* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Uto-Aztecan *na
Sanskrit grammar wikipedia , lookup
Polish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Ancient Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup
Malay grammar wikipedia , lookup
Udmurt grammar wikipedia , lookup
Japanese grammar wikipedia , lookup
Spanish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Latin syntax wikipedia , lookup
Kannada grammar wikipedia , lookup
Yiddish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Lithuanian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Esperanto grammar wikipedia , lookup
Zulu grammar wikipedia , lookup
Navajo grammar wikipedia , lookup
Scottish Gaelic grammar wikipedia , lookup
Germanic strong verb wikipedia , lookup
Old Irish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Proto-Indo-European verbs wikipedia , lookup
Ojibwe grammar wikipedia , lookup
Old Norse morphology wikipedia , lookup
Swedish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Ukrainian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Germanic weak verb wikipedia , lookup
Modern Hebrew grammar wikipedia , lookup
Old English grammar wikipedia , lookup
Lexical semantics wikipedia , lookup
Serbo-Croatian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Turkish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Georgian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Hungarian verbs wikipedia , lookup
Uto-Aztecan *na-Class Verbs Author(s): Jeffrey Heath Source: International Journal of American Linguistics, Vol. 44, No. 3 (Jul., 1978), pp. 211-222 Published by: The University of Chicago Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1264945 . Accessed: 09/10/2013 22:48 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to International Journal of American Linguistics. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions UTO-AZTECAN *NA-CLASS VERBS JEFFREYHEATH HARVARDUNIVERSITY Lexical turnover in this class has been Introduction PNUA thematic suffixes extremely high. We can reconstruct for PUA only *tapa- to split, while for PNUA The PNUA intransitive paradigm we can additionally reconstruct *k"iti- to The distributive The iterative get up (Ca -cap-i- and Az -Aapa-na-; SoPa kwiti-'ki- and Lu kw6:t-ax-). However, Perfective truncation classes reflectingthe morphology of the old The PSUA intransitive paradigm *na-class agree in including verbs describThe element *-kaThe PSUA transitive (causative) para- ing simple physical events, especially those digm leaving an imprint (break, twist, smash, transitive The PNUA 2.2. (causative) split, bend, hit, etc.), even though the actual items are generally not cognate from paradigm one language to another. 2.3. Some SoPa forms The PUA *na-class is continued by the 2.4. SoPa i-n?a- and i-naSoPa -'ki-/1-na-class, the Mo -?i-/-hi- class, 2.5. The iterative the Mo color-verb class, the Lu -ax-/-i2.6. The element *-yV2.7. Instrumental prefixes class, the Cu -yox-/-in- class, the Ca -i-/-in3. PNUA and PSUA participles class, the Se q-class, the Ho k-class, the Pa 4. Implications for UA subgrouping -n/-S class and equivalents in other Tepiman languages, the Ta -na-/-ca- class, and 0. In this article, I reconstruct the main the Az -ni-/-na- class. Paradigms, in some morphological features of the *na-class for cases slightly simplified, are presented in PUA and for the intermediate proto- tables 1-11.2 Two recurrent problems in reconstruclanguages PNUA and PSUA. This class was dominated by bisyllabic roots and tion will be mentioned here. First, it is not was characterized by rich thematic varia- always easy to distinguish stative intransition, each theme consisting of a stem-grade tive it is bent from nominalized a bend or like hardened *CV'CV- or reduplicated mediopassive participle bent, since the *CV-CVCV- plus one or more special latter two can be used as predicates in thematic suffixes (the most widespread being *-na- or *i-na-).1 0. 1.1. 1.2. 1.3. 1.4. 1.5. 1.6. 1.7. 2.1. 1This article presumes familiarity with my IJAL 43 "Uto-Aztecan Morphophonemics," (1977): 27-36, which describes such processes as i-ablaut, hardening, truncation, and various reduplications, as well as stress alternations referred to in this article. Language abbreviations are: [IJAL, vol. 44, no. 3, July 1978, pp. 211-22] ? 1978 by The University of Chicago. 0020-7071/78/4403-0005 $01.09 Az(tec), Ca(huilla), Cu(pefio), Ho(pi), Lu(isefio), Mo(no), Pa(pago),Se(rrano),So(uthern)Pa(iute), Ta(rahumara),and Tu(batulabal). 2 The remainderof this article should be read hand in hand with the paradigms shown in the eleven tables. Specific cross-references are not given, and forms mentioned in the body of the article are often left unglossed if their meanings can be determined from the tables and accompanying notes. 211 This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 212 INTERNATIONALJOURNAL OF AMERICANLINGUISTICS VOL.44 TABLE 1 SOPA -'KI-/i-NA- CLASS1 Type 1 Intransitive: punctual................ durative................. distributive-iterative...... plural durative........... plural punctual........... iterative................. pa'ka-'kipaka-'kipaki-caipaki-cai-ruNCV-CV'CV-'ki- Type 2 yu'm?u-'kiyu'm?u-kaCVCV-ki-(rare) CVCi-caiCVCi-cai-JuNyu-yu'm?u-'ki- Transitive(causative): punctual ............... -ka'pi-n?a- durative................. -kapi-na- plural-objectdurative..... plural-objectpunctual.... iterative................ -paki-ca-paki-ca-juN-CV-CV'Ci-n?a- f-ninci-'ki-ki- takiu'kiu-'ki-ki-ninci-kita'kiu-ki-rjki- -CVCi-ca-CVCi-ca-ruN-CV-CVCV-'ki-jki- 1 Data from Edward Sapir, SouthernPaiute, Proceedingsof the AmericanAcademy of Arts and Science 65 (1930-31). Forms given in CV symbols are not attested with actual roots but marks seem to be possible according to Sapir's grammatical statements. A dash (-) clearly impossible forms. The roots are paka- to tear, yu'm?u- to move, to start, kapV- to cut, to break, -ninci- to shake, ta'kiu- to reduceto small pieces. Type-1 verbs contain medial stops, type-2 verbs contain medial nonstops (or, with ta'kiu-, a medial stop which is invariably hardened). Thus type-1 verbs can undergo stem-internalhardening, type-2 verbs cannot. The suffix -!iuN- found in some forms is the productive punctual suffix in other verb classes and has secondarily intruded into this class to insure that the punctual/durative opposition is rigorously maintained. nonverbalsentences.This facilitateshistoricalreinterpretation of stativesas nouns and vice versa. (or participles) the derivational Second, primacy of intransitive versus transitive themes is ambivalent;eitherthe intransitiveis interpreted as a (medio)passivederivativeof the transitive,or the transitiveis taken as the causativederivativeof the intransitive. This resultsin historicalchangessuch as the rebuildingof the intransitiveparadigm by replacing archaic simple (underived) SoPa and Mo (in the Numic group) show -ki- and -'ki-, derivedfrom *-ki- by generalization of the i-ablauted form *-k(i)-,which is seen in Ho -ki-n- and Se -k-in(a-)(*-ki- plus *i-na-),and by extension of (punctual)consonant hardening (C - 'C) to thematic suffixesas well as root-medialconsonants. Lu -ax- derives from *-ki- by spirantizationand shift of morphemeboundariesresultingfromfinalvowel elision (*kwiti-xki- *kit-x *kwo to-x -* kw6 t-ax, the last step showing generalizationof -ax at the expenseof the less commonendings*-ox and *-ix). Cu has extended *-ax- as *-yax- (now 1.1. I begin by consideringthe PNUA -yox-),andCa-i- mayperhapsbe a (slightly intransitiveparadigm.The reconstructible irregular)reflex of the same *-yax-. The thematic suffixes are *-ki- (Se -qi-, Ho source of the *y is obscure, but it should be -k-/-ki-, Lu -ax-) and probably *i-tai- noted that Lu replacesthematic-ax- (and (SoPa t-cai-, Ho t-ta). The latter was causativeequivalent-i-) with morphemes specifically distributive, while *-ki- was basedon an element-ya in somepasttense forms. At any rate, the extensionof *y in less specific aspectually. intransitive forms by new intransitive derivatives based on transitive forms. This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions NO.3 213 UTO-AZTECAN *NA-CLASSVERBS Cu can be motivated functionally; because of various phonological rules the crucial distinction between intransitive *-ax- and causative *-in- would otherwise be neutralized in the important construction (found in Cu but not Lu) Root-PronominalThematic-Tense characteristic of some tenses. Thus with 3sg -po- we get Root-poyox-Tense (intr.) versus Root-po-n-Tense (caus.), and were it not for the y in -yox-, the former would be Root-po-x-Tense. But before the important set of tense suffixes beginning with q (-qal, -qa, etc.) both x and n are zeroed, so were it not for the y in -yox-, we would get total neutralization as Root-po-0-Tense. The extension of the *y then can be seen as functionally motivated. TABLE 3 MO COLOR-VERB CLASS1 .................. punctual puhi-pono-ki- emphatic punctual........... distributive................ intermittentiterative......... pu'hi-'po?no-'kapuhi-pono-wapu'hi-'po?no-'ka- 1 Data from Lamb (see table 2). The root here is puhi-, with dummy augment -pono- (helping to create the CVCVCVCVcanonical shape which is the norm for this class). reduplicateddistributivewith *i-tai-).There was also an iterative reduplication, and the (meager) evidence available suggests that hardening might have applied to it as shown above. The type *CV'CV-ki- occurs in SoPa pa'ka-'ki- (contrast paka-'ki- from duraand probably Mo qwa'cative *CCVCV-ki-) ?i-, though in the latter the suffix may not 1.2. The following is offered as a reason- directly reflect *-ki-. Ho punctual ("simable approximation to the system of plex") forms like ?iwi apparently reflect intransitive themes in the PNUA *na- *CV'CV (i.e., the perfective truncation of class: punctual *CV'CV-ki), with hardening preserved most clearly in preaspiration of *CV'CV-kipunctual durative *CVCV-kistops (yihti- to run, pl.), and the *-kidistributive *CV-CVCi-taireappears when any nonzero suffix is or *CVCi-taipresent. Neither Mo nor Ho directly preserves durative *CVCV-ki-, filling this gap iterative *CVCV-CV'CV-kiAt least for roots of the shape *CVTV-, by extending the productivity of the diswith *T a stop, it seems certain that there tributive and/or iterative forms. However, Lu (like SoPa) appears to prewas a hardened/unhardened opposition serve the old punctual/durative opposition *CV'TV- versus *CVTV-, the former in the form of vowel-length alternations basically punctual and the latter nonlike kw6t-ax(*ki'ti-ki-) versus kw6ot-axpunctual (probably occurring in a simple of durative with suffix *-ki- as well as in the (*kwiti-ki-),reflecting morphologization short of redundant formerly lengthening stressed vowel before simple but not fortis TABLE 2 consonant. Together, SoPa and Lu eviMo -II--HI- CLASS1 dence points to a PNUA opposition q a'ca-iipunctual.................... *CV'CV-ki- versus *CVCV-ki- of unre.qWaca-ca-hiaca-ca-hi.................... durative durative.{ duplicated themes. It is possible, however, Ltani-ni-hithat this opposition was actualized only ..... qwaca-ca-'paiirepetitive.............. for* CVTV- roots with medial stop (it is intermittentiterative.......... tani-'ni-kinot certain that nasals, liquids, or semi1 Data from Sidney Lamb, "Mono Grammar"(Ph.D. diss., vowels could distinguish simple from fortis University of California, Berkeley, 1958). Roots are qwacato fall and tani- to ring. There are no separate intransitiveand transitive paradigms. varieties). This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 214 LINGUISTICS INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OFAMERICAN In Mo, -'ki-/-ki- (the reflex of *-ki-) has been replaced in its original primary function by -?i- (unless this is a phonologically irregular reflex of *-ki-). It survives, however, in its secondary function as part of iterative *CVCV-CV'CV-ki- (now intermittent iterative of the type tani-'ni-ki-), and has been extended to other verb classes as an intermittent iterative suffix (without reduplication). It also survives, in its original primary function (nondistributive, hence mainly punctual) in the colorverb class (puhi-pono-ki-), which has split off from the -?i-/-hi- thematic class. VOL.44 TABLE4 Lu -AX-/-I- CLASS1 Intransitive Transitive (Causative) punctual........ kw6t-ax- hus-i- durative ........ kw6: t-ax- hui:S-i- dual-paucal...... intensive I....... intensive II...... CVC-CVC-ax- car-car-ipeci-ve6-ipl6a-v61-axpee: -ve6-ihaka-hak-ax- 1 Data from A. L. Kroeber and George Grace, The Sparkman Grammarof Luiseno, UCPL 16 (1960), esp. pp. 161-70. The semantic value of the three reduplicative types is somewhat unclear. Roots are kw6t- to get up, p61- to chap, crack open, hak- to pant, hu?- to smell, to smoke, dir- to tear off, ped- to drip. The *na-class iterative seems to have ended with *-ki- rather than *i-tai- or a separate iterative ending. The strongest evidence for this is Numic (e.g., SoPa and Mo), since the SoPa iterative uses -'kirather than i-cai- and since in Mo, -ki-/-'kinow survives only as a kind of iterative marker. In Takic (Se, Lu, Cu, Ca) the iterative always ends in *-ki-, though the value of this evidence is diminished by the fact that *-ki- has generalized throughout and has displaced *-tai- even in distributive forms. In Ho, 1-ta(rather than -k-) is now predominant in iterative reduplications such as royai-ya-ta, but this is just one result of the significant expansion of 1-tato (almost) all nonpunctual themes in the k-class in this language. A special inceptive iterative in -yki (perhaps *-ki preceded by an intrusive morpheme *-yV-) is a probable vestige of an archaic iterative type with 1.3. A distributive reduplication type *CV- can be reconstructed for PNUA verbs (not only the *na-class). In the *naclass, this probably resulted in a theme *CV-CVCi-tai-, though it is possible that an unreduplicated variant *CVCi-tai- also occurred. In the SoPa type, paki-caireduplication is usually absent, but this is probably because the form is now mainly a plural-subject type, hence dissociated in part from the synchronic distributive reduplication. Ho type c61-ta or cohc6l-ta is frequently but not always reduplicated. Ho 1-ta has been greatly expanded in scope and has been extended to new formations in other verb classes, in the k-class transitive paradigm, etc. SoPa 1-cai-, however, is restricted to the -'ki-/I-na-class intransitive paradigm. The Ca type -cap- -ci-cap- (the re*-ki-. duplicated form occurring in morphologiI reconstruct the PNUA iterative as cally specifiable environments) is probably *CVCV-CV'CV-ki- with hardened *'C a reflex of distributive *(CV-)CVCi-tai-, chiefly on the basis of Numic data. Both but has dropped the old thematic suffix. SoPa iteratives (unfortunately, no clear 1.4. The iterative has been subject to example for the -'ki-/-na- class happens to extensive analogical reshaping and is thus be available) and Mo intermittent iteradifficultto reconstruct. I reconstruct the re- tives (tani-'ni-ki-) show hardening of the duplicative segment as bisyllabic (*CVCV-), final C in the root. It is conceivable that both for the *na-class and other classes, on Proto-Numic extended hardening from the basis of direct or indirect evidence from (simple) punctual to iterative themes, but all four NUA subgroups. there is no particular evidence against This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions NO. 3 UTO-AZTECAN *NA-CLASS VERBS positing hardening for the iterative as far back as PNUA. The Takic evidence is at least compatible with this. The Ho evidence is noncommittal. 