HEMISPHERE: F1.894,47.355 = 7.113, p = .002, ε = .947,
eta2 = .222; STIMULUS TYPE × ELECTRODE: F4.283,107.069 = 4.140,
p = .003, ε = .714, eta2 = .142; STIMULUS TYPE: F1.698,42.444 = 11.923,
p < .001, ε = .849, eta2 = .323; HEMISPHERE: F1.000,25.000 = 13.100,
p = .001, ε = 1.000, eta2 = .344; ELECTRODE: F2.525,63.133 = 7.081,
p = .001, ε = .842, eta2 = .221). Together, these effects point to a
topographical distribution that favoured the right hemisphere as
well as lateral parietal and mesial parieto-occipital leads and
showed an amplitude reduction from original paintings over filtered paintings to plain colours at all positions (see Table 1). While
this interaction remained significant after data normalisation, the
underlying interaction of STIMULUS TYPE with HEMISPHERE just
failed conventional significance level (p = .059).
The ANCOVA revealed furthermore a significant association
between EXPERTISE SCORE and the amplitudes of the first
ERP component, in particular at bilateral parieto-occipital
electrodes (see Fig. 4) (EXPERTISE SCORE × ELECTRODE:
F2.525,63.133 = 4.396, p = .010, eta2 = .150; EXPERTISE SCORE:
F1,25 = 7.723, p = .010, eta2 = .236; ELECTRODE: F2.525,63.133 = 7.081,
p = .001, eta2 = .221) and over the right hemisphere (EXPERTISE
SCORE × HEMISPHERE: F1.000,25.000 = 7.427, p = .012, eta2 = .229;
EXPERTISE SCORE: F1,25 = 7.723, p = .010, eta2 = .236; HEMISPHERE:
F1.000,25.000 = 13.100, p = .001, eta2 = .344). Greater art expertise was
thus associated with lower electro-cortical amplitude at parietal
leads (P3/P4: r = −.365, p = .061; P5/P6: r = −.416, p = .031), parietooccipital leads (PO3/PO4: r = −.504, p = .007; PO7/PO8: r = −.541,
p = .004) and more so over the right hemisphere (r = −.546, p = .003)
than over the left hemisphere (r = −.277, p = .162). The interaction
of STIMULUS TYPE and EXPERTISE SCORE was not significant for
the first component.
Table 1
Paired t-tests results for the comparison of different stimulus types.
Mean amplitude ± SD (␮V)
Original
Left
Right
P3
P4
P5
P6
PO3
PO4
PO7
PO8
*
2.72
4.81
2.38
4.64
2.07
4.76
3.87
5.69
2.56
4.14
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
2.8
4.6
2.8
4.3
2.9
4.8
3.5
5.3
2.9
4.7
Filtered
1.54
2.86
1.39
2.69
1.22
3.35
2.16
3.13
1.38
2.27
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
Original – filtered
Blank colour
3.0
4.3
2.9
3.8
2.9
4.7
3.6
4.8
3.2
4.8
0.10
0.53
0.22
0.96
-0.38
-0.29
0.78
1.58
-0.23
-0.14
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
±
1.8
2.4
1.6
2.0
2.1
2.5
1.9
3.2
2.1
2.5
Original – blank colour
Filtered – blank colour
t
p
Cohen’s d
t
p
Cohen’s d
t
p
Cohen’s d
3.614*
5.359*
2.905*
5.787*
2.556*
3.270*
4.759*
7.288*
3.058*
3.641*
.001
<.001
.007
<.001
.017
.003
<.001
<.001
.005
.001
.700
1.039
.570
1.154
.494
.630
.927
1.463
.587
.698
5.403*
6.660*
4.005*
5.577*
4.752*
6.604*
5.599*
6.343*
5.963*
6.634*
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
1.097
1.603
.815
1.349
.934
1.492
1.258
1.521
1.196
1.621
3.049*
3.863*
2.177*
3.011*
3.076*
4.974*
2.704*
2.634*
3.478*
3.489*
.005
.001
.039
.006
.005
<.001
.012
.014
.002
.002
.653
.905
.464
.708
.617
1.103
.657
.587
.723
.823
Significance determined according to Holm (1979); df = 26; latency range 245–390 ms.
