* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Do addicts use substances to
Survey
Document related concepts
Attachment disorder wikipedia , lookup
Attachment and Health wikipedia , lookup
Human bonding wikipedia , lookup
History of attachment theory wikipedia , lookup
Attachment in adults wikipedia , lookup
Dyadic developmental psychotherapy wikipedia , lookup
Transcript
Do addicts use substances to compensate for the lack of intimacy? 1 Title of Study Do addicts use substances to compensate for the lack of intimacy? Abstract Objectives: The purpose of this study is to examine whether addicts who have in common impoverished intimate relationships expect that the use of substances will provide soothing substitutes for intimacy. Design: Two questionnaires are used in the study. The first; Descutner and Thelen’s 1991 Fear of Intimacy Scale, (FIS) was used to identify impoverished intimate relationships in the participant group. A second questionnaire; Emotional Self- soothing Expectancy Scale, (ESES) was completed by the participants to identify expectations that substances would provide a soothing substitute for the lack of intimate relationships. Methods: The participants were 24 substance dependant clients undergoing treatment for drug dependency. The controlled group of 24 had professional and semi-professional backgrounds and were similar in age and gender to the participant group. The intention of the study was to discover whether the inability to be intimate was a common trait in the sample and whether there was a correlation for those deficiencies and any self-soothing substitute attachments to substances. Results: As expected the participant group reported high scores on the FIS tool indicating ‘poor intimacy’ and low scores on the ‘good intimacy’ indicators. Conversely the controlled group scored low on ‘poor intimacy’ and high on ‘good intimacy. Participants showed an expectation Conclusions: Refiguring addiction as a kind of intimacy is one way of making sense of the intense relationships people can develop with substances …..” (Keane 2004) 2 Introduction This research stems from a protracted study and interest of commonly applied therapeutic interventions and a desire to contribute to their content and scope. The current position regarding the treatment of substance dependency henceforth referred to as drug 1addiction, ranges from dispensing substitute medications at one end of the scale, to facilitating sophisticated therapeutic interventions at the other. There is a plethora of theoretical data on the underlying causes and symptoms described as both pre-onset and post-onset of addiction and as many theories for interpreting them. In addition to this there is of course the view that addiction is a brain disease; not specifically related to any underlying causes other than a malfunction in brain chemistry, Erickson and Wilcox (2001); McCauley (2003). When applying the popular 12 Step approach used as a recovery tool in Alcoholics Anonymous and Narcotics Anonymous and a number of other selfhelp groups dealing with addiction the belief is held that there is no cause for the condition and that it is unproductive to look for things, or people, to blame. As one addict unfolds a particular pathological trait or past experience it is suggested that there will be a sober or drug free counterpart who will report no addiction to substances. However, even though the author acknowledges the success of the 12 Step philosophy and agrees that there are no single determinants or causes of the condition this study will focus on identified common traits in particular those pertinent to impoverished relationships. The study will then seek to identify any expectancy that the use of substances may relieve and compensate for the lack of intimacy. In addition the study will investigate the possible culpability of others when applying attachment theory as a framework for understanding intimacy and interpreting the results of the research. The expansion of addiction theory as a means of characterising problematic desires, feelings and behaviors is a common feature of modern psychological approaches to depression, stress and compulsive disorders (Lefever 2002). 1 the sample refer to themselves as addicts 3 The transference of the human need for intimacy into the attachment to form a relationship of sorts with potentially addictive objects has widened as can be seen by the expansion and growth of the self-help group, dealing with a wide range of addictions; gambling, shopping, food, exercise, smoking, drugs, alcohol, sex and many others. A common view of addiction is that it resides in the conflict between the desire for pleasure and release from stress/problems on the one hand and demands for control and performance on the other. Addictive desire is now attached to rigorous activities such as exercise and work (Keane (2004). She argues; “addiction is a dependence on external actions as a way of regulating one’s feelings and sense of self.” (p193); and ‘feelings’ are brought into this model through the notion that “it is our emotional frailties that make us susceptible to external mood modifiers”. (p194) The connection between addiction and intimacy as a common correlation features significantly in addiction recovery materials. Nakken (1998) argues that western thinking in modern society glorifies autonomy, self-reliance, self centeredness and consumerism. Nakken distinguishes between “natural relationships” to which people turn for support, love and growth, and addictive relationships in which people rely on objects such as alcohol or drugs, or even events such as sex or shopping to meet their emotional needs. Nakken argues that “addiction is a process of buying into false and empty promises: the promise of relief, the promise of emotional security, the false sense of fulfilment, and the false sense of intimacy with the world . . . Finding emotional fulfilment through an object or event is an illusion” (14–15.) Nakken’s account suggests that addicts have an intimate relationship of sorts with their drug of choice and insists; the formation of an intense and emotional bond with things is the essence of addiction. The DSM-IV manual used in the classification of dependency by many addiction treatment professionals does not have a diagnostic category concerned with intimacy although it does highlight links between intimacy and a number of disorders. There is though a wealth of empirical evidence that connects difficulties with intimacy as contributing to a number of psychological problems (Berman and Margolin, 1992). 4 According to Descutner and Thelen, 1991), fear of intimacy is a major risk factor for many emotional problems, including those associated with substance abuse. This thinking provides the theoretical framework for this enquiry. Theories on intimacy As there are many theories of addiction there also many authors and theories on intimacy. Erikson’s understanding of intimacy in Sharabany (1994) is more complex than the commonly held view of a simple equation of comfortable self disclosure and close relationships. He emphasises two intrapersonal components: the ability to make commitments to intimate relationships and the ability to develop the emotional strength necessary to maintain them. In order to do this there is a necessity to possess what he calls an ethical strength which allows the person to be emotionally secure enough to put self interest aside and to pay the cost of maintaining an intimate relationship in terms of being able to make personal sacrifices. When this theory is applied to addicts most theorists tend to leave out important factors that ought to be taken into account concerning post-onset detrimental effects. These are specifically though not exhaustively; the long term effects of continued drug use on social functioning and the idiosyncratic properties of specific drugs. For example, to include a cannabis or cocaine user in a research trial in the same category as a heroin or 2crack cocaine user with regard to their functioning in relationships may not be useful depending on the duration of their drug use. The properties of cocaine; a stimulant drug are often reported as enhancing performance in relationships specifically intimate encounters, (Morrison 2001), whereas opiate users will report having no interest in relationships or sex within a relatively short period of time using the drug. Taking into account that admissions into treatment for dependency include a wide range of substances it is clear that emotional and psychological effects can result from continued use of most drugs. However, it could be argued that issues relevant to relationships could be attributed to a particular drug lifestyle. For example, the short-term effects and resultant overwhelming urge to repeat use immediately Cocaine that has been heated with bicarbonate producing a small white lump or ‘rock’, hence the term ‘rocks’ is often used to described it. It produces a popping sound when smoked; hence the name crack. 2 5 following the dysphoric after effects off crack cocaine determines a lifestyle for the user which leaves little time for interaction with others, in fact according to Carnes (1983), “family and friends are abbreviated and sacrificed”. According to Morrison (2001), “With continued acute use the euphoric effects lessen and the dysphoric effects (anxiety, depression, fatigue) increase. Motivation diminishes to the point that the user is interested in only one thing: obtaining more.” (100). Unless the user is in a relationship with a person whose lifestyle mimics their own there is little time available for investing in the care needs of the other unless one is concerned with the shared acquisition of the drug. In any case the self-centred obsessive desire to focus only on the drug and its acquisition for personal use may even get in the way of sharing their drugs. In the case of heroin use, obsession with the drug and its acquisition is the same but the acute lack of any relational ties is often also apparent. (Pearson 1987). Taking into account Erikson’s view of intimacy being about displacing self interest and being able to pay the cost of personal sacrifice there appears to be little hope for addicts who have already formed their attachment to drugs to engage in this form of intimacy. The participants in this study were addicts whose drug use was problematic enough for them to have entered treatment, their ability to demonstrate self interest by this time would no doubt be seriously impaired. The importance of the capacity to be intimate has been discussed in clinical literature recently although it has been included less in the conceptualisation process of intimacy and addiction (Seginer and Noyman 2005). In order to develop therapeutic tools for the treatment of the psychological and emotional aspects of addiction discussions will need to include investigations into identifying reasons for the lack of intimacy in relationships as well as reasons for how the fear of intimacy develops. There may be emotional damage to start with due to unmet developmental needs resulting from poor parenting but as well as viewing addictions as an attempt to repair these deficiencies, the resultant compounding of those existing traits, or the deterioration of existing abilities need also to be considered. For example, shame and guilt features amongst addiction literature as it relates to relationship functioning. Lutwak et al (2003) believe that fear of intimacy is a direct correlate of shame on the sense of self. 6 Researchers have identified fear of intimacy as cause of the lack of intimacy however it could be due to a person simply lacking the tools to engage in close relationships even though the desire to engage is present. In this case the issue is not so much fear as an inadequacy or resource deficit. Beck and BeckGernsheim (1995) believe there are other reasons for poor functioning in relationships and the lack of intimacy generally. They believe that the process of individualisation has created a destructive glorification of intimacy where traditional bonds of religion, family and class have diminished and where the traditional norms and roles no longer determine a person’s behavior. According to these sociologists; individuals look to romantic love in a more self-seeking and self-centred way; to provide stability, meaning and a sense of self. Even if one were to give credence to this statement, if addicts do not possess the internal structure necessary to acquire romantic love their sense of self could never be defined in this way. As indicated by a review of the literature the most common precursors to difficulties with intimacy are attributed by some as to having strong implications on a person’s identity or to dysfunctional attachments in earlier childhood (Adams & Archer 1994, Hoefler and Kooyman 1996, Thorberg and Lyvers 2005). Those researchers who adopt Erikson’s descriptions conceptualise it in terms of the qualities of relationships (Craig-Bray & Adams, 1986). Orlofsky, (1993) and Orlofsky, Marcia, & Lesser (1973) undertook in-depth research using semi-structured interviews using five intimacy-status parameters which ranged from ‘isolation’ to ‘intimacy’. The continuity between the stages underlying the model were emphasised in support of the identity and intimacy model. In support of the identity-intimacy viewpoint Erikson (1968) argues, ‘‘It is only when identity formation is well on its way that true intimacy—which is really a counterpointing as well as a fusing of identities—is possible’’ (135). Accordingly, the