1.5. Perfective truncation (loss of final *CV) appears to have applied in PNUA to the punctual form *CV'CV-ki, producing *CV'CV. Se still distinguishes perfective mimi from punctual mimi?-(i-). Ho preserves suffixless *CV'CV, but has abandoned *CV'CV-ki (though presuffixal *CV'CV-ki- is very common). The result is that there is no perfective/punctual opposition (the single suffixless form is called simplex or punctual), while -k- (*-ki-) now patterns as a thematizing augment required when suffixes are present. The only vestige of word-final *-ki in Ho is inceptive iterative -yki (*-yV-ki). Aside from Ho and Se, the perfective truncation appears to have been lost by NUA languages. 215 labial stop, since *p is often lenited (e.g., to h) or zeroed in SUA languages which preserve *'p as p or b. However, any aspectual stem alternations (such as *ta'paversus *tapa- for to split) have been leveled out, and this leveling probably preceded PSUA. 1.7. At first sight the comparison of Az -ka- and SoPa -ka- (both intransitive thematic suffixes in the *na-class) invites reconstruction of *-ka- as a PUA durative suffix contrasting with punctual *-0 or *-ki-. However, I regard both the Az and SoPa examples with -ka- as representing independent intrusions of an adjectival suffix *-ka- into the *na-class. SoPa indeed retains *-ka- in this function; it is added to color verbs before participial -ti-, hence sakwa-ka-ti- blue from sakwa- to be blue. Though color-verb -kaand thematic suffix -ka- in SoPa are synchronically dissociated, I feel they are 1.6. Moving south to SUA languages, I historically identical and that thematic reconstruct for PSUA the following *na- -ka- is a specialized instance of adjectival *-ka- which has been adopted by the -'ki-/1class intransitive paradigm: na- class (or rather its subdivision type 2). This has been motivated by the fact that *CVCV simple without a special durative suffix, type-2 iterative-intensive *CV-CVCV verbs could not express the punctual/durafor evidence tive opposition (very crucial for all SoPa no is There satisfactory forms. in these suffixes verb classes) since type-2 verbs cannot thematic positing sufthematic intransitive lack Ta Pa and undergo punctual hardening for phonotransitive (causative) logical reasons (they have no medial fixes, though showing in the stops). those to suffixes thematic cognate -kasuffixes thematic intransitive In Proto-Numic, *-ka- was probably a Az north. derived is -niare -niand innovations; TABLE 5 from transitive -na- by extension of an CU -YOX-/-IN-CLASS1 intransitive/transitivestem-finali/a alternaTransitive Intransitive (Volitional) tion, while -ka- is an intruder into the *na(Nonvolitional) class (see below). Ic,-X-yoxfc,-X-inPSUA lacked a productive hardened/un(pisil-X-in(pisilY-X-yoxhardened stem alternation, mainly because 1Data from Jane Hill, "Volitional and Non-Volitional of phonological mergers of the type *C and Verbs in Cupeno," Papers from the Fifth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society (1969): 348-56. X here stands for *'C becoming PSUA *C. The only stop zero in most forms, but can be a pronominal like 3sg -pa- in distinguishing reflexes of *C and *'C is the some tense forms. Roots are cAs- to shine and pisily- to spill This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 216 VOL.44 INTERNATIONALJOURNAL OF AMERICANLINGUISTICS suffix (of somewhat obscure function) used mainly with color verbs; note Mo -'ka- in color-verb emphatic punctual pu'hi-'po'no'ka- (where all medial C's have acquired punctual hardening) as well as SoPa colorverb -ka-. It seems that this color-verb class, though possessing some distinctive features, was morphologically affiliated to the (main) *na-class, though the two have since diverged. It is thus easy to see how SoPa -ka- could have split into two suffixes, one of them a -'ki-/I-na- class thematic suffix. Probably related are Se resultative nominalizer -ka? (found chiefly in the qclass, hence c6i:c-ka? shriveled thing), Pa stative -k(a-) with adjectives (sopol-k to be short, sopol-ka-him to become short), Az -k and presuffixal-ka- with adjectives, and possibly Tu thematic -ha:- (*-ka-) in a few alternations like caba-ha:- to be worn versus distributive caba:-?a-. I am inclined to consider Az thematic -ka- in -ni/-naclass frequentives like te-tekWi-ka-as representing a specialization of this *-ka- similar to that seen in SoPa. The suggested cognate set for *-ka- involves some apparent difficulties since it includes verbal, adjectival, and nominalziing elements. However, the problems seem surmountable in light of the discussion in 0 above. This cognate set, incidentally, is distinct from those of phonologically similar etyma, such as PNUA agentive *-'ka- and various tense suffixes. Other than noting that PUA *-ka- was an essentially adjectival element, it is difficult to pin down its precise function (nominalizing, stative aspect, connecting augment) since there has been so much semantic skewing in the various languages. 