252
C.Y. Pang et al. / Biological Psychology 93 (2013) 246–254
Like the first component, the second component was more
pronounced over the right hemisphere as compared to the left
(HEMISPHERE: F1.000,25.000 = 18.738, p < .001, ε = 1.000, eta2 = .428)
and larger for original paintings as opposed to filtered paintings
or plain colours (STIMULUS TYPE: F1.787,44.678 = 27.627, p < .001,
ε = .894, eta2 = .525). While the effect of stimulus type was somewhat differently pronounced for the different bilateral electrode
pairs (STIMULUS TYPE × ELECTRODE: F5.087,127.172 = 2.947, p = .014,
ε = .848, eta2 = .105), large effects were found for all of them (Fvalues ranging between 21.7 at P3/P4 and 28.4 at PO7/PO8). Unlike
the first component, the hemisphere asymmetry of the second component did not interact with the type of stimulus being presented
or with art expertise. Nevertheless, also for this component, there
was a general negative relationship between art expertise and ERP
amplitude (EXPERTISE SCORE: F1,25 = 8.331, p = .008, eta2 = .250;
see also Fig. 4). The interaction of STIMULUS TYPE and EXPERTISE SCORE was not significant for the second component (p = .059,
eta2 = .111).
4. Discussion
The aim of the present study was to investigate the relationship
between art expertise, as measured through a newly developed
questionnaire, and the electro-cortical correlates of processing
paintings. To that end, the participants of our study freely contemplated unfamiliar paintings of different genres in their original
or filtered versions as well as plain colours. This study revealed
the following main results. First, under free viewing conditions
the stimuli used here elicited a P3b-like posterior positive deflection. Second, processing original paintings elicited greater posterior
ERP amplitudes than processing filtered paintings or plain colours.
Third, the electro-cortical correlates of art processing were more
pronounced over the right than over the left hemisphere. Fourth,
greater art expertise was associated with smaller ERP amplitudes
at parieto-occipital sites and over the right hemisphere during an
“early” processing stage (around 300 ms) and more broadly posterior at a “late” processing stage (around 500 ms). Fifth, this negative
expertise-amplitude relationship generalised to filtered paintings
and even plain colour fields.
Ad (1). The P3b and the Late Positive Complex (LPC; Friedman
et al., 1978) that often follows it belong to the most intensively and
extensively researched ERP components, and it is beyond the scope
of this article to review this literature. Suffice to say, that the P3b
has been associated with the “context updating” or contextual “closure” in working memory (e.g., Donchin and Coles, 1988; Verleger,
1988) and has been shown to index the retrieval of knowledge from
long-term memory (Koester and Prinz, 2007). As outlined in Section 1, current models of art appreciation have emphasised the
role of knowledge in long-term memory (Leder et al., 2004). The
mere presence of this component, thus suggests that the participants of our study were attentively processing the stimuli they
were exposed to during our free-viewing condition.
Ad (2). The P3b/LPC waveform exhibited the largest amplitudes
for paintings and the smallest for plain colours. Various previous
studies have shown that degradation of visual stimuli cause reductions in the P3b amplitude (reviewed in Kok, 2001). These findings
are in line with the positive relationship between information
transmission and P3b amplitude as specified in Johnson’s (1986)
Triarchic Model of the P3b. Applying filtering techniques intended
to degrade the painting stimuli by dissolving their constituent elements, a technique that was put to an extreme by removing these
elements altogether in the plain colour stimuli. The posterior positive deflection reported in the present paper sensitively reflected
these experimental manipulations. The original painting stimuli,
however, were not only the least degraded ones; with their specific
content, that is visual art, they conveyed a kind of information the
processing in working memory of which can be assumed to elicit
emotional reward and arousal to some extent (Chatterjee, 2004;
Leder et al., 2004). The present results therefore not only confirm
results of a study that used stimuli similar to the ones used here
(Vartanian and Goel, 2004), but also results of studies that used
different kinds of emotionally relevant stimuli (e.g., Cuthbert et al.,
2000; Keil et al., 2002).