formation of identity as a process focuses on the self and is a necessary condition for the development of intimacy as a process centring on interpersonal relationships. However, since intimacy is concerned with interacting with others whilst the main theme of identity is the self, it also contains relational aspects. Similarly whilst the main theme of intimacy is close relationship, it also contains aspects of self, so that 7 according to (Dyk & Adams, 1987; Grotevant & Cooper, 1986); identity and intimacy share common elements of individuality and connectedness. Studies that measured the relationship between identity and intimacy (Adams & Archer, 1994; Dyk & Adams, 1990) supported the link between them. These studies also showed that Erikson’s (1968) view that the identity –intimacy link is gender-related was not empirically supported (Seginer and Noyman 2005). Although not specifically related to the identity-intimacy link there was evidence in the study presented here to support a variance in gender differences resulting from the FIS tool. Intimacy can be analogous to the idea of addiction when viewed as the reliance on external factors to regulate our internal states of stress and discomfort. In other words, it is necessary, in order to feel emotionally balanced, to feel good about oneself in relation to those close to us. Addictions then are connections which impact upon persons inter and intra-personal dynamic experiences and which can then become destructive of other relationships. This creates a recurring situation so that it is often difficult for drug counsellors in a therapeutic setting to distinguish the origins of dysfunction in relationships as pre or post the onset of addiction. According to attachment theorists, the correlation for addiction to substances forming a self-soothing substitute behaviour resulting from the lack of adequate care or intimacy in childhood can be attributed as the primary desire before the desire for drugs. Adams & Archer (1994), Hofler and Kooyman (1996) and Thorberg and Lyvers (2005) assert that unmet needs to attach to a prominent figure; typically a parent and more specifically a mother, from childhood continue into adult life. Subsequently, if those needs for protection, security, affirmation and belonging are present yet the necessary tools or abilities to engage with and attach oneself to a significant other are not present, it is understandable that compensation may be sought in the use of drugs. These abilities and features of intimacy are referred to by Schaefer and Olson (1981), as openness, honesty, self-disclosure, care devotion, mutual attentiveness, commitment, surrender of control, dropping of defences and emotional attachment. Kohut and Wolfe (1978) write; “Individuals whose nascent selves have been insufficiently responded to will use any available stimuli to create a pseudo-excitement in order to ward off the painful feeling 8 of deadness that tends to overtake them………” (416). In their view, “the individual’s inability to obtain self-soothing via transmuting internalisations (234) is the reason they look for relief from those tensions in the use of substances. They argue that it is their inability to calm and soothe themselves. This leads them to look outside of themselves this means that for those who eventually discover drugs by whatever means will no doubt form attachments to them. Flores (1997) describes a phase prior to this; he argues that an addict’s life is dominated by a deficiency in which s/he would experience themselves as being empty and deprived. “They believe that if only another would supply them with this deprived ‘refuelling’, they could become whole and complete.” (235) His explanation continues to describe how they will always be disappointed because as adults they have not learned to move away from infantile interpretations of interaction in relationships. Such interpretations formed around basic beliefs that if a person disappoints us it is because they don’t love us become counterproductive to forming long term satisfying relationships. As such they become lonely and resentful and more importantly, they have no understanding of the part they play in the process. According to Flores (1997) and supported by Lefever (2000) due to the frustration endured as the result of continued unsuccessful relationships; perceiving others predominantly as withholding and rejecting, it can result in the denial of an addict’s own limitations and lead on to the denial of any need for relationships. Flores argues that until addicts are able to form positive relationships with others they remain vulnerable to addiction. This avoidance of need creates alienation from others and self and makes the notion of one addict helping another not only a success for 12 Fellowships in terms of promoting recovery but also assists in reversing the isolation of self from society. Isolation in this case is viewed as being a consequence of shame and one of the driving forces behind addiction. Flores (1997) asserts that the use of chemicals enhances the denial and fuels grandiose defences keeping an addict isolated but that the recovery process brought about by the12 Step programme reverses this process by requiring individuals to honestly admit that they do need substances to survive but more importantly; that their hope for future survival depends on the help of others. 9 In fig 1 below; adapted from Flores diagram (245), a simple diagram defines active addiction as the denial of the need for others leading to a denial of the need for drugs and recovery consequently leads from acceptance of the need for drugs leading to the acceptance of the need for others. Fig 1 1) Addiction 2) DENIAL OF NEED FOR OTHERS (“I don’t need anyone”) DENIAL OF NEED FOR ALCOHOL AND DRUGS (I am not addicted to alcohol or drugs”) 1st Step - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - ------------------------------------1) Recovery 2) ACCEPTANCE OF NEED FOR ALCOHOL AND DRUGS (“I am an alcohol or an addict”) ACCEPTANCE OF NEED FOR OTHERS (“I need other people”) Intimacy referred to in this paper is concerned with the ability to form and sustain close meaningful relationships which are important in terms of belonging to the social and cultural structures of the particular societies we live in. In addition the concept includes taking into account the emotional and psychological needs of a person to feel significant, loved and secure. Theories which include an investigation of the etiological aspect of these not only include attachment theorists but also merge into biological issues, issues of identity, and our more basic needs as identified by evolutionary theorists. Research into impoverished relationships amongst addicted populations has been widely discussed and there is clearly an interest with regard to intimacy and direct correlates to substance dependency. Thorberg and Lyvers (2005) investigated this is their study. They employed three questionnaires in their research presented in a paper; Attachment, fear of intimacy and differentiation of self among clients in substance disorder treatment facilities. The first questionnaire they used was the; Revised Adult Attachment Scale, (Collins 1996) which was designed to measure how comfortable the participants felt with closeness and intimacy. The second was the one used in this study, the Fear of Intimacy Scale (Descutner and Thelen, 10 1991) this was used to assess inhibition and capacity to disclose intimate feelings and thoughts to significant others. The third was the Differentiation of Self Inventory (Skowron and Friedlander, 1998) designed to evaluate individuals’ significant relationships. Their findings confirmed that addicts reported significantly higher levels of fear of intimacy than the controlled group in the study. Keene (2004) discusses the contemporary formulation of addiction as being related to unhealthy feelings about the self originating from childhood pain or neglect. Her views link with one of the four abilities required for intimate relationships; the ability to seek care, the ability to give care, the ability to feel comfortable with an autonomous self, and the ability to negotiate. Accordingly the connectedness necessary for intimacy in relationships is formed from a healthy attachment to the mother and subsequently to significant others throughout life. If this attachment is not cemented due for example to the mother not being available emotionally then the detached child focuses its need for care onto objects such as drugs. Expanding on theories of Addiction: Interpreting addiction as a kind of intimacy can be a useful way of making sense of the intense relationships that addicts develop with substances and with activities. Using an addictions perspective within Keane’s (2004) framework of disordered desire, it is not the objects of addiction that determine the disorder but an intense and rigid relationship between the addict and their drug. According to Keane (2004) the increasing acceptance of addiction as an all-purpose model for explaining unruly and troubling desires needs to be challenged. The study presented here is not attempting to provide an all inclusive meta-theory, nor challenge existing theory, rather there is arguably a need here for a paradigm to understanding intimacy which includes Keane’s perspective for understanding addiction and problems with intimacy but also draws on a range of other perspectives. The model of substance dependence set out in the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) provides a clinical diagnosis of addiction. This manual is often referred to by 11 addiction professionals to assist in distinguishing those substance misusers or abusers from those considered to be dependent or addicts. It is difficult for treatment professionals and researchers alike to distinguish between the two populations. There is often more clarity for alcoholism and alcohol abuse than for such drugs as heroin due to the addictive properties of the drug. DSM-IV defines dependence as a “maladaptive pattern of substance use, leading to clinically significant impairment or distress” and identifies major elements of the impaired control over use of a substance and harmful consequences such as health problems; financial, legal and employment difficulties; and strained family and social relations. Fig 2 (Koob and Moal 2000) Such elements are noted through behavioral patterns or subjective states—for example, taking a substance in larger amounts or over a longer period than intended; a persistent desire or unsuccessful effort to control use; a great deal of time spent in obtaining, using or recovering from use of a substance; and important social, occupational or recreational activities given up in favour of substance use. Significantly, while the DSM-IV states that withdrawal and tolerance are often present, these signs of physiological change are not necessary to make a diagnosis of dependence and are not found with some substances. This minimization or de-emphasis on biological elements (withdrawal and tolerance) in the definition of addiction goes against widely held views that it is the presence of “physical” as well as “psychological” dependence that marks a true addiction. 12 Disorders of Desire According to this theory addicts come to rely on experiences to achieve a desired state of being. They limit gratification from other sources usually brought about as the result of conforming to society’s norms and rules for success. Examples might include working hard to achieve exam success at school and college or achieving career promotions after dedicated commitment to work but might best be illustrated by a house buying scenario; saving for a mortgage; several years, getting a mortgage, and paying off a mortgage, around 25 years. By opting out of these life options due to over-involvement with a mood-altering activity in favour of achieving shorter term gratification serious social impairment occurs. Keane (2004) points out that the reliance on models of addiction to evaluate and interpret harm, feelings of compulsion and obsessive involvement with mood altering substances emphasises its inevitable enmeshment with cultural norms and ethical judgments about what people should value and what makes life meaningful. According to this view an addict’s priorities are profoundly disturbed and instead of caring about work or family or other ‘normal’ responsibilities they will devote themselves to the destructive pursuit of pleasure or oblivion. The addict in this scenario is the fundamental opposite of the ideal of the rational, productive and self-reliant adult. Ultimately then, addiction is a condition marked by caring too much about destructive and disruptive things and not enough about the constructive things that help us develop and advance in terms of fostering natural feel good factors. The modern term of addiction as a description of unhealthy feelings, (usually unhealthy feelings about the self originating in childhood pain or neglect), is a viewpoint that feeds into or expands the paradigm of addiction used here. When analysing addiction with regard to feelings; levels of contentment, versus depressed, anxiety and stress versus feeling at ease with self and others or feeling ill versus at ease in the world, then the pattern of drug use and the nature of consumption becomes much less relevant to the condition and its treatment. 