2.1. We now move on to the transitive (causative) paradigm. For PSUA the following can be reconstructed: *CVCV-nasimple *CVCV-cadistributive iterative-intensive *CV-CVCV-naor *CV-CVCV-caThe suffixal opposition *-na-/*-ca- is well attested: Ta -na-/-ca-, Pa -n/-s, Az -na-/-ca-. In the reduplicated iterativeintensive it is not certain whether *-na- or *-ca- was used. 2.2. The PNUA transitive system was quite different. There were apparently two competing paradigms, a simple one not too different from the PSUA system just sketched, and a complex one with two thematic suffixes for each theme. Leaving aside the iterative, the simple paradigm was this: *CV'Ci-napunctual *CVCi-nadurative distributive *CV-CVCi-caor *CVCi-caNote that in PNUA we have i-ablaut suffixes *i-na- and *i-ca- contrasting with nonablauting PSUA *-na- and *-ca-. PNUA *i-na- is attested in Ho 1-na-,SoPa 1-na-,Cu and Ca -in-, Se -ina-, Lu -i-, and Tu t-na- (in Tu this is now the productive causative suffix for all verbs). Distributive *i-ca- survives only in SoPa i-ca-. TABLE 6 CA -I-/-IN- CLASS1 Transitive (Causative) Intransitive simple......... -Eap-i- distributive I... -Ci-Spa-m- -E, p-ind-iApa-m-n- distributive II .. .-{e-cap- distributiveIII.. { - -ay-n-Ce-cA-9pa:-n1 Data from Anna Fuchs, Morphologiedes Verbsim Cahuilla (The Hague, 1970), esp. pp. 72-73. The root is -dSp(a)- to split and a semantically similar derivative-de-ap-. In cases of two variants for distributive forms (e.g., -Up- vs. -c-Aap-), the choice of forms depends on the following suffix. This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions NO.3 UTO-AZTECAN *NA-CLASS VERBS 217 TABLE 7 SE Q-CLASS1 Intransitive punctual............ resultative........... perfective ............ distributive.......... frequentativeI ....... frequentativeII ...... 'hW4:-q(i-) biu: r-q(i-) a]4q-q(i-) nq-P-q(i-) mimi (mimi-0) Transitive (Causative) rririr-k-in(a-) nb-k-in(a-) min4i-k-in(a-) mini (mni-0) nibi-m-in(a-) hwc-hwc-q(i-) rjirir-ab(a-) 1 Data from Kenneth Hill, "A Grammar of the Serrano Language" (Ph.D. diss., University of California, Los Angeles, 1967). Dots representretroflexion (of consonants and vowels). The resultative suffix has base form /-i-/ but has a variant -i?- when retroflexion is assimilated from a neighboring phoneme. Roots are hwa: - to climb, biu:r- to roll up, }aq- to perch, mimi- to die, iirir- to move, nibi- to bury, min}- to swallow. The corresponding forms of the complex paradigm are these: *CV'CV-ki-napunctual distributive *CVCV-mi-naThe form *CV'CV-ki-na- consists of the intransitive theme *CV'CV-ki- plus causative *i-na- (1.2). It is theoretically possible that there was a punctual/durative opposition of *CV'CV-ki-na- versus *CVCV-kina-, at least for roots with medial stop, but there is no direct evidence for this and the idea is based entirely on structuralanalogy from the intransitive paradigm. The form *CVCV-mi-na- is apparently based on *CVCV-mi with plural suffix *-mi. In Ho, some k-class roots like p6ro to be perforated; a perforation can be formally verbal or nominal, and in the latter case can be pluralized (p6ro-m group of holes). The construction *CVCV-mi-na can then be taken as the causative with *i-na- of a predicate-nominal use of *CVCV-mi (e.g., it is a group of holes). The complex paradigm is best preserved in Ho (q6hi-k-na, ripa-m-na) and Se (nib-k-ina-, nibi-m-ina-). That the theme in *-mi-na- was built on unhardened *CVCV- (rather than *CV'CV-) is shown by indirect phonological evidence-the stress placement in Ho and the retention of the root-final V in Se. Cupan (Lu, Cu, Ca) generally abandons the complex paradigm, but vestiges survive. Ca -capa-m-n-reflects*CVCV-mi-na-, and the reduplicated variant -ca-spa-m-imay be from *-ca-capa-mi-n- with later irregular loss of the final *n. Ca has intransitive -ca-spa-m-, which may be another trace of the old predicate-nominal type *CVCV-mi presumed to underlie the more common causative theme *CVCVmi-na-. In Cu, the puzzling repeated /-ma-/ in plural subject-marking pronominals inserted between root and causative thematic suffix, for example, 3pl -pa?mo- from /-po-m(o)-mo-/ before causative -(i)n- (contrast 3pl -pomo- before intransitive thematic suffix), may be a specialized reflex of *-mi in *CVCV-mi. 2.3. There are possible vestiges of the complex paradigm in SoPa as well, notably in type-2 -'ki-/-na- class causatives (-ninci'ki-rjki,etc.). Here thematic -'ki- (punctual) or -ki- (durative) is followed by benefactive -1ki-, which here has causative sense. In the case of-ninci-, -r1ki-is used after -'kibut omitted after -ki-, while for -ta'kiu- we get -jki- in both forms. In one way or another, SoPa -'ki-jki- This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 218 INTERNATIONALJOURNAL OF AMERICANLINGUISTICS VOL. 44 TABLE 8 Ho K-CLASS1 Intransitive Transitive (Causative) rp6ro roya punctual................. qohi-k-na [lana punctual (presuffixal)...... flnja-k(i)h-( ? (r6:yi-n-ta distributiveI.............. distributiveII............. inceptive iterative.......... iterative.................. imperative................ c6l-ta Lc6-hcol-ta roya-ya-yki royi-ya-ta wari-ki-i qohJ-k-na- Lanja-kj-n-ta laia-k-ni-ta [larj-toy-ni-ta rip.-m-na royi-ya-yki-na roya-ya-toy-na laia-k-na-'a 1 Data chiefly from Benjamin L. Whorf, "Hopi," Microfilm Collection of Manuscriptson Middle AmericanCulturalAnthropology48 (1935) and "Toreva Dialect," in Linguistic Structures of Native America (New York, 1946), pp. 158-83. A few transitive forms were obtained by me in a few hours' fieldwork with Ho speakers in Phoenix. Roots are pbro to perforate, r6ya to turn,llaoa to pull, ?iwi to flame, c6lo to drip, qohi to break, rihpa to separate. Dots indicate extra-short vowel length (not retroflexion). Acute and grave accents indicate primary and secondary stress, respectively. Whorf's transcriptions with extrashort length are probably unreliable for vowels showing secondary stress, but seem reliable for vowels with primary stress. and -ki-iki- probably reflect the complex transitive structure *CV'CV-ki-na-. Perhaps -iki- itself was originally *I-na- plus *-ki-, the latter possibly identical to (normally intr.) thematic suffix *-ki- seen above. However, it is also possible that -riki- has simply replaced older *i-na- in these forms. The reason SoPa type-2 (but not type-1) -'ki-/I-na- class verbs preserved this construction was that it enables them to maintain the punctual/durative opposition (chiefly by distinguishing -'ki- from -ki- before -qki-). Type-1 verbs are phonologically capable of hardening the rootmedial stop to express the punctual and thus have no need of aspectual oppositions in the suffix. All in all, then, the simple paradigm is preserved clearly in SoPa (type 1) and in Cupan (Lu, Cu, Ca), while the complex paradigm is preserved in Ho and Se and leaves vestiges in SoPa and Cupan. Because both paradigms thus cut across subgroup divisions it is quite probable that both occurred in PNUA. Whether this means that there was free variation, or some criterion was used to divide *na-class roots into two types, is not clear. One possibility is that the SoPa distribution is archaic, with the simple paradigm typical of roots capable of stem-internalhardeningto mark punctual aspect, while the complex paradigm was used with other roots and permitted suffixal expression of the punctual/ durative opposition. 2.4. SoPa has recently (since ProtoNumic) split *"-na- into two suffixes, punctual 1-n?a-and durative i-na-. This is a further extension of consonantal hardening as a punctual morphophonemic change affecting thematic suffixes (as well as many roots); note intransitive -'ki- from *-ki-. Since SoPa has no opposition 'n versus n (i.e., fortis versus lenis n), glottalization has been called upon as the manifestation of hardening. Glottalization of nonstops as This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions NO. 3 219 UTO-AZTECAN *NA-CLASS VERBS TABLE 9 PA -N/-S CLASS1 simple ............. distributive/pluralobject ............ Intransitive Transitive (Causative) ta: p ta: pa-n ta:pa-$ 1 Data from Dean Saxton and Lucille Saxton, Papago and Pima-English, English-Papago and Pima Dictionary (Tucson, Ariz., 1969). The base forms of the transitive suffixes are /-na-/ and /-sa-/, and the latter reflects *-ca-. The root is /ta: pa-/ to split. a correlate of hardening (gemination) of stops is found also in some SoPa reduplication types. 2.5. As in the intransitive,reconstruction of PNUA transitive forms is made difficult by the possibilities for analogical reshaping based on unreduplicated forms in each language. An educated guess would be *CVCV-CV'Ci-na- for the simple paradigm and *CVCV-CV'CV-ki-na- (in the latter case, stem hardening may have been vacuous for phonological reasons) for the complex one. These are simple reduplicated forms of the punctuals, just as in the intransitive it seems that the iterative was based on the punctual (rather than distributive) form. However, there is another possibility. Se has causative frequentatives with thematic suffix -ab(a-) in the q-class for certain roots (generally CVCVC-, i.e., *CVCVCV-). This may match Mo iterative -'pa?i-, which can be etymologically analyzed as *-pa- plus punctual thematic suffix -?i-, before which hardening regularly applies. So a PNUA iterative *CVCV-pa-, or reduplicated *CVCV-CVCV-pa-, is conceivable for the transitive paradigm. It appears that hardening did not take place before *-pa-, so Mo shows qwaca-ca-'pa?i-rather than *qwaca-'ca-'pa?i-; contrast tani-'niki- showing hardening in intermittent iteratives with -ki-. PSUA probably had one or more iterative and/or intensive reduplications in the transitive *-na-class