Download 17. Structural word analysis Spelling Development

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Spelling of Shakespeare's name wikipedia , lookup

German orthography reform of 1996 wikipedia , lookup

Scripps National Spelling Bee wikipedia , lookup

Spelling reform wikipedia , lookup

English-language spelling reform wikipedia , lookup

American and British English spelling differences wikipedia , lookup

English orthography wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Pratt-Struthers, J., Struthers, T. B., & Williams, R. L. (1983). The effects of the
Add-A-Word Spelling Program on spelling accuracy during creative writing.
Education & Treatment of Children, 6(3), 277-283.
The following is excerpted from PsychInfo.
Abstract. Examined whether correct spelling by 9 learning disabled students
(aged 10-12 yrs), placed in a special education resource room, could be increased in the
context of creative writing through the use of C. A. McGuigan's (1975) add-a-word
spelling program. Target words were those the Ss used frequently but consistently
misspelled in their creative writing. Target words were placed on the add-a-word spelling
program format using a multiple baseline across words. Interobserver checks were made
for each misspelled word, and overall agreement was 100%. Ss' correct spelling of target
words during their creative writing increased from 0% to a mean of over 90% after the
words were introduced in the program. Results indicate generalization of accurately
spelling words to creative writing assignments.
Stevens, K. B. & Schuster, J. W. (1987). Effects of a constant time delay procedure
on the written spelling performance of a learning disabled student. Learning
Disability Quarterly, 10, 9-16.
The following is excerpted from Stevens & Schuster (1987).
Abstract. A constant time delay procedure was used to teach written spelling to
an 11-year-old, school-labeled LD youngster who had severe spelling deficits. The time
delay procedure is a near errorless instructional method that transfers stimulus control
from a controlling stimulus (a prompt that signals the correct response) to a new stimulus
(a target response). As a result of a 5-second constant delay procedure the student
acquired, maintained, and generalized 15 spelling words. The procedure is easy to
implement, requires little teacher preparation time, and results in a low student error rate.
In the absence of published time delay research with LD individuals, the results are
encouraging. Based on our findings, the time delay procedures is a viable alternative for
LD students who have not benefited from traditional instructional procedures. Since the
results of this study and those in the unpublished literature indicate that time delay
procedure is effective, further examination with other LD students is warranted.
Method. The constant time delay procedure is a near errorless instructional
method that transfers stimulus control from a controlling stimulus (a prompt that signals
the correct response) to a new stimulus (a target response). This is accomplished by
pairing the controlling stimulus with the new stimulus and systematically increasing the
amount of time between their presentations. The interval between the presentation of the
task request (new stimulus) and the teacher’s model/prompt (controlling stimulus) is
systematically increased until the student emits the correct response before the
controlling stimulus is presented. For example, the new stimulus (the task request, Spell
[target word]” is given to the student. Simultaneously, a printed model of the target word
(the controlling stimulus) is presented (zero-second delay) to allow the student to copy
the model. After the model has been presented at the zero-second delay for a
predetermined number of trials, the time between the task request and presentation of the
model is systematically increased. This gives the student an opportunity to respond
before the presentation of the model or to wait for the model if further prompting is
required.
Gordon, J., Vaughn, S., & Schumm, J. S. (1993). Spelling interventions: A review of
literature and implications for instruction for students with learning
disabilities. Learning Disabilities Practice, 8(3), 175-181.
The following is excerpted from Gordon, Vaughn & Schumm (1993).
Abstract. The purpose of this article is to summarize 17 spelling intervention
studies for students with learning disabilities and provide implications for improved
spelling instruction. Interventions are reviewed with respect to subjects, procedures, and
results. Discussion focuses on typical spelling instruction and suggested spelling
instruction for students with learning disabilities in the areas of error imitation and
modeling, unit size, modality, computer-assisted instruction, peer tutoring, and study
techniques.
17 Spelling Interventions
1. Individual instruction of four words in each condition on 4 consecutive days.
Bradley, L. (1981). The organization of motor patterns for spelling: An effective remedial
strategy for backwards readers. Developmental Medical Child Neurology, 23, 8391.
2. Group instruction for 3 days, including: oral spelling, individual word presentation,
worksheet practice, mastery training, corrective feedback, and distributed and cumulative