13 Medical experts have also come to emphasise feelings as having important significance for addiction. Psychiatrist Aviel Goodman (1993) argues that “all addictions are manifestations of a single underlying pathology related to the inability to self-regulate emotional states”. Addiction then is a process in which behaviour that can produce pleasure and also provide escape from internal discomfort is used in an uncontrolled and harmful way. In other words, addiction becomes a dependence on things external as a way of regulating one’s feelings and sense of self. However, according to Leshner (1999) the emphasis on feelings does not signal the surrender of biology to psychology in the field of addiction. The dominant focus on feelings exists alongside increasingly powerful neurobiological explanations of how destructive attachments take hold of us (Wilcox 2001; McCauley 2003). Addictions have varying levels of scientific legitimacy and popular acceptance, according to Shaffer (1999). An important determining factor is how successfully a scientist can account for the effects of drugs in the brain that feature neurotransmitters and neural reward pathways. In neuroscience addictions are all evaluated in terms of chemical properties, even if no drugs are involved as in the case of gambling or sex addiction. Specific meanings and cultural interpretations of pleasurable activities are reduced to a scientific cause and effect model (Keane 2004). Thinking about addiction in this way however sub-human this approach may appear has a lot to offer studies on addiction. For example it removes all moral judgements and de-stigmatises addiction by explaining that addiction happens in mid-brain rather than in the pre-frontal cortex where such things as love, morality, decency, responsibility, spirituality, free will and conscious thought are understood and acted upon. According to McCauley (2006) the mid-brain is simply a way-station for incoming sensory information on its way to the cortex. He argues that moral issues of personality and behaviour and social learning variables can concur with addiction 14 but they cannot cause addiction, nor can addiction cause them. This is demonstrated in fig 3, 4, 5 following. Fig 3 The Frontal Cortex: Defective in addiction Fig 4 It does not handle Love Morality Decency Responsibility Spirituality Free will Conscious thought Mid-brain is a way-station for incoming sensory information on the way to the cortex Fig 5 The Drug becomes Survival at the level of the unconscious 15 Addictions trigger the release of serotonin, dopamine and or endorphins in the brain which manage our experiences of pleasure. The brain becomes dependent on internal fixes referred to as endogenous, just as it does on psychoactive drugs, exogenous. Our human need for stimulation may drive us to seek out pleasurable experiences; although in a healthy person not as an obsessive activity, but it cannot cause addiction. The compulsive behavior of the addict may work through the same mechanisms of neurotransmitter activation as other more socially acceptable rewarding activities but addiction is powered by a mid-brain where needs for survival take place. According to Goodman (1993) feelings enter the neurological model because of the belief that our emotional frailties make us susceptible to external mood modifiers but if one goes along with McCauley then susceptibility can only lead a person to try self-soothing or self-regulation, it is actually the brain that decides. Berlant’s (1998) description of intimacy as being similar to the idea of addiction in terms of the reliance on external factors to regulate our internal states described earlier does not bring any moral judgements. There are no derogatory descriptions or suggestions of any abnormality. Addictions here are connections which impact and transform but like all intimate bonds they can become destructive of other relationships. Also that dependency on others, including what Berlant calls, non-human others, is a condition of life and not necessarily an ailment to be cured. Thinking about addiction in this way allows us to discuss the disruptive potential of the neurological account of compulsion, which identifies valued and damaging pleasures as products of the same natural and necessary process. Acknowledging the prominence in modern society of ideals of physical and emotional independence and separation from family our needs for connection can be difficult to manage and relationships with people or things often go wrong. Addiction makes sense as a way to understand compulsive attachments in a cultural context which glorifies autonomy and self-reliance on one hand and the rewards of romantic love and material possessions on the other. This view clearly presents the high costs of losing self-control in a society where for the young, it is considered cool to let yourself go and party. 16 The move in the last decade to understand all addictive disorders as fundamentally the same in their structure and etiology can obscure the complex and often contradictory nature of the types of consumption. In addition, it does not capture the numerous forms that failure to conform to these norms can take. Nevertheless, models of addiction, whether based on neurotransmitters or theories of intimacy, have the potential to be useful critical resources. They can render irrelevant the distinctions between normal and abnormal and natural and unnatural behaviour or give credence to addiction as an ethical and political force. Attachment Theory The application of attachment theory to this study of adult drug dependant close relationships provides a framework for understanding feelings and behaviours. Working models (Bowlby 1973) are central to this approach. They are presumed to guide how people operate in relationships and how they interpret their social world. The present research directly explores this by incorporating attachment style into a paradigm that also includes other models of intimacy concerned with the developmental processes and patterns that affect feelings and behavior. This study was designed to provide insight into the inhibitors of intimacy in addicts and their attempts to self-repair attachment theory provides a valuable framework for pursuing this. Attachment theory is used here as a framework for understanding and interpreting the data in the study. According to Collins (1996), every situation we meet with in life is constructed in terms of the representational models we have of the world about us and of ourselves. Information reaching us is selected and interpreted in terms of those models, its significance for us and for those we care about is evaluated in terms of them, and responses to situations are made with these models in mind. According to Bowlby (1980), how we interpret and evaluate each situation determines how we feel. Thus, when adults engage in new relationships, they carry with them a catalogue of past experiences both personal and interpersonal. These experiences affect how they think, feel and behave in relationships. According to attachment theory 17 frameworks for interpreting intimacy begin to develop during early parent-child interactions and are then carried forward into adult relationships. They then become a mind-map for how individuals manage their relationships and view the world around them. Bowlby (1973) coined the term working models to describe internal representations of the world and significant people, including the self that begin in infancy. Thus models are largely formed by the emotional caregiver's availability and responsiveness to a child's needs. Whether infants’ attempts to gain comfort and security are met determines the decisive classification of secure, avoidant or preoccupied, within this theory. Cassidy (2001) argues that ‘secure attachment’ facilitates the ability to form intimate relationships and to seek care and that models that begin their development in childhood are likely to remain influential in adult life. This is because over time, experiences are absorbed as beliefs and expectations about the warmth and responsiveness of others and about the worth of self. These beliefs are then used to predict and interpret the behaviour of others and to behave in old ways in new situations without first evaluating them as unique experiences. Secure adults are those described as comfortable with intimacy, willing to rely on others when needed and confident and accepting of being loved and valued. Avoidant adults on the other hand are those who are uncomfortable getting close to people and depending on others although they tend to be unconcerned about whether others will accept or reject them. The preoccupied adults possess a strong desire for close relationships but generally experience difficulty depending on others and, according to Cassidy, also tend to worry a great deal about being rejected and abandoned. Other studies showed that adults with different attachment styles differed greatly in the way they viewed themselves and the world around them. Collins and Read (1990) found that people with a more secure attachment style were higher in selfworth, more confident in social situations, and more self-assertive. Secure adults also had more positive beliefs about the world around them and generally viewed others as trustworthy, dependable, and well intentioned. Those with a more preoccupied attachment style had low self-worth, self-confidence, and 18 assertiveness and their view of others was much less positive and trusting. They also believed that people had little control over their lives, and they viewed others as complex and difficult to understand. Avoidant adults tended to have positive views of themselves; high self-worth and assertiveness, although they saw themselves as less confident in social situations and were not confident in interpersonal encounters. They also had negative views of human nature; not trusting others as trustworthy or dependable. Studies generally found that secure adults reported more positive relationship experiences than preoccupied or avoidant adults. Secure individuals generally described their relationships as intimate, stable, and satisfying. In contrast, avoidant adults tended to report low levels of intimacy, commitment, and satisfaction, whereas preoccupied individuals reported jealousy, conflict, and high levels of negative emotional experiences. Peoples’ emotional responses and response patterns play a central role in attachment theory. Cassidy (2001) argues that secure attachment is associated with the capacity to participate in successful intimate relationships and that four key abilities are required for participation in intimate relationships. These are; the ability to seek care, to give care, to feel comfortable with an autonomous self, and the ability to negotiate and choose the level of intimacy one intends to engage in. Accordingly, intimacy is sharing and making one’s innermost self known as well as accepting the innermost self of another. “It is being able to tell both the good and bad parts of oneself, to tell of anger, ambivalence, love; and to accept both the good and the bad parts of another, to accept anger, ambivalence, love. It is to share the self: one’s excitements, longings, fears and neediness, and to hear of these in another”. (122) Thus, in order to be intimate a person must be capable of care- seeking (123). This means that a person must be able to turn to appropriately selected others effectively when needed. This skill is learned by secure children who experience parents as loving responsive and supportive. A second requirement is to be able to trust that the self is loveable; determined by being valued in childhood. (Research undertaken by Sroufe (1977) showed higher self-esteem for pre- 19 school children who were positively attached to their mothers than those who were not) Consequently, positive experiences further the capacity for intimacy by making children confident enough to seek care because they feel worthy of receiving care. (Cassidy 2001) Secure individuals will therefore bring a set of expectations to new relationships that are conducive to allowing intimacy to be negotiated and if chosen, to be developed. The adaptive approach The scope of theory related to drug dependency relates to diverse processes in diverse areas, from tolerance and sensitivity in the dopamine system to the effect of attachment on life’s coping strategies. The adaptive approach can provide a useful analytical framework for examining specific underlying effects of addiction. For example, with the dopamine system, the difference between independent functioning and adaptive functioning in addition to the importance of the environment in shaping the functional design of the system both come from adaptationist thinking. (Lende and Smith 2002) According to Lende and Smith (2002) an evolutionary approach can help in developing our understanding of certain therapeutic interventions, for example, therapies that deal with initial resistance to treatment due to deviance or denial such as motivational interviewing (Miller & Rollnick 1991) are likely to be effective. They need to be followed by more focused interventions which help to develop better self-regulation and longer-term behavioral and life strategies. The strongest submission in their studies is that; “until interventions deal with the adaptive nature of the short-term life strategies individuals have developed, risk for substance abuse—given how drugs signal immediate salience—will remain high as individuals continue to seek short-term pay-offs” (455.) They examine three components to drug use and abuse; a biological mechanism comparable to McCauley’s brain disease model, a developmental course