paradigm, but it is difficult to decide which thematic suffix was used. Reconstruction of *CV-CVCVna- and/or *CV-CVCV-ca- would probably not be too far from the mark. 2.6. There are several remaining problems, including a possibly reconstructible element *-yV-. Consider Ho laq-toy-na, lai-toy-ni-ta, and royai-ya-toy-na.Here we find an element -toy- replacing distributive i-ta when directly followed by causative 1-na-.Since -ta is replaced by -tota in some plural-subject forms, we can perhaps isolate *-tu- and hence take -toy- as *-tu-yVand -tota as *-tu-ta. This *-yV- brings to mind the -y- in Ho inceptive iterative -yki (which includes thematic -ki). In both cases the *-yVelement occurs as a kind of linking element in a nonpunctual k-class theme. A possible cognate shows up in the Ca distributivetype -ce-cip-ay-n- (reduplicated -ce-ca-spa:-n-), apparently reflecting *-citapa-y(a)-n-; synchronic morpheme boundaries are debatable. The -n- here is perhaps related to distributive -n- in a few Ho forms like r6: yai-n-ta,and a relationship to a suffixal cognate set pointing to PUA continuative (past) *-ni- is possible (SoPa -n?i-, Ya -n, Hu narrative -ni-). As in Ho, Ca *-yV- (*-ya-) seems to be a linking element in nonpunctual *-na-class themes. However, it is not clear exactly where *-yVTABLE 10 TA -NA-/-CA- CLASS1 simple ............. distributive/pluralobject ............ Intransitive Transitive (Causative) kagi- kagi-nikasi-ca- 1 Data from David Brambila, Gramdtica Rardmuri (Mexico City, 1953), p. 407. The root is kasi- to break (quebrar). This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 220 INTERNATIONALJOURNAL OF AMERICANLINGUISTICS VOL.44 proves we will discover additional evidence of the interaction between *-na-class thematic morphology and other features of morphosyntax such as tense inflection. 2.7. Particularly in the Numic group This article has been presented on the (SoPa, Mo), transitive *-na-class themes somewhat artificial assumption that *-nagenerally require an instrumental prefix class morphology can be analytically isomeaning with the handor the like. Thus Mo lated from the rest of the verbal system, but -qo'ti- to break occurs in prefixed forms in the long run this approach (even more like ca-'qo'ti-, wi-'qo'ti-, etc. Some "ex- dangerous in historical than in synchronic ceptions" like SoPa -ta'kiu- (which does linguistics) is inadequate and more comnot require a prefix) may be frozen com- prehensive, overarching restudies will be binations including old instrumentals (e.g., needed. *-ta-'kiu-). 3. Finally, we must deal with participial Instrumental prefixes have generally vanished from Cupan languages, but a few forms of *-na-class stems. For PNUA I survive marginally in Ca. These are es- would reconstruct the following: agentive pecially common in the -i-/-in- class (the *-'ka- (perhaps in this class used mainly *-na-class). One Ca prefix, -ce-, is seman- with transitives), (medio)passive participles tically basically an intensive and may be *-'pi- (punctual) and *i-pi- (nonpunctual), related to Pa intensive prefix s- (both from and derived nominal (e.g., product-of*ci-). In Ca there is a statistical correlation action, place-of-action) *-'pi-. Of these, between use of an instrumental prefix or *-'ka- was also productive in other verb -ce- and the occurrence of the -yV- element classes, while the three others seem to have in distributives (2.6). Of ten verbs attested been particularly productive in the *-nawith -yV-, seven have one or another of class and perhaps in some cases restricted to it. It is not clear whether another these prefixes. Pa also has instrumental and similar nominalizing process, stem-final i-ablaut prefixes, though the actual inventory (e.g., CVCV - CVCi) and possible addigenerally differs from those of NUA lan- tion of regular nominal suffixes, occurred guages and the historical relationships here in the *-na-class, but regardless of this it is are problematic. In Pa, as in SoPa and Ca, quite possible that derived nominal *-'pithere is a close association between pre- is really (medio)passive participle *-'pifixes and transitive *-na-class (-n/-S class) plus i-ablaut. stems. For example, -ko- occurs in ma'iBy "(medio)passive"is meant a participle ko-n and ma?i-ko-* to deliver a glancing which creates passive participles (e.g., blow with thrownobject. broken)from transitive bases, but can also These data at least raise the possibility create participles from intransitive stative of a significant tendency for transitive (mediopassive) bases (e.g., having burst). *-na-class forms to occur with instrumen- The boundary between passive and mediotal prefixes, and this seems to have clearly passive is often hazy in NUA participles, been the case in PNUA. It is likely that as since the participial suffixes are added to our knowledge of the protolanguages im- verb forms which may lack causative or other voice-marking thematic suffixes. 