Ad (3). The posterior ERP reported here were not only sensitive to the experimental task manipulations, they also exhibited
a clear right-hemispheric preponderance that is in line with previous publications on aesthetic judgement (Höfel and Jacobsen,
2007; Jacobsen and Höfel, 2003) or the processing of metaphors
and emotional memories (Bhattacharya and Petsche, 2002). One
of the important implications of the posterior, right-sided predominance of the ERP components for paintings, their degraded
(filtered) versions and even plain colour stimuli is that the underlying electro-cortical generators are not unique to the processing of
visual art. While this conclusion may read peculiar at first glance, it
must be stated that more than a decade of neuroimaging of visual
art and aesthetic appreciation has yielded no evidence that evolution has produced any anatomic “module” or neurophysiological
process that is unique to the processing of visual art (Chatterjee,
2004, 2011; Skov, 2010; Zaidel, 2005). Our findings here thus
support the notion that the appreciation of visual art relies on
cognitive and neural processes involved in processing other kinds
of visual stimuli (Aglioti et al., 2012; Calvo-Merino et al., 2005,
2008, 2010a,b; Harvey et al., 2010; Kirk, 2008; Kirk et al., 2009a,b,
2011; Salimpoor et al., 2011; Skov, 2010). This finding is important
because it argues against the popular belief that because “art is special” there should be “some special brain process” associated with
art or aesthetics.
Ad (4). The greater the art expertise, the smaller was the electrocortical activation by the experimental stimuli. This fourth major
finding is in agreement with decreases in the extent and intensity
of brain activation related with practice and experience observed
in many other domains (Kelly and Garavan, 2005) and was also
reported in an fMRI study of drawing portraits (Solso, 2001) and
an EEG study of preparation and execution of the right movements
(Percio et al., 2010). In all of these studies, experts showed lower
degrees of activation when performing tasks they are specialised in.
This apparently well replicable observation can be subsumed
under the broader topic of “neural efficiency” due to intensive
practice (see Haier et al., 1988). As Kelly and Garavan (2005) have
elaborated in their review, task practice leads to decreases in extent
or intensity of brain activation through increases in neural efficiency, defined as a sharpening of neural responses in task-relevant
processing networks (see also Percio et al., 2010; Petersen et al.,
1988; Poldrack, 2000).
Our results thus add to the research on the effects of expertise on the cognitive processes involved in the appreciation of art
(Augustin and Leder, 2006; Kay, 1991, 2000; Kozbelt, 2001; Kozbelt
and Seeley, 2007; Locher et al., 2008; Nodine and Krupinski, 1998;
Pihko et al., 2011; Silvia, 2007; Vogt and Magnussen, 2007) and
their neural underpinnings (Altenmüller et al., 2000; Berkowitz
and Ansari, 2010; Bhattacharya and Petsche, 2005; Brattico et al.,
2008; Calvo-Merino et al., 2005, 2010b; Cross et al., 2006; Kirk et al.,
2009b, 2011; Orgs et al., 2008; Tervaniemi, 2009). Understanding
the effects of expertise on behaviour and its neural foundations
is not only important for research on learning, training, and skill
development. In the domain of art appreciation, moreover, it constitutes a key argument against the notion of the stability and
objectivity of the art appreciation, and a fundamental tool in
understanding how the art experience is generated. The studies
quoted here have shown that such experience is modulated by contextual cues, by the information provided, and by the development
of expertise.
C.Y. Pang et al. / Biological Psychology 93 (2013) 246–254
Despite the plausibility of the interpretation given before, a
potential caveat of all expertise studies is the quasi-experimental
nature of the comparison between high-scorers and low-scorers,
which opens the possibility of confounds between expert status and
other known or unknown variables (such as differences in recognition of artistic style, age of production of the artwork, etc.; Augustin
et al., 2011) associated with it that may be relevant for the processes under scrutiny. (The same applies to correlative studies like
the present one, in which art expertise is investigated as a continuous variable.) Therefore, the present results are preliminary in a
specific sense: They do show that art expertise is negatively correlated with the amplitude of the electro-cortical potentials that
accompany “higher-order” art processing (cognition, evaluation,
affect rather than low-level perception), but they do not show why
this is the case. Future studies that put less emphasis on the relative ecological validity of “non-instructed” free viewing conditions,
may be guided by current models of aesthetic experience such as
Leder et al. (2004) model in designing experimental task modifications that have more specific bearings for processing components
specified in this model.
Ad (5). That the negative relationship between art expertise and
ERP amplitude held not only for paintings, but also for their filtered
versions and even plain colour fields, is another finding that reads
counter-intuitive only at first glance. Indeed, although expertise
is by definition domain-specific, experts in certain domains have
in various studies been shown to excel non-experts in tasks
that are not related to their domain of expertise. Chi (1978), for
instance, found that young chess experts outperformed children
non-experienced with chess in memory and learning tasks through
better use of grouping and rehearsal strategies. Similar effects
have been found repeatedly in relation to musicians’ performance
in general attention (Posner et al., 2008), the recognizing of emotions in speech prosody (Lima and Castro, 2011), spatial abilities
(Rauscher et al., 1993; Rauscher and Zupan, 2000), or verbal memory (Chan et al., 1998; Franklin et al., 2008; Ho et al., 2003; Jakobson
et al., 2003) as well as visual artists’ performance in detecting
and resolving figural problems (Kay, 1991, 2000) or in general
perception tests (Kozbelt, 2001; Kozbelt and Seeley, 2007). These
studies can be conjointly summarised by hypothesising that art
experts “export” their art viewing strategies to non-artistic stimuli
such that relationships between art expertise and the processing
of artistic stimuli are mirrored in the processing of control stimuli.
To summarise, the present study contributed to the existing art expertise literature in demonstrating that art expertise
is associated with reduced “higher-order” event-related potential
amplitudes under free viewing conditions of visual art and nonartistic visual stimuli that can be considered to reflect increased
neural efficiency due to extensive practice.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by a research grant of the Deutsche
Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) given to Raphael Rosenberg and
Christoph Klein (RO 2281/3-1). The authors would like to thank
Dr. Christopher Saville for his help with the figures.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found,
in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.
2012.10.013.
References
Aglioti, S.M., Minio-Paluello, I., Candidi, M., 2012. The beauty of the body. Rendiconti Lincei, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12210-12012-10169-12211, Published
online ahead of print.
253
Altenmüller, E., Gruhn, W., Parlitz, D., Liebert, G., 2000. The impact of music education on brain networks: evidence from EEG-studies. International Journal of
Music Education 35, 47–53.
American Electroencephalographic Society, 1991. American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. Journal
of Clinical Neurophysiology 8, 200–202.
Augustin, M.D., Leder, H., 2006. Art expertise: a study of concepts and conceptual
spaces. Psychology Science 48, 135–156.
Augustin, M.D., Defranceschi, B., Fuchs, H.K., Carbon, C., Hutzler, F., 2011. The neural
time course of art perception: an ERP study on the processing of style versus
content in art. Neuropsychologia 49, 2071–2081.
Averill, J.R., Stanat, P., More, T.A., 1998. Aesthetics and the environment. Review of
General Psychology 2 (2), 153–174.
Berg, P., Scherg, M., 1994. A multiple source approach to the correction of eye artefacts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 90 (3), 229–241.
Berkowitz, A.L., Ansari, D., 2010. Expertise-related deactivation of the right temporoparietal juction during musical improvisation. Neuroimage 49, 712–719.
Bhattacharya, J., Petsche, H., 2002. Shadows of artistry: cortical synchrony during
perception and imagery of visual art. Cognitive Brain Research 13, 179–186.
Bhattacharya, J., Petsche, H., 2005. Drawing on mind’s canvas: differences in cortical
integration patterns between artists and non-artists. Human Brain Mapping 26,
1–14.
Blood, A.J., Zatorre, R.J., 2001. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate
with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98,
11818–11823.
Brattico, E., Pallesen, K.J., Varyagina, O., Bailey, C., Anourova, I., Järvenpää, M., Eerola,
T., Tervaniemi, M., 2008. Neural discrimination of nonprototypical chords in
music experts and laymen: an MEG study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
21, 2230–2244.
Brown, S., Martinez, M.J., Parsons, L.M., 2004. Passive music listening spontaneously
engages limbic and paralimbic systems. Neuroreport 15, 2033–2037.
Brown, S., Martinez, M.J., Parsons, L.M., 2006. The neural basis of human dance.
Cerebral Cortex 16, 1157–1167.
Bukach, C.M., Gauthier, I., Tarr, M.J., 2006. Beyond faces and modularity: the power
of an expertise framework. Trends in Cognitive Science 10, 159–166.
Calvo-Merino, B., Ehrenberg, S., Leung, D., Haggard, P., 2010a. Experts see it all:
configural effects in action observation. Psychological Research 74, 400–406.
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D.E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R.E., Haggard, P., 2005. Action
observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex 15, 1243–1249.
Calvo-Merino, B., Jola, C., Glaser, D.E., Haggard, P., 2008. Towards a sensorimotor
aesthetics of performing art. Consciousness and Cognition 17, 911–922.
Calvo-Merino, B., Urgesi, C., Orgs, G., Aglioti, S.M., Haggard, P., 2010b. Extrastriate
body area underlies aesthetic evaluation of body stimuli. Experimental Brain
Research 204, 447–456.
Cela-Conde, C.J., Ayala, F.J., Munar, E., Maestú, F., Nadal, M., Capó, M.A., 2009.
Sex-related similarities and differences in the neural correlates of beauty.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
106, 3847–3852.
Cela-Conde, C.J., Marty, G., Maestú, F., Ortiz, T., Munar, E., Fernández, A., 2004.
Activation of the prefrontal cortex in the human visual aesthetic perception.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
101, 6321–6325.
Chan, A.S., Ho, Y.-C., Cheung, M.-C., 1998. Music training improves verbal memory.
Nature 396, 128.
Chatterjee, A., Widick, P., Sternschein, R., Smith, I.I., Bromberger, W.B.B., 2010. The
assessment of art attributes. Empirical Studies of the Arts 28, 207–222.
Chatterjee, A., 2004. Prospects for a cognitive neuroscience of visual aesthetics.
Bulletin of Psychology of the Arts 4, 55–60.
Chatterjee, A., 2011. Neuroaesthetics: a coming of age story. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience 23, 53–62.
Chi, M.T.H., 1978. Knowledge structures and memory development. In: Siegler, R.
(Ed.), Children’s Thinking: What Develops? Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 73–96.
Cross, E.S., Hamilton, A.F.d., Grafton, C.S.T., 2006. Building a motor simulation de
novo: observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage 31, 1257–1267.
Cupchik, G.C., Vartanian, O., Crawley, A., Mikulis, D.J., 2009. Viewing artworks:
contributions of cognitive control and perceptual facilitation to aesthetic experience. Brain and Cognition 70, 84–91.
Cuthbert, B.N., Schupp, H.T., Bradley, M.M., Birbaumer, N., Lang, P.J., 2000. Brain
potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal
and affective report. Biological Psychology 52, 95–111.
Donchin, E., Coles, M.G.H., 1988. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context
updating. Behavioural and Brain Science 11, 357–374.
Farrington-Darby, T., Wilson, J.R., 2006. The nature of expertise: a review. Applied
Ergonomics 37, 17–32.
Fechner, G.T., 1876. Vorschule der Ästhetik. Breitkopf und Härtel, Leipzig.
Franklin, M.S., Rattray, K., Moore, K.S., Moher, J., Yip, C.Y., Jonides, J., 2008. The effects
of musical training on verbal memory. Psychology of Music 36 (3), 353–365.
Friedman, D., Vaughan, H.G., Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., 1978. Stimulus and response
related components of the late positive complex in visual discrimination tasks.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 45, 319–330.
Gauthier, I., Tarr, M.J., Bub, D. (Eds.), 2010. Perceptual Expertise: Bridging Brain and
Behavior. Oxford University Press, New York.
Haier, R.J., Siegel Jr., B.V., Nuechterlein, K.H., Hazlett, E., Wu, J.C., Paek, J., Browning, H.L., Buchsbaum, M.S., 1988. Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of
254
C.Y. Pang et al. / Biological Psychology 93 (2013) 246–254
abstract reasoning and attention studied with positron emission tomography.
Intelligence 12, 199–217.
Harvey, A.H., Kirk, U., Denfield, G.H., Montague, P.R., 2010. Monetary favors and
their influence on neural responses and revealed preference. The Journal of
Neuroscience 30, 9597–9602.
Hekkert, P., van Wieringen, P.C.W., 1996. Beauty in the eye of expert and nonexpert
beholders: a study in the appraisal of art. American Journal of Psychology 109
(3), 389–407.
Ho, Y.-C., Cheung, M.-C., Chan, A.S., 2003. Music training improves verbal but not
visual memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuropsychology 17, 439–450.
Holm, S., 1979. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian
Journal of Statistics 6 (2), 65–70.
Höfel, L., Jacobsen, T., 2007. Electrophysiological indices of processing aesthetics:
spontaneous or intentional processes? International Journal of Psychophysiology 65, 20–31.
Ishai, A., Fairhall, S.L., Pepperell, R., 2007. Perception, memory and aesthetics of
indeterminate art. Brain Research Bulletin 73, 319–324.
Jacobsen, T., Höfel, L., 2002. Aesthetic judgments of novel graphic patterns: analyses
of individual judgments. Perceptual and Motor Skills 95, 755–766.
Jacobsen, T., Höfel, L., 2003. Descriptive and evaluative judgment processes: behavioral and electrophysiological indices of processing symmetry and aesthetics.
Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 3 (4), 289–299.
Jacobsen, T., Schubotz, R.I., Höfel, L., von Cramon, D.Y., 2006. Brain correlates of
aesthetic judgment of beauty. NeuroImage 29, 276–285.
Jakobson, L.S., Cuddy, L.L., Kilgour, A.R., 2003. Time tagging: a key to musicians’
superior memory. Music Perception 20, 307–313.
Johnson, R., 1986. A triarchic model of the P300 amplitude. Psychophysiology 23,
367–383.
Kawabata, H., Zeki, S., 2004. Neural correlates of beauty. Journal of Neuropsychology
91, 1699–1705.
Kay, S.I., 1991. The figural problem solving and problem finding of professional
and semiprofessional artists and nonartists. Creativity Research Journal 4 (3),
233–252.
Kay, S.I., 2000. On the nature of expertise in visual art. In: Friedman, R.C., Shore, B.M.
(Eds.), Talents Unfolding: Cognition and Development. American Psychological
Association, Washington, DC, pp. 217–232.
Keil, A., Bradley, M.M., Hauk, O., Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., Lang, P.J., 2002. Large-scale
neural correlates of affective picture processing. Psychophysiology 39, 641–649.
Kelly, A.M.C., Garavan, H., 2005. Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes
associated with practice. Cerebral Cortex 15, 1089–1102.
Kirk, U., 2008. The neural basis of object-context relationships on aesthetic judgment. PLoS One 3 (11), e3754, doi:3710.1371/journal.pone.0003754.
Kirk, U., Harvey, A., Montague, P.R., 2011. Domain expertise insulates against judgment bias by monetary favors through a modulation of ventromedial prefrontal
cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America 108, 10332–10336.
Kirk, U., Skov, M., Christensen, M.S., Nygaard, N., 2009a. Brain correlates of aesthetic
expertise: a parametric fMRI study. Brain and Cognition 69, 306–315.
Kirk, U., Skov, M., Hulme, O., Christensen, M.S., Zeki, S., 2009b. Modulation of aesthetic value by semantic context: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 44, 1125–1132.
Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., von Cramon, D.Y., Müller, K., Friederici, A.D., 2006. Investigating
emotion with music: an fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping 27, 239–250.
Koester, D., Prinz, W., 2007. Capturing regularities in event sequences: evidence for
two mechanisms. Brain Research 1180, 59–77.
Kok, A., 2001. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity.
Psychophysiology 38, 557–577.
Kozbelt, A., 2001. Artists as experts in visual cognition. Visual Cognition 8, 705–723.
Kozbelt, A., Seeley, W.P., 2007. Integrating art historical, psychological, and neuroscientific explanations of artists’ advantages in drawing and perception.
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 1, 80–90.
Kristjanson, A.F., Antes, J.R., 1989. Eye movement analysis of artists and nonartists
viewing paintings. Visual Arts Research 15, 21–30.
Lacey, S., Hagtvedt, H., Patrick, V.M., Anderson, A., Silla, R., Deshpande, G., 2011.
Art for reward’s sake: visual art recruits the ventral striatum. Neuroimage 55,
420–433.
Landau, M.J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Martens, A., 2006. Windows
into nothingness: terror management, meaninglessness, and negative reactions
to modern art. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90 (6), 879–892.
Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., Augustin, D., 2004. A model of aesthetic appreciation
and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology 95, 489–508.
Lima, C.F., Castro, S.L., 2011. Speaking to the trained ear: musical expertise enhances
the recognition of emotions in speech prosody. Emotion 11, 1021–2031.
Locher, P.J., Stappers, P.J., Overbeeke, K., 1999. An empirical evaluation of the visual
rightness theory of pictorial composition. Acta Psychologica 103, 261–280.
Locher, P., Frens, J., Overbeeke, K., 2008. The influence of induced positive affect and
design experience on aesthetic responses to new product designs. Psychology
of Aesthetics Creativity, and the Arts 2, 1–7.
McWhinnie, H.J., 1968. A review of research on aesthetic measure. Acta Psychologica
28, 363–375.
Münte, T.F., Altenmüller, E., Jäncke, L., 2002. The musician’s brain as a model of
neuroplasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3, 473–478.
Nadal, M., Pearce, M.T., 2011. The Copenhagen Neuroaesthetics conference:
prospects and pitfalls for an emerging field. Brain and Cognition 76, 172–183.
Nodine, C.F., Krupinski, E., 1998. Perceptual skill, radiology expertise, and visual test
performance with NINA and WALDO. Academic Radiology 5, 603–612.
Nodine, C.F., Locher, P.J., Krupinski, E.A., 1993. The role of formal art training
on perception and aesthetic judgement of art compositions. Leonardo 26 (3),
219–227.
Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh
inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113.
Orgs, G., Dombrowski, J.-H., Heil, M., Jansen-Osmann, P., 2008. Expertise in dance
modulates alpha/beta event-related desynchronization during action observation. European Journal of Neuroscience 27, 3380–3384.
Percio, C.D., Infarinato, F., Iacoboni, M., Marzano, N., Soricelli, A., Aschieri, P., Eusebi,
F., Babiloni, C., 2010. Movement-related desynchronization of alpha rhythms
is lower in athletes than non-athletes: a high-resolution EEG study. Clinical
Neurophysiology 121, 482–491.
Petersen, S.E., van Mier, H., Fiez, J.A., Raichle, M.E., 1988. The effects of practice on the
functional anatomy of task performance. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, USA 95, 853–860.
Pihko, E., Virtanen, A., Saarinen, V.-M., Pannasch, S., Hirvenkari, L., Tossavainen,
T., Haapala, A., Hari, R., 2011. Experiencing art: the influence of expertise and painting abstraction level. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5,
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00094.
Poldrack, R.A., 2000. Imaging brain plasticity: conceptual and methodological issues.
Neuroimage 12, 1–13.
Posner, M., Rothbart, M.K., Sheese, B.E., Kieras, J., 2008. How arts training influences cognition. In: Asbury, C., Rich, B. (Eds.), Learning, Arts, and the Brain:
The Dana Consortium Report on Arts and Cognition. The Dana Foundation,
NY/Washington, DC, pp. 1–10.
Ramachandran, V.S., Hirstein, W., 1999. The science of art: a neurological theory of
aesthetic experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6 (6/7), 15–51.
Rauscher, F.H., Shaw, G.L., Ky, K.N., 1993. Music and spatial task performance. Nature
365, 611.
Rauscher, F.H., Zupan, M.A., 2000. Classroom keyboard instruction improves kindergarten children’s spatial-temporal performance: a field experiment. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly 15, 215–228.
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., Winkielman, P., 2004. Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social
Psychology Review 8 (4), 364–382.
Rossion, B., Collins, D., Goffaux, V., Curran, T., 2007. Long-term visual expertise with
artificial objects increases visual competition with early face categorization processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19, 543–555.
Rossion, B., Kung, C.C., Tarr, M.J., 2004. Visual expertise with nonface objects leads to
competition with the early perceptual processing of faces in the human occipitotemporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America 101, 14521–14526.
Salimpoor, V.N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., Zatorre, R.J., 2011. Anatomically
distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion
to music. Nature Neuroscience 14, 257–262.
Schmidt, J.A., McLaughlin, J.P., Leighten, P., 1989. Novice strategies for understanding
paintings. Applied Cognitive Psychology 3, 65–72.
Scott, L.S., Tanaka, J.W., Sheinberg, D.L., Curran, T., 2008. The role of category learning in the acquisition and retention of perceptual expertise: a behavioral and
neurophysiological study. Brain Research 1210, 204–215.
Shanteau, J., Weiss, D.J., Thomas, R.P., Punds, J., 2003. How can you tell if someone
is an expert? Empirical assessment of expertise. In: Schneider, S., Shanteau, J.
(Eds.), Emerging Perspectives on Judgment and Decision Making. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 620–641.
Silvia, P.J., 2005. Emotional responses to art: from collation and arousal to cognition
and emotion. Review of General Psychology 9 (4), 342–357.
Silvia, P.J., 2007. Knowledge-based assessment of expertise in the arts: exploring aesthetic fluency. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 1,
247–249.
Skov, M., 2010. The pleasure of art. In: Kringelbach, M.L., Berridge, K.C. (Eds.), Pleasures of the Brain. Oxford University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 270–283.
Smith, L.F., Smith, J.K., 2006. The nature and growth of aesthetic fluency. In: Martindale, C., Locher, P., Petrov, V. (Eds.), New Directions in Aesthetics, Creativity,
and the Arts. Baywood, Amityville, NY.
Solso, R.L., 2001. Brain activities in a skilled versus a novice artist: an fMRI study.
Leonardo 34 (1), 31–34.
Tanaka, J.W., Curran, T., 2001. A neural basis for expert object recognition. Psychological Science 12, 43–47.
Tervaniemi, M., 2009. Musicians—same or different? Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences 1169, 151–156.
Vartanian, O., Goel, V., 2004. Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference for
paintings. NeuroReport 15 (5), 893–897.
Verleger, R., 1988. Event-related potentials and cognition: a critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences 11, 343–427.
Vogt, S., Magnussen, S., 2007. Expertise in pictorial perception: eye-movement patterns and visual memory in artists and laymen. Perception 36, 91–100.
Volke, H.J., Dettmar, P., Richter, M., Rudolf, M., Buhss, U., 2002. On-coupling and
off-coupling of neocortical areas in chess experts and novices. Journal of Psychophysiology 16 (1), 23–36.
Zaidel, D.W., 2005. Neuropsychology of Art: Neurological, Cognitive, and Evolutionary Perspectives. Psychology Press, Hove, England.