equivalent to the attachment model and a social development concept based on issues such as dominance, submission and social dependence. Their paper outlines how attachment affects self-regulation and examines also how inequality affects drug 20 abuse. From this perspective issues of social dependence and manipulative behaviors play a role in relationships with drugs. The paper concludes with an analysis of how the adaptive approach applies to interventions to treat addictive behavior. The impact of environment Peele (1985) stressed that addiction relates to an experience constructed jointly from pharmacological, individual, and socio-cultural sources. It is an experience that is the interactive product of social learning involving physiological events as they are interpreted, labelled and given meaning by the individual (Lindesmith 1968). The traditional view of the way to develop a successful future is to emphasise restraint in terms of pleasure seeking in favour of a long-term investment for security in later life. This conventional view offers few immediately or short-term wins. For those with poor attachment whose world is not a secure place it may make more sense to them to maximise short-term benefits. The immediate short term effects of drugs may arguably offer the soothing, stimulating, and motivating properties experienced by those experiencing the eventual rewards for their restraint. Negative emotional states like anxiety and interpersonal conflicts, feelings of frustration, anger and social pressure can create an environment where drugs are believed to be a positive amelioration of negative internal and interpersonal feelings. In this context, the person desires the state drugs seem to provide even if the ensuing state is actually not rewarding (Marlatt & Gordon 1985). Attachment and regulation Problems in parent and child attachment are related to drug use and abuse through the developmental impact on self-regulation (Brook, Whiteman and Finch 1993). Parents are supposed to help children to modulate emotional states and reduce internal tension (Cicchetti et al. 1996). This is adaptive because via the parent it allows a child to match environmental patterns as well as safeguarding against possible failure and the adverse psychological effects caused if these are not addressed and remedied. (Hofer 1994). Parents who lack sensitivity and 21 appropriate reaction may fail to help to reduce tension and stress. In these unconstructive attachment situations, emotional regulation involves inherent trade-offs and “…makes non-optimal strategies of managing emotion expectable, perhaps inevitable, in a context of difficult environmental demands and conflicting emotional goals’ (Thompson and Calkins 1996 - 179). According to this theory, when confronted by insensitive and damaging care children generally manage to regulate on their own without the emotional support parents ought to provide. According to (Brown 1998) though, this can often result in a fragile regulatory system, which is a major risk factor for future substance abuse. This can happen through poor emotion and attention regulation as well as difficulties with self-consciousness, according to (Lende and Smith 2002) the “addictive experience provides regular, stimulating and controlling effects and compromised homeostatic systems can reorient around drug consumption” . Thus, a person with deficient self-regulation due to poor parental care can find in drugs what has been missing, but according to Peele (1985); “none the less was evolutionarily expected”. (452) Drug use and deviance Within evolution theory, the life history model predicts that children with less access to resources are at an evolutionary disadvantage and will mature more rapidly. Adolescents facing conditions of underprivilege “will take greater risks to gain an immediate evolutionary advantage.” They argue that this risk-taking includes using drugs but also consists of “expensive or dangerous adult-like behaviours such as early reproduction, high-risk resource acquisition; theft, ……in other words, general deviance” (Wilson and Daly 1985- 71). Early attachment problems then can set up this developmental path which places great emphasis on immediate gains. These problems lead to adolescents adopting short-term strategies and becoming involved in more mature and or untraditional behaviors for resource acquisition. These behaviours are often enmeshed with sub-cultural values around peer affirmation and popularity and ‘street-cred’. Experimenting with drugs can form a part of this deviant behaviour. Often young adults are compromised in their ability to handle the high-risk behaviors they have become dependant upon for survival. Under these 22 conditions young adults focus on immediate issues with the resultant effect of high vulnerability to deviance and to drug use. Drugs often offer an escape from negative and depressive feelings or difficult stressful situations (Cooper et al. 1995). In general individuals do not engage consciously in dependent behavior with drugs, but rather fall into it as they personalise the relationship with the drug and project learned dependent patterns onto it. As addicts build a close relationship with a drug without recognising the negative effects of the relationship, they will focus on the personal benefits of drug use such as the release from depressive feelings. This argument is supported by three assumptions: (Bourgois 1995); that in some cases drug dependency is related to unequal social and economic conditions; that people have the ability to displace patterns learned from interpersonal behaviours onto objects (Krystal 1994) and that addicts tend to personalise their relationships with drugs, often through symbolism (Anderson 1994). Thus, when addicts lend human qualities to drugs and project patterns of behaviour from other areas of their life onto them, their behaviour will be built upon, and will continue to develop on, the very foundation of dependence. Expectancy Theory People generally begin to experiment with drugs because they expect to have a positive experience. Those experiences range from heightened feelings of euphoria to peace and calm concerning present circumstances. In order for a person to identify drugs as something that will reduce their resource deficit in relationships they must first encounter the drug. Few people come to their first drug experience from a position of ignorance; children have already acquired definite expectations about alcohol, for example, long before they ever drink it (Orford 2001). Social Learning theorists refer to the acquisition of new knowledge and behaviours by observing a model as a rapid and highly efficient way of producing new learning and picking up complex social behaviours and cultural norms. Peer group influence and experimentation in early stages of drug use before the neuro-psychological processes take a hold and addiction is formed is often neglected according to Sargent (1992), “Few writers appear to have 23 recognised that pleasure is a main motivation for the use of drugs, for they are too busy seeking a deficit in the individual…….” (74). Self-regulation can be defined as a person’s capacity to regulate their own behaviour by using internal standards and self-evaluative assessments. It can explain why behaviour continues despite a lack of external environmental rewards. In the process of self-regulation humans make self rewards depending on the achievement of a specific internal standard. If there is a discrepancy or even a perceived discrepancy between internal standards and performance then an individual will be motivated to change either the standard or the behaviour or both. Addicts could be described as having a great deal of self-regulation. A large part of their time and effort is devoted to obtaining the drug, using it and concealing its use from others. Self-regulating could also be equated with coping. Some addicts may use drugs or alcohol in order to cope or self-regulate themselves with internal standards that are difficult to cope with due to past trauma. According to social learning theorists, the person, the behaviour, and the environment are all thought to be continually interacting and influencing and being influenced by each other. Each of the individual components can change the nature of the interactions at any time, and in different situations each of these components can assume dominance. White et al 1990 have outlined an interactive model in fig 6 which explains alcohol consumption from a social learning theory perspective. Fig 6 PERSON SOCIAL ENVIRONMENT Affective states, Cognitive intellectual Proximal Influences: Family, Friends, capacity, Genetic factors, Coping Colleagues. resources, Values, Goals Distal Influences: Socio-cultural group membership, mass media institutions DRINKING BEHAVIOUR ALCOHOL Pharmacological actions, Perceived actions and experiences 24 According to social learning theory self-efficacy expectations are primarily cognitive mediators that determine whether or not a person will engage in a particular coping response. If self-efficacy regarding a particular coping skill is high they will be more likely to use it in an attempt to cope with life. But if it is low the person will be more likely use some other coping strategy with which they feel more comfortable. Self-efficacy is a perception or a judgement of one’s capacity to execute a particular course of action required to deal effectively with an impending situation (Abrams and Niaura, 1987). There are two components of self-efficacy: outcome expectations and efficacy expectations. An outcome expectation is an estimate that a particular outcome will occur. An efficacy expectation is a person’s belief that they can carry out the necessary course of action to obtain the anticipated outcome. Social learning theory views substance dependency as a basic failure of coping (Abrams and Niaura 1987). This could be due to a combination of inappropriate conditioning, reinforcement contingencies, modeling of inappropriate behaviours, failure to model appropriate coping skills and reduced self-efficacy with regard to behaviours that enhance coping. Ellis (1988) believed that an adolescent’s decision to experiment with drugs was influenced by beliefs perceived by the peer group and that lack of self-efficacy could lead to difficulty in refusing. He believed that negative emotions based on irrational beliefs together with an inability to tolerate frustration or other negative emotions set the stage for using drugs as a means of coping with negative emotions Fig 7 Webb et al 1993: The Relationship among Social Factors, Expectancies and Alcohol Use SOCIAL FACTORS Peer influence, Parental attitudes EXPECTANCIES ALCOHOL USE INTRAPERSONAL FACTORS Tolerance of deviance, Sensation seeking 25 Methodology Design Two questionnaires are used in the study. To establish the variance in impoverished intimate relationships in the participant compared to the controlled group both groups completed Descutner and Thelen’s Fear of Intimacy Scale, (FIS). This tool has been validated as a reliable test/re-test measurement of individuals’ anxieties about close dating relationships. In their study Descutner and Thelen (1991) compared the self-report data from the questionnaires with the individuals’ therapists’ ratings about their clients’ fear of intimacy. A second questionnaire; Emotional Self- soothing Expectancy Scale, (ESES) was specifically designed for this study. This tool was completed only by the participant group to identify any expectations that drugs would provide a soothing substitute for intimate relationships. The results were also intended to contribute to the debate on why addicts turn to or form attachments to substances. Both groups were given the materials to complete in their own time. The participants completed theirs on the treatment facility premises in order to register their details in the office to ensure receipt of the store voucher given for taking part. The controlled group were more leisurely in their response; the time calculated for all returns was circa three and a half weeks. Participants The participants were 24 substance dependant clients attending abstinence based recovery services using facilitated 12 Step programmes in Bradford West Yorkshire. Invitations to take part in the research were issued by staff working at the relevant facilities. There was also a discussion relating to the purposes of the investigation and issues of anonymity and confidentiality were explained. Participants were asked to give only their initials and date of birth under the Personal Details section of the questionnaire. Full names were retained against initials by the treatment facility staff in order that store vouchers could be distributed on completion. The group comprised 12 male and 12 female and ages ranged from 23 to 43 with a mean age of 30.5 years. Seventeen participants described themselves as being White/British, 1 as White/Irish, 1 Mixed Race Asian, 1 Mixed Race Afro/Caribbean, 1 Asian British, 1 Asian, 2 as Afro 26 Caribbean. Six had been using drugs for 0-5 years, 10 for 5-10 years and 8 for 10-15 years. (See fig 35) Heroin and Crack Cocaine were the most commonly used drugs; 21 of the 24 reported using Heroin as their first drug. Of those; 19 also used Crack Cocaine. Fifteen reportedly used Cannabis; 11 reported Ecstasy (MdMa) use, 8 Amphetamine, 3 Cocaine, 6 Alcohol, 3 Benzodiazepines, (Diazepam), and one person reported non-prescribed Methadone as ‘drug use’. No one reported as using only 1 drug; 1 person reportedly used 6 drugs. Fig 8 Type of Drugs Used Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Cannabis Cannabis Cannabis MdMa Amphetamine Cannabis MdMa MdMa MdMa MdMa Amphetamine Amphetamine Amphetamine Cannabis Cannabis Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Heroin Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Crack Amphetamine Alcohol Cocaine Alcohol Methadone Alcohol Cannabis Cannabis Cannabis Cannabis Cannabis Cannabis Cannabis MdMa MdMa Amphetamine Amphetamine Benzodiazepines Benzodiazepines MdMa MdMa Cocaine Alcohol Amphetamine Alcohol Alcohol Cannabis MdMa Cannabis MdMa Cocaine Benzodiazepines The controlled group of 24 had professional and semi-professional backgrounds with no self-reported history of substance misuse problems. They were similar in age and gender to the participant group and approximately 50% of the group worked in positions associated with drug dependency; the remainder were friends and colleagues enlisted by them. Ages in this group ranged from 19 to 45 with a mean age of 31.25 years. Eighteen described themselves as White British, 3 as Afro/Caribbean British, 2 as Asian and 1 as Mixed Race Asian. 27 Apparatus Two questionnaires were used in the study. Both the participants and the controlled group completed ‘QUESTIONNAIRE 1’. ‘QUESTIONNAIRE 2’ was only completed by the participants. In order to anonymise the information, a table was attached asking for personal details; initials, date of birth, gender, and ethnicity. The participants were asked for information on substance use history and types of drugs used. The controlled group were asked for information regarding their professional status. A letter to both groups was also attached to the questionnaire/s explaining the procedure and purposes of the study. Questionnaire 1 represented Part A and Part B of Descutner and Thelen’s 1991 Fear of Intimacy Scale, (FIS). In addition a second questionnaire was completed by the participants; the 3Emotional Self-soothing Expectancy Scale, (ESES). This was used to identify any expectations that the use of substances would provide a substitute for intimate relationships. The titles of the questionnaires were not revealed to the groups. This was to prevent any possible influencing of responses. The title of the second questionnaire; Emotional Self-soothing Expectancy Scale was particularly suggestive of the enquiry and it was anticipated, may have affected responses. The FIS tool was used because of its popular use as a research instrument. Items on the questionnaire were developed from other extensively used test measures. Eight items were included from Holt (1977) and two modified from Erickson’s subscales, (Descutner and Thelen 1991). Procedure Invitations to take part in completing the self reporting questionnaires were issued by staff working in the respective treatment facilities. The participants used in this study had previously taken part in research from a number of universities. It was explained that information would be anonymised. They were given a £5 store voucher because previous researchers had obliged in this way, the controlled group were also given a voucher. Both groups were informed that they would be given access to the finished paper. 3 designed and so named specifically for this study 28 A letter attached to the questionnaire explained the purpose of the study and the questionnaires provided instructions for completion. The FIS questionnaire was in two parts; Part A and B. Part A comprised questions 1-30 and Part B 31-35. 4Part A Instructions: [were as follows] “Imagine you are in a close, dating relationship. Respond to the following statements as you would if you were in that close relationship. Rate how characteristic each statement is of you on a scale of 1 to 5 as described below, and put your responses on the answer sheet. Part B Instructions: Respond to the following statements as they apply to your past relationships. Rate how characteristic each statement is of you on a scale of 1 to 5 as described in the instructions for Part A. Note. In each statement “O” refers to the person who would be in the close relationship with you”. Fig 9 1 not at all characteristic of me 2 Slightly characteristic of me 3 moderately characteristic of me 4 very characteristic of me 5 extremely characteristic of me The participant group were asked to follow the same procedure for the second questionnaire (ESES). This was designed to measure the expectancies of the effects that came with the use of drugs. The questions were designed to disclose the expectation that use of substances would substitute for the poor quality of close relationships. The questionnaire had 30 questions; 7 were red herrings designed to distract from a conscious and immediate connection with any intended outcome. These seven are shown here in fig. 10: Fig 10 2. When I take drugs I like listening to music 7. When I take drugs I am not interested in going clothes shopping. 11. I take drugs because my friends take them 14. When I take drugs I am not interested in reading the papers or watching the news on TV. When I take drugs I bathe less I try to sort my problems out when I take drugs I feel less lazy when I take drugs 16. 23. 30. 4 Instructions taken from the Fear of Intimacy Scale tool cited in Descutner and Thelen 1991 29 Of the remaining 23 questions 10 were correlated with questions in the FIS; Fig 11 FIS QUESTIONS 1 I would feel uncomfortable telling 0 about things in the past that I have felt ashamed of. 2 I would feel uneasy talking with 0 about something that has hurt me deeply. 4 If 0 were upset I would sometimes be afraid of showing that I care. 5 9 11 12 15 24 28 I might be afraid to confide my innermost feelings to 0. A part of me would be afraid to make a long-term commitment to 0. I would probably feel nervous showing 0 strong feelings of affection. I would find it difficult being open with 0 about my personal thoughts. I would be afraid to take the risk of being hurt in order to establish a closer relationship with 0. I would be afraid that I might not always feel close to 0. I would sometimes feel uncomfortable listening to 0's personal problems. ESES Questions 21 When I take drugs I expect to be able to cope with feelings of shame and guilt associated with things from my past 24 When I take drugs I expect to feel more comfortable telling my experiences, even sad ones, to O 5 When I take drugs I expect to be more able to show those close to me that I care when they are upset 1 When I take drugs I expect to be less afraid to confide my innermost feelings to people 13 When I take drugs I expect to be less afraid to make a long-term commitment in a relationship. 9 When I take drugs I expect to feel more at ease telling 0 that I care about him/her. 22 When I take drugs I expect to find it easier being open with 0 about my personal thoughts. 15 When I take drugs I am less afraid of rejection in relationships. 25 8 When I take drugs I expect to be able to have a feeling of complete togetherness with 0. When I take drugs I expect to be comfortable discussing significant problems with 0. Results When the FIS total scores for the participant group and the controlled group were added together there was only a small variance in score of 50. This was due to some of the items in the Scale being marked with an X to show they had been reversed and therefore produced a negative score. An X item, for example; “I would feel comfortable telling O…………” or; a non-X question “I would be afraid of telling O………….” By adding the total scores of positive and negative responses the effect was that overall the scores evened themselves out with only a variance in score of 50. The participant group reported high scores on the items reporting ‘poor intimacy’; the non-X items, and low scores on ‘good intimacy’ indicators the X items. Conversely the controlled group scored low on ‘poor intimacy’ and high on ‘good intimacy. When the X items were scored separately and excluded the non-X, the score variance increased to 466. 30 Fig 12 FIS RESULTS: Total score variance, participant group and controlled group X and Non-X total variance Questions: 1-30 1018 Questions: 31-35 194 Total: 1-35 Total scores for FIS: Participant Group: Female Male Total 970 1988 202 396 1172 2384 Total scores for FIS: Controlled Group: Female Male Total 1074 1084 2158 141 135 276 1215 1219 2434 Scores: X questions, Participants: Female Male Total 528 480 1008 * * * 1008 Scores: X questions, Controlled: Female Male Total 742 732 1474 * * * 1474 Scores: non-X questions, Participants: Female Male Total 490 490 980 194 202 396 684 692 1376 332 352 684 141 135 276 473 487 960 Scores: non-X questions, Controlled: Female Male Total * All 31-35 items were non-X Variant 1212 50 466 416 The X items pointed to positive feelings, attributes and behaviours. For example; X19 “I would feel comfortable trusting O with my deepest thoughts and feelings.” On this item the difference in score was 40; participant group 56 and controlled group 96. Item X22 also had a 40 score difference; “I would be comfortable with having a close emotional tie between us.” Participant group 66 controlled group 106. (See fig 18). The scores for the non-X items indicating poor functioning in relationships, showed a variance of 416. This provided evidence to support the hypothesis that the drug dependant group, (participants) had more impoverished relationships than the controlled group. Because the non-X and X items signify opposites they have been presented separately. Fig 13 and 14 represent the non-X items of Part A and Part B of the FIS. (Part B does not contain any X items). They show the variance in score for 31 each question based on the totals for each category; female participant, male participant, female controlled group, male controlled group. Fig 17 represents the X items contained in Part A. (Tables show total variance in descending order). Fig 13 11. 15. 2. 4. 13. 9. 5. 28. 1. 12. 20. 23. 24. 26. 16. FIS RESULTS: Variance; participant group and controlled group, non-X items in descending order – (Part A) Pg Pg Pg Cg Cg Cg TOTAL TOTAL F M F M I would probably feel nervous showing 37 33 70 19 20 39 0 strong feelings of affection. 37 38 75 23 23 46 I would be afraid to take the risk of being hurt in order to establish a closer relationship with 0. 38 35 73 22 23 45 I would feel uneasy talking with 0 about something that has hurt me deeply. 30 25 55 12 15 27 If 0 were upset I would sometimes be afraid of showing that I care. 30 32 62 17 17 34 I would feel uneasy with 0 depending on me for emotional support. 34 34 68 20 25 45 A part of me would be afraid to make a long-term commitment to 0. 35 35 70 26 23 49 I might be afraid to confide my innermost feelings to 0. 25 27 52 16 18 34 I would sometimes feel uncomfortable listening to 0's personal problems. 35 43 78 28 33 61 I would feel uncomfortable telling 0 about things in the past that I have felt ashamed of. 34 27 61 21 23 44 I would find it difficult being open with 0 about my personal thoughts. 25 33 58 17 24 41 I would sometimes feel uneasy if 0 told me about very personal matters. 33 25 58 23 19 42 I would be afraid of sharing my private thoughts with 0. 36 30 66 26 26 52 I would be afraid that I might not always feel close to 0. 27 34 61 22 27 49 I would be afraid that 0 would be more invested in the relationship than I would be. 34 39 73 40 36 76 I would feel comfortable keeping very personal information to myself. TOTAL Fig 14 35. 33. 34. 32. 31. 490 490 980 332 352 Variance 31 29 28 28 28 23 21 18 17 17 17 16 14 12 <3 684 296 Variance results; non-X questions in descending order – (Part B) (REFERENCE TO PAST RELATIONSHIPS) I have done things in previous relationships to keep me from developing closeness. There are people who think that I am afraid to get close to them. There are people who think that I am not an easy person to get to know. I have held back my feelings in previous relationships. I have shied away from opportunities to be close to someone. TOTAL TOTAL BOTH COLUMNS; NON-X 39 39 78 23 24 47 31 39 41 80 25 30 55 25 34 35 69 22 23 45 24 43 45 88 37 28 65 23 39 42 81 34 30 64 17 194 202 396 141 135 276 120 684 692 1376 473 487 960 416 32 The highest variance, (fig 15) was for item 11; total pg >31. Pgf, >37, cgf >19, pgm >33, cgm >20. The cgf feel the least nervous showing strong feelings and affection. The cgf would feel the most nervous. Fig 15 11. I would probably feel nervous showing 0 strong feelings of affection. Pg F Pg M TOTAL Pg Cg F Cg M TOTAL Cg 37 33 70 19 20 39 Variance 31 The surprising anomaly was item 16 which is arguably ambiguous. This produced a contra value of <3 on the total score. Cgf >40, pgf 34, pgm 39, cgm 36. The scores indicate that the cgf would feel most comfortable keeping personal information to themselves and the pgf would feel the least comfortable. Since this is included in the negative measures; non-X questions, it appears as though this should be counted as a poor intimacy indicator. However it could be argued that this question could be interpreted as having a healthy and positive implication and in the context of the consistency of the variance of the remaining items it has not been interpreted as demonstrating a poor intimacy indicator by the controlled group. Fig 16 16. Fig 17 X19. X22 X7. X29 X17 X10 X21 X27 X18 X30 I would feel comfortable keeping very personal information to myself. 34 39 73 40 36 76 <3 Variance; participant group and controlled group, X questions in descending order – (Part A) Variance Pg Pg Pg Cg Cg Cg F M TOTAL F M TOTAL I would feel comfortable trusting 0 with my deepest thoughts and 30 26 56 46 50 96 40 feelings. 35 31 66 54 52 106 40 I would be comfortable with having a close emotional tie between us. 28 29 57 47 49 96 39 I would have a feeling of complete togetherness with 0. 38 28 66 55 50 105 39 I would feel at ease to completely be myself around 0. 35 26 61 48 50 98 37 I would not be nervous about being spontaneous with 0. 33 34 67 51 51 102 35 I would feel comfortable telling my experiences, even sad ones, to 0. 33 29 62 47 50 97 35 I would be comfortable revealing to 0 what I feel are my shortcomings and handicaps. 36 38 74 55 54 109 35 I would feel comfortable about having open and honest communication with 0. 31 33 64 51 46 97 33 I would feel comfortable telling 0 things that I do not tell other people. 39 41 80 56 54 110 30 I would feel relaxed being together and talking about our personal goals. 33 X6. X25 X8. X3. X14 I would feel at ease telling 0 that I care about him/her. I would be comfortable telling 0 what my needs are. I would be comfortable discussing significant problems with 0. I would feel comfortable expressing my true feelings to 0. I would not be afraid to share with 0 what I dislike about myself. TOTAL 36 34 70 51 48 99 29 38 26 64 46 43 89 25 37 38 75 48 50 98 23 38 36 74 47 42 89 15 41 31 72 40 43 83 11 528 480 1008 742 732 1474 466 Variance in score for positive indicators was more significantly marked; items X19 and X 22 showed a variance of 40. Item X19; pgt 56, cgt 96. Cgm >50, cgf >46, pgf >30, pgm >26. Cgm were most comfortable trusting O with their deepest thoughts and feelings, pgm was the least comfortable. Item X22; pgt 66, cgt 106. Cgf >54, cgm >52, pgf >35, pgm >31. Cgf were most comfortable having a close emotional tie, pgm were the least comfortable. Fig 18 X19. X22 I would feel comfortable trusting 0 with my deepest thoughts and feelings. I would be comfortable with having a close emotional tie between us. Pg F Pg M Pg TOTAL Cg F Cg M Cg TOTAL Variance 30 26 56 46 50 96 40 35 31 66 54 52 106 40 Item X14 showed the lowest variance for total score; 11. Here the controlled group males scored highest indicating they would not be afraid to share with their partner what they disliked about themselves. However, the participant group females scored 1 point higher than cgf and pgm scored significantly lower with a score of 31 against the highest of 43; attributing most of the variance to pgm. Fig 19 X14 I would not be afraid to share with 0 what I dislike about myself. 41 31 72 40 43 83 11 Regarding Erikson’s assertion (1968) concerning gender variation; results of this study did show a difference in scores, particularly in response to certain questions. 34 Fig 20 FIS RESULTS: Total score gender variance, pg and cg; X and Non-X Questions: 1-30 1018 Questions: 31-35 194 Total: 1-35 Total scores gender variance pg: Female Male Total 970 1988 202 396 1172 2384 Total scores gender variance cg: Female Male Total Gender variance X questions, pg: Female Male Total Gender variance X questions, cg: Female Male Total Gender variance non-X questions, pg: Female Male Total Gender variance non-X questions, cg: Female Male Total Fig 21 1074 1084 2158 48 65 53 13 24 28 33 41 24 37 1474 194 202 396 332 352 684 53 1008 * * * 490 490 980 87 1215 1219 2434 * * * 742 732 1474 M 1212 141 135 276 528 480 1008 Variant F 684 692 1376 141 135 276 473 487 960 Variance in gender scores; female/male participant and controlled group Total scores variance Pgf Pgm 53 87 Cgf Cgm 48 65 >34 Gender variance X questions 53 <17 13 24 28 >40 Variance non-X questions 33 >4 41 24 37 <8 <13 Fig 22 - 31 set out variants for the participant group in descending order differentiating non-X from X items and male from female. Fig 22 1. 20. 26. 16. 13. 28. Gender Variance, pgm/pgf - non-X descending order (PART A) I would feel uncomfortable telling 0 about things in the past that I have felt ashamed of. I would sometimes feel uneasy if 0 told me about very personal matters. I would be afraid that 0 would be more invested in the relationship than I would be. I would feel comfortable keeping very personal information to myself. I would feel uneasy with 0 depending on me for emotional support. I would sometimes feel uncomfortable listening to 0's personal problems. Pg Gender variance Pg F Pg M TOTAL 35 25 43 33 78 58 M 8 8 27 34 61 7 34 39 73 4 30 25 32 27 62 52 2 2 F 15. I would be afraid to take the risk of being hurt in order to establish a closer relationship with 0. TOTAL I would be afraid of sharing my private thoughts with 0. I would find it difficult being open with 0 about my personal thoughts. I would be afraid that I might not always feel close to 0. If 0 were upset I would sometimes be afraid of showing that I care. I would probably feel nervous showing 0 strong feelings of affection. I would feel uneasy talking with 0 about something that has hurt me deeply. TOTAL 23. 12. 24. 4. 11. 2. 37 38 75 1 33 34 25 27 58 61 8 7 36 30 37 30 25 33 66 55 70 6 5 4 38 35 73 3 32 33 Fig 23 5. 9. I might be afraid to confide my innermost feelings to 0. A part of me would be afraid to make a long-term commitment to 0. 35 34 35 34 0 0 0 0 Items 5 and 9 showed no gender variance. On the non-X items there was only a one point difference for total scores, however when distinguished there were some significant variables; items 1, 20 and 23 had an 8point difference. This showed males to be more uncomfortable telling 0 about things in the past they felt ashamed of and more uneasy telling 0 about very personal matters. Conversely females were more afraid of sharing private thoughts with 0. (Part A) – relating to current relationships; non-X Of the non-X category only item15; (I would be afraid to take the risk of being hurt in order to establish a closer relationship with 0), had a difference in score of only (1>m); pgf >37, pgm >38. There was a difference of 2 (2>m) in the score for item 28 (I would sometimes feel uncomfortable listening to 0's personal problems), pgf >25 and pgm >27. There was also a 2 factor variance for item 13, (I would feel uneasy with 0 depending on me for emotional support), pgf >30, pgm >32; (2>m). Item 2 (I would feel uneasy talking with 0 about something that has hurt me deeply), was the only item with a variance to the factor of 3; pgf >38, pgm >35, (3>f). Item 11 (I would probably feel nervous showing 0 strong feelings of affection) varied to the factor of 4; pgf >37, pgm >33 (4>f). Items 4 and 16 varied to a factor of 5. Item 4; (If 0 were upset I would sometimes be afraid of showing that I care) showed pgf >30, pgm >25; (5>f). Item 16, (I would feel comfortable keeping very personal information to 36 myself) showed pgf >34, pgm >39; (5>m). Item 24 showed a 6 factor variance, (I would be afraid that I might not always feel close to 0), pgf >36, pgm >30; (6>f). Items 12 and 26 both confirmed a factor of 7. Item 12, (I would find it difficult being open with 0 about my personal thoughts), pgf >34, pgm >27; (7>f). Item 26, (I would be afraid that 0 would be more invested in the relationship than I would be), pgf >27, pgm >34; (7>m). (Part B) The total variance factor for Part B; relating to past relationships was 8 with the highest individual item factor of 3. All variance factors related to higher male scores; (8>m). Female participants reported only slightly less fear of intimacy for Part B items. Fig 24 31. 32. 33. 34. I have shied away from opportunities to be close to someone. I have held back my feelings in previous relationships. There are people who think that I am afraid to get close to them. There are people who think that I am not an easy person to get to know. I have done things in previous relationships to keep me from developing closeness. TOTALS 35. Fig 25 X25 X14 X29 X17 X19. X21 X22 X3. X6. X7. X8. 39 43 39 34 42 45 41 35 3 2 2 1 39 39 0 194 202 0 8 Gender Variance, pgf/pgm - X items descending order I would be comfortable telling 0 what my needs are. I would not be afraid to share with 0 what I dislike about myself. I would feel at ease to completely be myself around 0. I would not be nervous about being spontaneous with 0. I would feel comfortable trusting 0 with my deepest thoughts and feelings. I would be comfortable revealing to 0 what I feel are my shortcomings and handicaps. I would be comfortable with having a close emotional tie between us. I would feel comfortable expressing my true feelings to 0. I would feel at ease telling 0 that I care about him/her. TOTAL I would have a feeling of complete togetherness with 0. I would be comfortable discussing significant problems with 0. 37 Pg F Pg M Pg TOTAL variance 38 26 64 F 12 41 31 72 10 38 28 66 10 35 26 61 9 30 26 56 4 33 29 62 4 35 31 66 4 38 36 74 2 36 34 70 2 28 29 57 1 37 38 75 1 M 57 X10 X18 X27 X30 I would feel comfortable telling my experiences, even sad ones, to 0. I would feel comfortable telling 0 things that I do not tell other people. I would feel comfortable about having open and honest communication with 0. I would feel relaxed being together and talking about our personal goals. TOTAL 33 34 67 1 31 33 64 2 36 38 74 2 39 41 80 2 9 The variance was more notably marked for the X items denoting positive feelings attributes and behaviours. Females demonstrated more skills over males in their interactions in intimate relationships. On the Scale there were no zero factor scores for variance. Fig 26 shows X7, X8 and X 10 as reporting v ~ 1; (1>m). Fig 26 X7. X8. X10 I would have a feeling of complete togetherness with 0. I would be comfortable discussing significant problems with 0. I would feel comfortable telling my experiences, even sad ones, to 0. 28 37 33 29 38 34 1 1 1 Fig 27 notes v ~ 2 for items X3, X6, X18, X27, and X30. X3 and X6 = pgf >2; (4>f). X18, X27, X30 = pgm >2; (6>m). Fig 27 X3. X6 X18 X27 X30 I would feel comfortable expressing my true feelings to 0. I would feel at ease telling 0 that I care about him/her I would feel comfortable telling 0 things that I do not tell other people. I would feel comfortable about having open and honest communication with 0. I would feel relaxed being together and talking about our personal goals. 38 36 31 36 36 34 33 38 39 41 2 2 2 2 2 Items X19, X21 and X22 note v ~ 4. X19 and X21 = pgf >4; (8>f). X22 = pgm >4; (4>m). Fig 28 X19. X21 X22 I would feel comfortable trusting 0 with my deepest thoughts and feelings. I would be comfortable revealing to 0 what I feel are my shortcomings and handicaps. I would be comfortable with having a close emotional tie between us. 30 26 4 33 29 4 35 31 35 26 4 Item X17 notes v ~ 9, pgf>9; (9>f) Fig 29 X17 I would not be nervous about being spontaneous with 0. 9 Items X14 and X29 note v ~ 10; X14 – pgf >10, X29 – pgf >10; (20>f). Fig 30 X14 X29 I would not be afraid to share with 0 what I dislike about myself. I would feel at ease to completely be myself around 0. 41 38 31 28 10 10 38 26 12 Item X25 notes v ~ 12; pgf >12, (12>f). Fig 31 X25 I would be comfortable telling 0 what my needs are. 38 ESES Results The ESES contained 30 items. Items 2, 7, 11, 14, 16, 30 were red herrings not connected with any intended enquiry, (see fig 8). They have not been considered or included in the study. Items 12 and 20 shown in Fig 33 are the same item reversed and have been dealt with separately. The results of the remaining 21 items are shown below in fig 32. Results are shown for male, female and total scores in descending order. Fig 32 3. 18. 8. 19. 21. 17. 10. 24. 1. 15. 22. 4. 6. 9. 5. 13. 26. 27. 25. 29. 28. ESES RESULTS; in descending order When I take drugs I expect to feel better about things that have hurt me deeply in the past. When I take drugs I expect to feel more at ease with people. When I take drugs I expect to be comfortable discussing significant problems with 0. When I take drugs I expect to feel better about myself and my circumstances. When I take drugs I expect to be able to cope with feelings of shame and guilt associated with things from my past. When I take drugs I expect to feel more comfortable just being myself I don’t care about my problems when I take drugs When I take drugs I expect to feel more comfortable telling my experiences, even sad ones, to 0. When I take drugs I expect to be less afraid to confide my innermost feelings to people. When I take drugs I am less afraid of rejection in relationships. When I take drugs I expect to find it easier being open with 0 about my personal thoughts. When I take drugs I feel fine on my own I like taking drugs on my own When I take drugs I expect to feel more at ease telling 0 that I care about him/her. When I take drugs I expect to be more able to show those close to me that I care when they are upset. When I take drugs I expect to be less afraid to make a long-term commitment in a relationship. When I take drugs I feel alone When I take drugs I expect to be able to express my true feelings about things easier. When I take drugs I expect to be able to have a feeling of complete togetherness with 0. I like other people more when I take drugs I like myself more when I take drugs TOTAL I never call my friends when I take drugs I call my friends more when I take drugs F M TOTAL 60 44 47 43 107 87 40 45 85 41 43 84 46 38 84 36 46 82 45 40 35 40 80 80 39 40 79 39 37 40 42 79 79 35 34 35 43 40 39 78 74 74 33 40 73 37 36 73 36 34 37 35 73 69 37 29 66 30 25 803 31 29 818 62 54 1621 37 24 38 26 75 50 Fig 33 12. 20. Participants were asked to rate their answers from not at all, to, extremely characteristic of me. As fig 34 shows; some participants were consistent; KHf and IWm responded by admitting that it was extremely characteristic of them to never call their friends and not at all characteristic to call them more. 39 Others were significantly inconsistent as can be seen by the response made Fig 34 Initials Gender MG KH IW AZ KE AD DJD MM JW JS JC SF KES GH JG BS SJH RH KG BH MT CR HS RC F F M M F F M M M F F F F M M M F F M M M F F M Item 12; never call 5 5 5 5 4 4 4 2 2 3 3 5 1 5 4 4 3 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 Item 20; call more 1 1 1 2 1 1 1 3 1 2 1 5 1 4 3 2 1 2 2 1 1 4 4 5 by SF who responded by saying that it was extremely characteristic of her to call her friends and extremely characteristic of her not to. GHm responded in a similar vein. Similarly, KESf was unlikely to never call, whilst at the same time being extremely likely to call more. MGf and RCm were entirely consistent with their responses albeit in reverse. For both groups represented in fig 32, the highest scores, f=60, m47 on the ESES scale were in response to item 3, When I take drugs I expect to feel better about things that have hurt me deeply in the past. Items 18, 8 and 19 were the next highest with female to male ratios in scoring remaining similar. The lowest scores were for items 29; I like other people more when I take drugs, f=30, m=31, and for 28; I like myself more when I take drugs, f=25, m=29. There were 24 participants who could have scored 5; extremely characteristic of me, allowing for a total score of 105 for 21 items. The highest score was 96f and the lowest was 39f; both these participants belonged to the 10-15 years duration of drug use category. The highest scores indicating higher expectations that drugs will ease; or soothe discomfort or ease; soothe situations and feelings were in the 5-10 years duration category. Apart form the highest female score of 96 the remainder had the least expectations rating; 15th 16th 17th 19th 22nd 23rd and 24th. Conversely the 5-10 years category showed the highest expectations rating; 2nd 3rd 5th 6th 7th joint 8th 10th 40 and joint 11th The 0-5 years duration category had medium to poor expectations apart from m=85 who was the exception and rated 4th. Fig 35 Initials Gender Total Scores Order no. Duration of drug use SF KH DJD BS RH JG MG AZ HS AD IW RC CR GH BH KG MT JS JC KE KES JW MM SJH F M M M F M M F F M F M F M F F M M M F M F F F 96 67 62 58 49 43 41 39 90 86 83 82 79 78 78 77 73 73 85 71 69 50 47 45 1 15 16 17 19 22 23 24 2 3 5 6 7 8 8 10 11 11 4 13 14 18 20 21 10-15 10-15 10-15 10-15 10-15 10-15 10-15 10-15 5-10 5-10 5-10 5-10 5-10 5-10 5-10 5-10 5-10 5-10 0-5 0-5 0-5 0-5 0-5 0-5 Types of drug used H, C, Am, Can, Alc Coc, Am, MdMa, H, C, Alc H, C H, C, Alc H, C, Alc H, C, Am, Can H, C, Coc, Can, MdMa, Alc Am, Can, MdMa, Alc Meth, Am, Can, MdMa H, C, Can, Bzo H, C, Can, MdMa, Bzo H, C, Can H, C, Am, Can, MdMa H, C, Am, Can, MdMa H, Am, Can, MdMa, Bzo H, C, Can H, C H, C H, C H, C H, C H, C, Coc, Can, MdMa H, C, Can, MdMa This table shows which things are in the whatsit and what sit and they are Fig 36 3. 18. 8. 19. 21. 17. 10. 24. 1. 15. 22. 4. 6. 9. 5. 13. 26. 27. 25. 29. 28. Past hurts Dropping of defences Self disclosure Dropping of defences Dealing with shame Openness Contentment Self disclosure Self disclosure Surrender of control Openness Isolation Isolation Care for others Care for others Commitment Isolation Openness Dropping of defences Care for others Self care 41 F M TOTAL 60 44 47 43 107 87 40 41 46 36 45 43 38 46 85 84 84 82 45 40 39 39 37 35 40 40 40 42 80 80 79 79 79 35 34 35 33 37 36 34 37 30 25 43 40 39 40 36 37 35 29 31 29 78 74 74 73 73 73 69 66 62 54 The remaining 21 questions included some correlates with the questions in the FIS. Fig 37 FIS QUESTIONS with corresponding Q. Nos 1 2 4 I would feel uncomfortable telling 0 about things in the past that I have felt ashamed of. I would feel uneasy talking with 0 about something that has hurt me deeply. If 0 were upset I would sometimes be afraid of showing that I care. ESES Questions with corresponding Q. Nos score score 78 84 24 73 I might be afraid to confide my innermost feelings to 0. 9 A part of me would be afraid to make a long-term commitment to 0. 11 I would probably feel nervous showing 0 strong feelings of affection. I would find it difficult being open with 0 about my personal thoughts I would be afraid to take the risk of being hurt in order to establish a closer relationship with 0. I would be afraid that I might not always feel close to 0. 24 I would sometimes feel uncomfortable listening to 0's personal problems. 73 9 70 61 79 15 75 When I take drugs I expect to find it easier being open with 0 about my personal thoughts. When I take drugs I am less afraid of rejection in relationships. 79 25 66 8 52 When I take drugs I expect to be able to cope with feelings of shame and guilt associated with things from my past When I take drugs I expect to feel more comfortable telling my experiences, even sad ones, to O When I take drugs I expect to be more able to show those close to me that I care when they are upset When I take drugs I expect to be less afraid to confide my innermost feelings to people When I take drugs I expect to be less afraid to make a long-term commitment in a relationship. When I take drugs I expect to feel more at ease telling 0 that I care about him/her. 74 22 66 28 79 13 68 15 73 1 70 12 80 5 55 5 21 85 When I take drugs I expect to be able to have a feeling of complete togetherness with 0. When I take drugs I expect to be comfortable discussing significant problems with 0. Now I’m gonna look at individuals who scored high on these. My sample did not go through the DSM1V manual to see if they were addicts or substance misusers and I would expect to find some misusers who scored freaky scores Discussion As expected the participant group scored high for poor intimacy demonstrating more impoverished relationships than the controlled group. The highest score for the participant group was for question 32 in Part B of the FIS; “I have held back my feelings in previous relationships.” Analysis of the response to this question needs to be considered in view of the fact that at the time of taking 42 part in the study none of the participants were in relationships. 5This being due to rules regarding relationships at the respective treatment centres attended. It may be reasonable to assume therefore that participants were referring to their most recent past relationships when giving their responses. The results were presented showing the variances between participants and controlled group. The responses with the biggest variance were; item X19, “I would feel comfortable trusting 0 with my deepest thoughts and feelings.” The total for the participant group was 56 and for the controlled group 96 out of a possible score of 120. Gender variance was taken note of and for item 1 “I would feel uncomfortable telling 0 about things in the past that I felt ashamed of”, the males in both the participant group and the controlled group were seemingly more afraid than the females. In item X25 “I would be comfortable telling 0 what my needs are”, there was a 12 point variance in male to female ratio; with females feeling more uncomfortable disclosing their needs. The results for the ESES showed higher expectations for relief from past hurts, and expectations for feeling more at ease with people. Expectations were higher for increasing ability for dropping of defences and enhancing self disclosure. Caring for others and self care were low on the list of expectations. Participants with the highest expectations for self soothing were those in the 5-10 years duration of drug use category and those with the least were in the 10-15. This fits with the view of Marlatt and Gordon (1985), that drug use often continues far beyond the positive effects and amelioration of negative feelings has ceased and when the resulting state is not rewarding. It also fits with what is commonly reported by heroin addicts with a protracted drug using history; that they just take it to feel normal without having any positive expectations. According to the attachment theorists the controlled group scores in this study qualified them as secure adults; comfortable with intimacy, confident and accepting of being loved and valued. 5 Both Day Treatment Programmes attended by the participants follow the principles of the 12 Step Fellowships; specifically that recovery depends to a large extent on abstaining from romantic relationships for the first 12 months of recovery. 43 The participants on the other hand showed signs of having a very insecure attachment style. They were inhibited in their ability to show feelings of affection, be close to, or depend on others. This was consistent with other evidence that drug dependency is associated with relationship problems. (Skowron and Friedlander 1998) Collins and Read (1990) found that people with a more secure attachment style were higher in self-worth, more confident in social situations, and more self-assertive. Secure adults also had more positive beliefs about the world around them and generally viewed others as trustworthy, dependable, and well intentioned. Those with a more preoccupied attachment style had low self-worth, self-confidence, and assertiveness and their view of others was much less positive and trusting. They also believed that people had little control over their lives, and they viewed others as complex and difficult to understand. Avoidant adults tended to have positive views of themselves; high self-worth and assertiveness, although they saw themselves as less confident in social situations and were not confident in interpersonal encounters. They also had negative views of human nature; not trusting others as trustworthy or dependable. Studies generally found that secure adults reported more positive relationship experiences than preoccupied or avoidant adults. Secure individuals generally described their relationships as intimate, stable, and satisfying. In contrast, avoidant adults tended to report low levels of intimacy, commitment, and satisfaction, whereas preoccupied individuals reported jealousy, conflict, and high levels of negative emotional experiences. Peoples’ emotional responses and response patterns play a central role in attachment theory. Cassidy (2001) argues that secure attachment is associated with the capacity to participate in successful intimate relationships and that four key abilities are required for participation in intimate relationships. These are; the ability to seek care, to give care, to feel comfortable with an autonomous self, and the ability to negotiate and choose the level of intimacy one intends to engage in. Accordingly, intimacy is sharing and making one’s innermost self known as well as accepting the innermost 44 self of another. “It is being able to tell both the good and bad parts of oneself, to tell of anger, ambivalence, love; and to accept both the good and the bad parts of another, to accept anger, ambivalence, love. It is to share the self: one’s excitements, longings, fears and neediness, and to hear of these in another” . (122) Thus, in order to be intimate a person must be capable of care-seeking (123). This means that a person must be able to turn to appropriately selected others effectively when needed. This skill is learned by secure children who experience parents as loving responsive and supportive. A second requirement is to be able to trust that the self is loveable; determined by being valued in childhood. (Research undertaken by Sroufe (1977) showed higher self-esteem for pre-school children who were positively attached to their mothers than those who were not) Consequently, positive experiences further the capacity for intimacy by making children confident enough to seek care because they feel worthy of receiving care. (Cassidy 2001) Secure individuals will therefore bring a set of expectations to new relationships that are conducive to allowing intimacy to be negotiated and if chosen, to be developed. 45 References Abrams, D. B. & Niaura, R. S. (1987). Social Learning Theory. In H. T. Blane & K. E. Leonard (Eds.), Psycholigical theories of drinking and alcoholism. New York: Guilford Press. Adams, G. R., & Archer, S. L. (1994) Identity: A precursor to intimacy. In: S. L. Archer (Ed.), Interventions for adolescent identity development (193 – 213) Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Anderson, T. L. (1994) Drug abuse and identity: linking micro and macro factors. Sociological Quarterly, 35, 159–174. In: Lende, D. H., Smith, E. (2002) Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, (447–458) Beck, U., Beck-Gernsheim, E. (1995) The Normal Chaos of Love trans. Mark Ritter and Jane Wiebel (Cambridge: Polity Press) In: Keane, H. (2004) Disorders of Desire: Addiction and Problems of Intimacy Journal of Medical Humanities, 25 (3) 189-204 Berlant, L. (1998) Intimacy: A Special Issue,” Critical Inquiry 24, (2) 281–288, 284–285. In: Keane, H. (2004) Disorders of Desire: Addiction and Problems of Intimacy Journal of Medical Humanities, 25 (3) 189-204 Berman, B., Margolin, G. 1992) Analysis of the association between marital relationships and health problems: An interactional perspective. Psychological Bulletin 112, 39-63 In: Cassidy, J. (2001) Truth, lies, and intimacy: An attachment perspective. Attachment and Human Development 3 (2) 121-155 Bourgois, P. I. (1995) In Search of Respect: Selling Crack in El Barrio. New York: Cambridge University Press. In: Lende, D. H., Smith, E. (2002) Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, (447– 458) Bowen, M. (1978) Family therapy in clinical practice. Jason Aronson, New York. In: Thorberg, A. F. & Lyvers, M. (2005) Attachment, fear of intimacy and differentiation of self among clients in substance disorder treatment facilities. Department of Psychology, Bond University, Gold Coast, Qld 4229, Australia Bowlby, J. (1973) Attachment and loss: Vol 2 Separation. New York: Basic Books. In: Cassidy, J. (2001) Truth, lies, and intimacy: An attachment perspective. Attachment and Human Development 3 (2) 121-155 Bowlby, J. (1980) Attachment and Loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York: Basic Books. In: Cassidy, J. (2001) Truth, lies, and intimacy: An attachment perspective. Attachment and Human Development 3 (2) 121-155 Bowlby, J. (1988) A Secure Base. New York: Basic Books. In: Cassidy, J. (2001) Truth, lies, and intimacy: An attachment perspective. Attachment and Human Development 3 (2) 121-155 46 Brook, J. S., Whiteman, M. & Finch, S. (1993) Role of mutual attachment in drug use: a longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 982–989. In: Lende, D. H., Smith, E. (2002) Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, (447– 458) Brown, J. M. (1998) Self-regulation and the addictive behaviors. In: Miller, W. R. & Heather, N., eds. Treating Addictive Behaviors, 2nd edn, pp. 61–73. New York: Plenum Press. Carnes, P. (1983) Out of the Shadows: Understanding sexual addiction. Compcare. Minneapolis, Minnesota. In: Orford, J (2002) Excessive Appetites, A psychological view of addictions, (2nd ed) Wiley Cassidy, J. (2001) Truth, lies, and intimacy: An attachment perspective. Attachment and Human Development 3 (2) 121-155 Cicchetti, D., (1996) Child maltreatment: implications for developmental theory and research. Human Development, 39, 18–39. In: Lende, D. H., Smith, E. (2002) Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, (447–458) Clarkson, P. (2003) (2nd ed) The Therapeutic Relationship. Whurr Publishers: London and Philadelphia Collins, N.L. (1996) Working Models of Attachment: Implications for Explanation, Emotion, and Behaviour. Journal of Social Psychology 71, (4) 810-832 Collins, N. L., Read, S. J. (1990) Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644-663 In: Collins, N.L. (1996) Working Models of Attachment: Implications for Explanation, Emotion, and Behaviour. Journal of Social Psychology 71, (4) 810-832 Concar, D. (1994): Prisoners of Pleasure,” New Scientist (26–31) In: Keane, H. (2004) Disorders of Desire: Addiction and Problems of Intimacy Journal of Medical Humanities, 25 (3) 189-204 Cooper, M. L., Frone, M. R., Russell, M. & Mudar, P. (1995) Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: a motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 69, 990–1005. In: Lende, D. H., Smith, E. (2002) Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, (447–458) Craig-Bray, L., Adams, G. R. (1986) Measuring social intimacy in same-sex and opposite-sex contexts. Journal of Adolescent Research 1, 95-101. In: Seginer, R., Noyman, M. S. 2005) Future orientation, identity and intimacy: 47 Their relations in emerging adulthood. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 2 (1) 17-37 Descutner, C. J. & Thelen, M. H. (1991) Development and Validation of a Fear-of-Intimacy Scale. A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 3, (2), 218-225 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (1980) 3rd ed. American Psychiatric Press. Washington DC Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (1994) 4th ed. American Psychiatric Press. Washington DC Dyk, P. A., & Adams, G. R. (1987) The association between identity development and intimacy during adolescence: A theoretical treatise. Journal of Adolescent Research, 2, 223 – 235. Dyk, P. A., & Adams, G. R. (1990) Identity and intimacy: An initial investigation of three theoretical models using cross-lag panel correlations. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 19, 91 – 110. Ellis, A., McInerneyJ. J., Di Guiseppe, R., & Yeager, R. J. (1988). Rationalemotive therapy with alcoholics and substance abusers. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press. Erickson, C.K. and Wilcox, R.E. (2001) Neurobiological Causes of Addiction, Journal Social Work Practitioners; Addiction 1:7-22 Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York: Norton. In: Seginer, R., Noyman, M. S. 2005) Future orientation, identity and intimacy: Their relations in emerging adulthood. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 2 (1) 17-37 Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth and crisis. New York: Norton. In: Seginer, R., Noyman, M. S. 2005) Future orientation, identity and intimacy: Their relations in emerging adulthood. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 2 (1) 17-37 Flores, P. J., (1997) Group Psychotherapy with Addicted Populations: An Integration of Twelve Step and Psychodynamic Theory. Howarth Press Goodman, A. (1993) Diagnosis and Treatment of Sex Addiction,” Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy 19, (3) 225–251. In: Keane, H. (2004) Disorders of Desire: Addiction and Problems of Intimacy Journal of Medical Humanities, 25 (3) 189-204 Grotevant, H. D., & Cooper, C. B., (1986) Individuation in family relationships: A perspective on individual differences in the development of identity and roletaking skill in adolescence. Human Development, 29, 82 – 100. 48 Hoefler, D. Z., Kooyman, M. (1996) Attachment transition, addiction and therapeutic bonding – An integrative approach Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 13 (6) 511-519 In: Thorberg, A. F. & Lyvers, M. (2005) Attachment, fear of intimacy and differentiation of self among clients in substance disorder treatment facilities. Department of Psychology, Bond University, Gold Coast, Qld 4229, Australia Hofer, M. A. (1994) Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 250–283. Holt, M. L. (1977) Human intimacy in young adults: an experimental development scale. (Doctrinal dissertation, University of Georgia, 1977) Dissertation Abstracts International, 38, 4583-A (University Microfilms No. 7730, 475) In: Descutner, C. J. & Thelen, M. H. (1991) Development and Validation of a Fear-of-Intimacy Scale. A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 3, (2), 218-225 Keane, H. (2004) Disorders of Desire: Addiction and Problems of Intimacy Journal of Medical Humanities, 25 (3) 189-204 Kohut, H., Wolfe, E. S. (1978) The disorders of the self and their treatment: An outline. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 60, 413-425. In: Flores, P. J., (1997) Group Psychotherapy with Addicted Populations: An Integration of Twelve Step and Psychodynamic Theory. Howarth Press Koob, G. F., Le Moal, M. (2001) Drug Addiction, Dysregulation of Reward, and Allostasis Neuropsychopharmacology, 24, (2) 97-129 Krystal, H. (1994) Self- and object-representation in alcoholism and other drug-dependence: Implications for therapy. In: Levin, J. D. & Weiss, R. H., eds. The Dynamics and Treatment of Alcoholism: Essential Papers, 300–309. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson. Lefever, R. (2000) Kick the Habit. Carlton Books Lende, D. H., Smith, E. (2002) Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, (447–458) Leshner, L. (1999) Forward, in Drug Abuse: Origins and Interventions, (eds. Meyer Glantz & Christine Hartel), American Psychological Association. In Keane, H. (2004) Disorders of Desire: Addiction and Problems of Intimacy Journal of Medical Humanities, 25 (3) 189-204 Lindesmith, A. R. (1968) Addiction and Opiates. Chicago, IL: Aldine Publications. In: Lende and Smith (2002). Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, 447–458 49 Lutwak, N., Panish, J., and Ferrari, J. (2003) Shame and guilt: characterological vs. behavioural self-blame and their relationship to fear of intimacy Personality and individual differences 35 909-916 Marlatt, G. A. & Gordon, J. R. (1985) Relapse Prevention: Maintenance Strategies in the Treatment of Addictive Behaviors. New York: Guilford Press. McCauley, K. (2003) Kant vs. Mill: The Ethical Implications of Saying Addiction is a Disease. Presentation at SECAD Conference 2003, Atlanta, USA. Available: http://www.addictiondoctor.com McCauley, K. (2006) Is Addiction Really a “Disease”? Presentation at UKESAD Conference, London, UK Available: www.addictiontoday.org Miller, W. R. & Rollnick, S. (1991) Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People to Change Addictive Behavior. New York: Guilford Press. Morrison, J. R. (2001) DSM-1V Made Easy, The Clinicians Guide to Diagnosis. The Guildford Press: New York Nakken, C. (1998) The Addictive Personality: Roots, Rituals, and Recovery. Centre City: Hazelden Press, (14–15) Narcotics Anonymous 5th Edition (2003) Narcotics Anonymous World Services Inc Orford, J. (2001) Addiction as excessive appetite. Addiction 96 15-33 Orford, J. (2002) 2nd ed. Excessive Appetites: A Psychological View of Addictions Wiley Orlofsky, J. L. (1993). Intimacy status: Theory and research. In J. E. Marcia, A. S. Waterman, D. R. Matteson, S. L. Archer, & J. L. Orlofsky (Eds.), Ego identity: A handbook for psychological research (pp. 111 – 133). New York: Springer-Verlag. In: Seginer, R., Noyman, M. S. 2005) Future orientation, identity and intimacy: Their relations in emerging adulthood. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 2 (1) 17-37 Orlofsky, J. L. Marcia, J. E., & Lesser, I. M. (1973). Ego identity status and intimacy versus isolation crisis in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 211 – 219. In: Seginer, R., Noyman, M. S. 2005) Future orientation, identity and intimacy: Their relations in emerging adulthood. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 2 (1) 17-37 Palmer, S. (1996) The multimodal approach: theory, assessment, techniques and interventions. In: S. Palmer and W. Dryden (eds), Stress Management and Counselling: Theory, Practice, Research and Methodology. Sage: London Pearson, G. (1987) 2nd ed The New Heroin Users. Basil Blackwell. Oxford. In: Orford, J (2002) Excessive Appetites, A psychological view of addictions, Wiley 50 Peele, S. (1985) The Meaning of Addiction: Compulsive Experience and its Interpretation. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. In Lende and Smith (2002). Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, 447–458 Rubenstein, C., Shaver, P. (1982) In search of intimacy: Surprising new conclusions from a nationwide survey on loneliness and what to do about it. New York: Delacorte Press. In: Descutner, C. J. & Thelen, M. H. (1991) Development and Validation of a Fear-of-Intimacy Scale. A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 3, (2), 218-225 Sargent, M. (1992) Women, Drugs and Policy in Sydney, London and Amsterdam. Avebury, Sydney. In: Orford, J. (2002) 2nd ed. Excessive Appetites: A Psychological View of Addictions. Wiley Schaefer, M. T., Olson, D. H. (1981) Assessing intimacy: The PAIR inventory. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. 7, 47-60 Seginer, R., Noyman, M. S. 2005) Future orientation, identity and intimacy: Their relations in emerging adulthood. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 2 (1) 17-37 Sexton, R. E., Sexton, V. S. (1982) Intimacy: A historical perspective. In: Fisher, M & Stricker, G. (eds), Intimacy (1-20) New York. Plenum Press. Shaffer, H. (1999) Strange Bedfellows: A Critical View of Pathological Gambling and Addiction. Addiction 94, (10) 1445–1448. In: Keane, H. (2004) Disorders of Desire: Addiction and Problems of Intimacy Journal of Medical Humanities, 25 (3) 189-204 Sharabany, R. (1994). Intimate friendship scale: Conceptual underpinnings, psychometric properties and construct validity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 449 – 469. Skowron, E. A., Friedlander, M. L. (1998) The differentiation of self inventory: Development and initial validation. Journal of Counselling Psychology 45, 235-246. In: Thorberg, A. F. & Lyvers, M. (2005) Attachment, fear of intimacy and differentiation of self among clients in substance disorder treatment facilities. Department of Psychology, Bond University, Gold Coast, Qld 4229, Australia Sroufe, L.A., Waters, E. (1977) Attachment as an organisational construct Child Development 48 1184-1199 Sweet, C. (1994) Off The Hook: How To Break Free From Addiction and Enjoy a New Way of Life. London: Piatkus (9) Thompson, R. A. & Calkins, S. D. (1996) The double-edged sword: emotional regulation for children at risk. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 163-182. In: Lende and Smith (2002). Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis 51 of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, 447–458 Thorberg, A. F. & Lyvers, M. (2005) Attachment, fear of intimacy and differentiation of self among clients in substance disorder treatment facilities. Department of Psychology, Bond University, Gold Coast, Qld 4229, Australia Webb, J. A., Francis, D. J., & Caid, C. D. (1993) Relationship among intrapersonal risk, alcohol expectancies, and alcohol usage among early adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 18, 127-43 White, H. R., Bates, M. E. & Johnson, V. (1990). Social reinforcement and alcohol consumption. In W. M. Cox (Ed.), Why people drink: Parameters of alcohol as a reinforcer. New York: Gardner Press. Wilson, M. & Daly, M. (1985) Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: the young male syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 59–73. In: Lende and Smith (2002). Evolution meets biopsychosociality: an analysis of addictive behavior. Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs Addiction, 97, 447–458 52