3 I am not optimistic about correlating these The PNUA system is best preserved in instances of *-yV- with Lu -ya (replacing thematic suffixesin some past tense forms). Ho, where we get agentive -qa (after occurred in the PNUA *-na-class system, and the reconstruction is tentative.3 I am aware of no clear SUA parallels. This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions NO. 3 UTO-AZTECAN *NA-CLASS VERBS TABLE 11 Az -NI-/-NA- CLASS1 Intransitive fkoto:-ni- simple....... {kWeyo:-ni- . Transitive (Causative) (koto:-na- koto:-ni-ya- ka-kapa-cafrequentative (te-tekWi-kakwee-kweyo:-ka1 Data from Stanley Newman, "Classical Nahuatl," Handbook of Middle AmericanIndians 5 (1967): 179-99, and from B. L. Whorf, "The Milpa Alta Dialect of Aztec," in Linguistic Structuresof Native America, pp. 367-97. Roots are koto- to break, tekwi- to burst into flame, kweyo- to flash, and kapato make a slappingsound. stressed vowel -hqa), (medio)passive -vi and -pi, and derived nominal -pi. The vowel preceding -vi, -pi, and -pi is syncopated in the examples available, so whether i-ablaut applies to this vowel is unclear. In Tu some of this system survives, though the *-na-class has no separate existence and the participial suffixes thus apply to all verbs. Agentive *-'ka- is not preserved, at least as a participial. Instead, we get punctual -pi:- (*-'pi-) and nonpunctual 1-bi:- (*'-pi-) as agentive participials. The shift from (medio)passive to agentive voice is not as surprising as might appear, since in PNUA when *-'pi- or *i-pi- followed a stative intransitive the resulting participle (aspectually stative or mediopassive) could have had its voice value attributed to the stem rather than to the participial suffix; the resulting partial ambivalence in the voice value of the suffix may have facilitated its reinterpretation as an agentive participial, especially if the productivity of *-'ka- was declining. Passive participles can be formed in Tu either by adding -pi:- or t-bi:- to a form with passive suffix 1-wa- or by using stem-final i-ablaut. Numic (SoPa, Mo) generally retains *-'pi- and *-'pi- as productive (medio) passive participles, now used with all verb classes. At least in SoPa, -'pi- is still distinguishable from -'pi- by its product-of- 221 action sense, though the distinction is hazy in some cases. I know of no sure Numic reflexes of *-pi-, though there are a few possibilities deservingfurtherinvestigation. Agentive *-'ka- has generally been replaced by -ti- in Numic, at least in its originally primary function as a participial. In Takic, PNUA participials have a long and complex history which deserves article-length treatment. Basically, *i-piand *-'pi- have become subordinators used in a variety of constructions, including gerunds and some kinds of relative clauses, while *-'pi- seems to have been lost. Agentive *-'ka- usually survives, but tends to become specialized as a predicate nominal with particular tense-aspect value and is thus functionally a verb form (though still using nominal inflectional suffixes like plural -m). Se agentive -Pac and Cu agentive I-voa-?c (including *i-pi-) appear to show an agentive element *-?ac or *i'-ac representing a Proto-Takic innovation. As far as the *-na-class is concerned, it seems that the participial endings in PNUA were regularly added to a theme of the form *CV'CV-ki-, and in the case of *'-pieither *CVCV-ki- or *CVCV-mi- (for the simple and complex paradigms, respectively). In Ho, which has the most archaic participial forms, note examples like lagak-pi pulled apart, por6-m-vi pierced with holes, and wari-k-pi runningplace. The PSUA participial system is less clear at the moment. Agentive *-'ka- in PNUA usually corresponds to something like *-(k)ami- or *-(k)ami- in the south, but since this is not restricted to or specifically associated with the *-na-class, a fuller discussion will be omitted here. I know of no reflexes of *-'pi-, *p-pi-, or *-'pi-, though stem-final i-ablaut occurs in several SUA languages to form a productof-action nominal. 4. The reconstructions suggested here This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 222 INTERNATIONALJOURNAL OF AMERICANLINGUISTICS for the *-na-class generally support the view that UA is divided into two basic genetic divisions, NUA and SUA. It is possible to show how the NUA *-na-class systems derived from PNUA, and how the SUA systems derived from PSUA, by positing rather distinct PNUA and PSUA systems as suggested above. Among many notable differences are: (a) the fact that PNUA had i-ablaut with *i-na- and *i-ca-, while PSUA had nonablauting *-na-, *-ca-; (b) PNUA intransitive forms had VOL.44 thematic suffixes(*-ki-, *I-tai-)while PSUA had suffixless forms; (c) PNUA but not PSUA had a complex transitive paradigm (*CV'CV-ki-na-, *CVCV-mi-na-); and (d) PNUA but apparently not PSUA had participials *-'pi-, *i-pi-, and *-'pi-. In conjunction with accumulating evidence from other historical investigations it now seems unlikely that the genetic split between NUA and SUA can be seriously disputed. This content downloaded from 141.211.123.197 on Wed, 9 Oct 2013 22:48:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions