* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Impact of poor nutrition in early life
Food studies wikipedia , lookup
Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics wikipedia , lookup
Food choice wikipedia , lookup
Stunted growth wikipedia , lookup
Thrifty gene hypothesis wikipedia , lookup
Epidemiology of metabolic syndrome wikipedia , lookup
Abdominal obesity wikipedia , lookup
Diet-induced obesity model wikipedia , lookup
Human nutrition wikipedia , lookup
Obesity and the environment wikipedia , lookup
Childhood obesity wikipedia , lookup
Obesity in the Middle East and North Africa wikipedia , lookup
Impact of poor nutrition on child development and educational attainment Dr Michelle McKinley (R.Nutr Public Health) Senior Lecturer, Centre for Public Health School of Medicine, Dentistry & Biomedical Science [email protected] Presentation structure • Impact of poor nutrition in early life • Effect of nutrition interventions on learning & performance of school age children • Breakfast • Sugar • Fish oils • Multivitamins/minerals • School food interventions • Conclusion Impact of poor nutrition Why is good nutrition in early life so important? • Optimal growth • Optimal cognitive development • Optimal health • Prevention of chronic disease in later life The first 9 months can shape the rest of your life……. Critical periods before and during pregnancy when specific nutrients are needed for optimal development. Risks of a number of chronic diseases in adulthood such as hypertension, diabetes, heart disease may have their origins before birth. Promoting optimal health through the lifecourse Critical and sensitive periods of growth & development • Critical periods – relatively short window of time when the body is vulnerable to adverse environmental exposures such as under-nutrition – significant and lasting effect on health e.g. folate and neural tube defects • Sensitive developmental stages – broader window of time when social and cognitive skills, habits, coping strategies, attitudes and values are more easily acquired than at later ages, e.g: • Language is acquired more readily during first 10 years of life • Can be acquired at a later age but not with same level of proficiency • Low maternal iodine status was associated with an increased risk of suboptimum scores for verbal IQ at age 8 years, and reading accuracy, comprehension and reading score at age 9 years • Results suggested a worsening trend in cognitive outcome with decreasing maternal status • Possible in-utero effect of sub-optimal iodine status 2013;382:331-37 Brain development • Diet provides the building blocks and fuel for the brain • Very rapid brain growth in last trimester of pregnancy and first 2 years of life • Malnutrition in the first year of life has a lasting effect on both behaviour and cognition • Motor skills and language develop more slowly • Intelligence scores and school performance are poorer • Early malnutrition associated with higher incidence of aggressive behaviour when children aged 9-15 years • Further peaks of brain growth have been found at 7, 12 and 15 years of age Impact of poor nutrition in early life • Vitamin / mineral deficiencies • Growth stunting • Impaired cognitive development • Childhood obesity • Childhood type 2 diabetes Impact of poor nutrition in early life • Vitamin / mineral deficiencies • Growth stunting • Impaired cognitive development • Childhood obesity • Childhood type 2 diabetes Diet of UK schoolchildren • Most children in the UK are consuming: • too much saturated fat, sugar and salt • too little fibre and fruit and vegetables - ~ 80-90% of children aged 11-18 yrs in the UK are not meeting the 5-aday recommendation • Few meeting oily fish recommendation • Micronutrients of concern – Iron, zinc, magnesium, iodine, vitamin D • Energy imbalance Global Prevalence of Overweight in Boys Prior to 1990 % Overweight <5% 5-9.9% 10-14.9% 15-19.9% 20-24.9% 25-29.9% ≥30% Self Reported data © International Association for the Study of Obesity Global Prevalence of Overweight in Boys 1990-1999 % Overweight <5% 5-9.9% 10-14.9% 15-19.9% 20-24.9% 25-29.9% ≥30% Self Reported data © International Association for the Study of Obesity, London 2010 Global Prevalence of Overweight in Boys 2000-2008 % Overweight <5% 5-9.9% 10-14.9% 15-19.9% 20-24.9% 25-29.9% ≥30% Self Reported data © International Association for the Study of Obesity, London 2010 Global Prevalence of Overweight in Girls Prior to 1990 % Overweight <5% 5-9.9% 10-14.9% 15-19.9% 20-24.9% 25-29.9% ≥30% Self Reported data © International Association for the Study of Obesity, London 2010 Global Prevalence of Overweight in Girls 1990-1999 % Overweight <5% 5-9.9% 10-14.9% 15-19.9% 20-24.9% 25-29.9% ≥30% Self Reported data © International Association for the Study of Obesity, London 2010 Global Prevalence of Overweight in Girls 2000-2008 % Overweight <5% 5-9.9% 10-14.9% 15-19.9% 20-24.9% 25-29.9% ≥30% Self Reported data © International Association for the Study of Obesity, London 2010 Social disparities in childhood obesity Source: www.oecd.org US Childhood Obesity Campaign Childhood obesity in UK & Ireland • UK and Ireland - about 1 in 3 children are considered overweight or obese by the time they finish primary school • By 2050 - 55% of boys and 70% of girls could be overweight/ obese Safefood Campaign http://www.safefood.eu/Childhood-Obesity Childhood diabetes in the UK Type 2 diabetes in UK children: an emerging problem Etishham et al. Diabetic Medicine 2000; 17: 867-871 The first cases of type 2 diabetes in children in UK - overweight girls 9-16y of Indian, Pakastani or Arabic origin Example of a life-course approach framework Childhood obesity prevention: a life-course framework. Pérez-Escamilla and Kac, International Journal of Obesity 2013. Example of a life-course approach framework Childhood obesity prevention: a life-course framework. Pérez-Escamilla and Kac, International Journal of Obesity 2013. Effect of nutrition interventions on learning and performance of school age children Inclusion criteria: Controlled trials, age range 4-18 years Geographical spread n=1 n=1 n=1 n=6 n = 16 n=1 n=1 n=1 n=1 Age range and duration •Two thirds of the studies were carried out in primary age children •Over half - short duration (<1 month) Type of intervention Breakfast Sugar N=15 N=6 Fish oil Vitamin/Mineral supplements N=5 N=2 Breakfast • Young people are more likely to skip breakfast than any other meal • Breakfast skipping increases with age • Girls at secondary school highest at-risk group for skipping breakfast (Hoyland et al, 2012. Nutrition Bulletin) • Children who eat breakfast are more likely to meet daily nutrient intake guidelines compared with children who eat breakfast infrequently or skip breakfast Breakfast Ells et al, Br J Nutr 2008 • 15 studies examined the effect of breakfast • 10 identified an association between breakfast provision and some small cognitive and behavioural improvements • 5 studies found no effect • Variable design: Breakfast clubs; Breakfast vs fasting; Low- vs high-energy breakfast; Low- vs high-protein breakfast; Habitual breakfast vs standardised breakfast Key findings • Breakfast consumption is more beneficial than skipping breakfast • More apparent in children with compromised nutritional status • Studies of school breakfast programs suggest they can have positive effect on academic performance Breakfast Adolphus, Lawton, Dye 2013 Frontiers in Human Neuroscience • 36 studies examined effect of breakfast on in-class behaviour & academic performance in children and adolescents • 21 studies provided suggestive evidence that: – Habitual breakfast and school breakfast programs (SBP) have a positive effect on children’s academic performance – Most consistent effects observed for maths performance, most pronounced in undernourished children • 11/19 studies reported positive effects on classroom behaviour e.g. less ‘off-task’ and ‘out of seat’ behaviour • But - there can be unintended consequences of SBP..... Sugar studies Ells et al, Br J Nutr 2008 • 6 studies examined the effect of short term exposure to sugar intake – – – – Sucrose vs aspartame or aspartame + saccharin RCT cross-over design Primary school aged children (n=5) Children with symptoms of ADHD (n=5) • Sugar intake had no clinically significant adverse effects on learning or behavioural outcomes • Difficult to derive any conclusive outcome - small number of studies & the populations examined Fish oil studies Ells et al, Br J Nutr 2008 • 5 studies examined the effect of fish oil supplementation • Randomised placebo controlled trial design • Population aged 5-13 years with symptoms of neurodevelopmental disorders (dyspraxia and ADHD) • Very different in design – fatty acid composition, dose, duration, population studied, cognitive tests used • Overall - the findings were mixed and therefore effect on learning and behaviour inconclusive Vitamin/Mineral studies Ells et al, Br J Nutr 2008 • 2 studies examined the effect of vitamin/mineral supplementation • 1 showed a significant positive effect on IQ in a small subsample • 1 found no effect • These two studies alone provided insufficient evidence to formulate any conclusion Recent evidence – Vitamins & Minerals • Frensham et al, Nutr Rev 2012. Influences of micronutrient and omega-3 fatty acid supplementation on cognition, learning and behaviour: • Children in developed countries may perform better on tests of nonverbal intelligence and behavioural measures after receiving vitamin and mineral supplements compared with placebo • Strongest effects in trials over 3 months and sub-groups of children (low socioeconomic status, symptoms ADHD and/ or learning disabilities) School food and learning behaviour in primary schools Golley et al. Eur J Clin Nutr 2010;64:1280-8. • Six primary schools in Sheffield: 4 intervention, 2 control; 12 week study; years 3,4 and 5; n=146 • Food interventions – new menus, taster sessions, themed weeks, healthy eating workshops • Dining environment – changing layout and Q system; redecorating; buying new furniture, artwork and murals • Classroom behaviours observed in the hour after lunch at baseline and 12 weeks – ‘on-task’ and ‘offtask’ behavours www.childrensfoodtrust.org.uk School food and learning behaviour • Pupils in intervention schools 3.4 times more likely to be ‘on-task’ in teacher-pupil interactions compared with control schools • BUT pupils in intervention schools were 2.3 times more likely to be ‘off-task’ in pupil-pupil interactions than control schools • Conclusion - intervention had positive impact on alertness • ‘if this raised alertness is not appropriately channelled and supervised it may result in increased off-task behaviour when pupils are asked to work together’ www.childrensfoodtrust.org.uk N=156 N=156 “....+ve effect on engagement and concentration and their ability to learn in the classroom after lunch..also suggests pupils less likely to be disengaged” Storey et al. A randomized controlled trial of the effect of school food and dining room modifications on classroom behaviour in secondary school children. Eur J Clin Nutr 2011;65:32-8. Conclusion Conclusion • The importance of good nutrition in early life for optimal growth and development is irrefutable • There is increasing evidence that nutrition interventions in school age children can have a positive effect on cognitive function, learning and classroom behaviour • Need for further research and robust evaluations to guide future practice • Whole school approach to nutrition – can provide an environment that encourages positive attitudes to food and healthful behaviours among students