practice.
Bryant, N. D. Drabin, I. R., & Gettinger, M. (1981). Effects of varying unit size on
spelling achievement in learning disabled children. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 14, 200-203.
3. Ten spelling words were taught in a 5 step study procedure during a 15-min training
session.
Frank, A. R., Wacker, D. P., Keith, T. Z., & Sagen, T. K. (1987). Effectiveness of a
spelling study package for learning disabled students. Learning Disabilities
Research, 2, 110-118.
4. Interevntion included a standard dictation test and imitation-modeling procedure to
100% mastery. A second list of words with the same orthographic features was
administered without instructions to promote generalization.
Gerber, M. M. (1986). Generalization of spelling strategies by learning disabled students
as a result of contingent imitation/modeling and mastery criteria. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 19, 530-537.
5. Instruction included eight sessions over a 3-week period. Lessons for the experimental
group incorporated reduced unit size, distributed practice and review, and training for
transfer.
Gettinger, M., Bryant, N. D., & Gayne, H. R. (1982). Designing spelling instruction for
learning disabled students: An emphasis on unit size, distributed practice, and
training for transfer. Journal of Special Education, 16, 439-448.
6. Classwide peer tutoring.
Harper, G. F., Mallette, B., & Moore, J. (1991). Peer-mediated instruction: Teaching
spelling to primary school children with mild disabilities. Reading, Writing, and
Learning Disabilities, 7, 137-151.
7. Groups were directed-study, student-controlled, or teacher-monitored groups.
Harris, K. R., Graham, S., & Freeman, S. (1998). Effects of strategy training on
metamemory among learning disabled students. Exceptional Children, 54, 332338.
8. Spelling instruction was given for 30 minutes, 5 days per week. Twenty words were
taught weekly for a 4-week period. Instruction consisted of phonics drill, drills with
flashcards, and practice writing words on the chalkboard.
Kauffman, J. M., Hallahan, D. P., Haas, K., Brame, T., & Boren, R. (1978). Imitating
children’s errors to improve their spelling performance. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 11, 217-222.
9. Subject received three 25-minute tutoring sessions each week with a time-delay
computer-assisted instruction (CAI) procedure.
Kinney, P. G., Stevens, K. B., & Schuster, J. W. (1988). The effects of CAI and time
delay: A systematic program for teaching spelling. Journal of Special Education
Technology, 9, 61-72.
10. Peer tutoring.
Mandoli, M., Mandoli, P., & McLaughlin, T. F. (1982). Effects of same-age peer tutoring
on the spelling performance of a mainstreamed elementary learning disabled
student. Learning Disability Quarterly, 5, 185-189.
11. The teachers used strategic procedures emphasizing awareness of word structure and
spelling strategies, combined with keyboard training in twice weekly, 45-minute sessions
for 3 months.
Margalit, M., & Roth, Y. B. (1989). Strategic keyboard training and spelling
improvement among children with learning disabilities and mental retardation.
Educational Psychology, 9, 321-329.
12. Intervention included a pretest, nine learning trials, and a test for transfer of learning.
Learning trials required the subject to say the words, and write them on a piece of paper.
A contingent imitation procedure was used if he misspelled a word.
Nulman, J. H., & Gerber, M. M. (1984). Improving spelling performance by imitating a
child’s errors. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 328-333.
13. 5-second constant time delay.
Stevens, K. B. & Schuster, J. W. (1987). Effects of a constant time delay procedure
on the written spelling performance of a learning disabled student. Learning
Disability Quarterly, 10, 9-16.
14. Subjects received spelling training under three experimental conditions that involved:
writing, sorting letter tiles, or typing on the computer.
Vaughn, S., Schumm, J., & Gordon, J. (1992). Early spelling acquisition: Does writing
really beat the computer? Learning Disability Quarterly, 15, 223-228.
15. Subjects received spelling training under three experimental conditions that involved:
writing, sorting letter tiles, or typing on the computer. Training procedures incorporated
components of best instructional practice, examined student performance over time, and
incorporated student interviews concerning condition preference.
Vaughn, S., Schumm, J., & Gordon, J. (1993). Which motoric condition is most effective
for teaching spelling to students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 26, 191-197.
16. Computer assisted instruction – The Spelling Machine program
Watkins, M. W. (1989). Computerized drill-and-practice and academic attitudes of
learning disabled students. Journal of Special Education Technology, 9, 168-172.
was taught concurrently with a self-questioning strategy.
Wong, B. Y. L. (1986). A cognitive approach to teaching spelling. Exceptional Children,
53, 169-173.
Following reviewed by Karyn Erkfritz
17. Structural word analysis Spelling Development
(Ehri, 2000)
1. Prealphabetic level
a. Children may store salient visual stimuli for words (e.g. golden arches for
McDonald’s)
b. Children may produce scribbles for writing but may lack letter details
2. Partial alphabetic level
a. Know names/sounds of alphabet letters
b. Can read/write words at semiphonetic level
c. Basic and incomplete knowledge
d. Vowel and consonant sounds not present in letter names are less wellknown
e. Spelling may be inventive and correct spellings may be difficult because
they alck sufficient knowledge of the alphabetic system
f. Spellings are partial because it is still difficult to segment words into
phonemes and because full knowledge of phoneme-grapheme isn’t
developed
3. Full alphabetic level
a. Can segment words into phonemes
b. Knowledge of conventional grapheme-phoneme correspondences
including vowels
c. Inventive spellings are more complete and are able to stretch out sounds in
words to spell them (may find that extra sounds are not symbolized in
conventional spelling)
d. Understanding of the basic system that accounts for many letters in
conventional spellings, easier to read/write
e. Have some words stored in memory in sufficient letter detail so that
reading new words by analogy to familiar words is possible (e.g. rig, fig,
big)
4. Consolidated alphabetic level
a. Learning structure of larger units consisting of letter sequences that recur
across different words
i. Units may involve spellings of syllables, or parts of syllables, or
affixes appearing at beginnings or endings of words
ii. Recurring blends of graphophonemic units become consolidated
into larger units
b. Easier for students to decode and invent spellings of longer multisyllabic
words.
*Correlations between reading and spelling .68 to .86
Prerequisite Skills
(Ehri, 2000)
1. Knowledge of letters
2. Ability to associate letters with sounds (e.g. t is “tee” This is referred to as
grapheme-phoneme knowledge. This is important for reading.
3. Ability to associate sounds with letters (e.g. “buh” is b). This is referred to as
phoneme-grapheme knowledge. This is important for spelling.
4. Knowledge of digraphs (e.g. “ch” or “ar”). These are two letters together that are
pronounced or read differently than their spelling suggests.
Teach Spelling Rules
While this is not an empirically validated treatment, an appropriate intervention may be
to teach a student the rules of spelling. A list of these rules can be found:
http://www.dyslexia.org/spelling_rules.shtml
http://www.mc3.edu/aa/lal/workshops/wksp_spelling/spellingrules.html (has practice
exercises)
Attack Strategy (for beginning spellers)
(Ehri, 2000)
Involves inventing spellings of words
1. Stretch out pronunciations
2. Detect sound units
3. Apply knowledge of alphabetic system to generate plausible letter sequences for
the sounds
*Likelihood that spellers will generate correct letter sequences is limited
Cover-Copy-Compare
(http://www.interventioncentral.org/htmdocs/interventions/ccc.shtml)
1. Provide the child with a list of words written on the left hand side of a page.
2. The teacher gives the student an index card.
3. The student is directed to look at each correct item (e.g., correctly spelled word,
computation problem with solution) on the left side of the page.
4. The student is instructed to cover the correct model on the left side of the page
with an index card and to spell the word in the space on the right of the sheet.
5. The student then uncovers the correct answer on the left to check his or her work.
6. If the student gets the word correct, they would give themselves +1
7. If the student gets the word incorrect, they would have to write the word 3 times
and not earn +1
8. General Rule: a child has acquired the word if they get it correct in two
consecutive trials
Constant Time Delay (Mushinski, & Stormont-Spurgin, 1995)
1. Provide the student with a blank sheet of paper.
2. The teacher would give the students the rules: “I will say a word, give it in a
sentence, and then say it again. You must give me your best answer. If you get it
right, you will earn 1 point. If you get it wrong, you will have to practice writing
it correctly 3 times and will earn no points. If you do not know a word, you can
sit and wait and then I will show you how to spell it. If you wait, you will not get
any points, but will only have to write it once.”
3. Wait 7 seconds before providing the student with the model. If they do not write
anything or write an incorrect response, write the word on a note card and then
have them copy it correctly three times (only if they missed it) or 1 time (if they
waited for the model) on the lined paper.
*These interventions have been looked at with respect to students w/ LD:
Error Imitation and Modeling (Gordon, Vaughn, & Schumm, 1993)
This intervention is designed to directs students' attention to discrepancies between
misspellings and correct spellings through imitation of errors prior to the presentation of
models
When a child makes an error, use this corrective feedback procedure:
1. Imitate the child’s error by rewriting it
2. Then present a correct model (especially in the case of non-phonetically spelled
words)
Considerations: Need a ‘sufficient’ number of trials
Peer Tutoring (Mandoli, Mandoli, & McLaughlin, 1982)
1. Tutee meets 15 minutes a day with the peer tutor
2. First 2 days, a modeling method was used, the adult presented the teaching
procedure and the peer tutor was encouraged to follow it.
3. On Day 3, the peer tutor conducted the entire session under the guidance of an
adult representing a good example of academic behavior (The peer tutor should
not require additional assistance beyond adult supervision in the room beyond
Day 3)
4. The tutor read the words aloud, then asked the tutee to read the words
5. Then the tutee was instructed to orally spell the words
6. Flashcards and games were used to enhance the teaching process
7. If the tutee got the words wrong, he had to write them out 10 times
8. The tutor provided assistance, encouragement, helpful hints and praise to the
tutee.
Study Techniques (Gordon, Vaughn, & Schumm, 1993)
1. Propose a word to learn
2. Correctly write the word or display the word with letter tiles
3. Name the word
4. Write the word
5. Name the word again, check accuracy, and continue steps 2-4 until mastery
6. Practice the word in this way for 6 consecutive days
Variation of steps:
1. Say the word
2. Write and say the word
3. Check the word
4. Trace and say the word
5. Write the word from memory and check
6. If the word is incorrect, repeat steps the first 5 steps
Variation of steps:
1. Do I know this word?
2. How many syllables do I hear in this word? (Write down the number)
3. I’ll spell out the word
4. Do I have the right number of syllables down?
5. Now, does it look right to me?
6. When I finish spelling, I’ll tell myself I’ve worked hard.
*May want to package with include interspersal with known and unknown words, use of
positive practice and reinforcement, and possible teacher supervision of study sessions
Directed Spelling Thinking Activity (Graham, Harris, Loynachan, 1996)
This intervention is directed at improving spelling skills by contrasting spelling patterns
1. Choose 2 or more patterns,1 that the student has at least some knowledge of, but
lack of understanding of its application
2. Contrast selected can include more than one spelling pattern and more than two
patterns
a. E.g. Compare short vowel consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) with long
vowel pattern marked by e (CVCe)
3. Once contrast in selected, words are selected that illustrate target patterns. One or
two exceptions are chosen to illustrate the consistencies/inconsistencies of
English orthography
4. Lists with 10-12 words are suggested with students w/ LD, “typical” students 1620 words
5. All words should be familiar to students and selected so that they will generate a
fairly even mix of correct/incorrect spellings.
6. Each word is written on a note card
7. The teacher begins the lesson by administering a test of words for that lesson (no
mention that words are related to particular patterns)
8. Discussion follows with students talking about why they spelled a word they way
they did
9. Then teacher presents correct spelling of the word, complimenting students on the
reasonableness of their spellings and highlight the parts of the word that caused
difficultly
10. The students proceed through the list this way and the teacher encourages
comparison by asking how words are similar and different
11. Then the students sort the words into their respective categories. One word (on a
card) is chosen to represent each category and for the exception words, a question
mark is used.
12. The teacher randomly selects one of the remaining words and says it, and puts in
under the appropriate category (a wall chart with pockets for holding word cards
can facilitate process)
13. As the teacher continues through the words, the students decide where to place
them
14. Then the students add words of their own to the categories w/the teacher
providing feedback
15. In concluding the activity, the teacher solicits from the group an explicit statement
concerning the orthographic principles underlying the contrasting patterns
Follow-up activities: looking for words in reading materials that fir the patterns,
scanning their own writing for words that fit the patterns, sorting new words that
correspond to the target patterns and developing their own pattern study book of
words that fit the patterns
Multisensory Study Techniques for Memorizing Spellings of Words
(Scott, 2000)
*Once a multisensory study techniques has been taught, Harris, Graham, and Freeman
(1988) found equal effectiveness for students with LD when practice was independent,
teacher-directed on an as-needed basis, or entirely teacher prompted
5-Step study strategy (Graham & Freeman, 1986)
1. Say the word.
2. Write and say the word.
3. Check the word.
4. Trace and say the word.
5. Write the word from memory and check spelling.
Horn Method 2 (Horn, 1954)
1. Pronounce each word carefully.
2. Look carefully at each part of the word as you pronounce it.
3. Say the letters in sequence.
4. Attempt to recall how the word looks, then spell the word.
5. Check this spelling attempt to recall.
6. Write the word.
7. Check this spelling attempt.
8. Repeat the above steps if necessary.
8-step method for pencil or computer practice
(Berninger, Abbot, Rogan, Reed, Abbott, Brooks, Vaughan, & Graham, 1998a)
1. Look carefully at the word while tutor sweeps finger over and says it out loud.
2. Watch and listen while tutor says sound corresponding to color-coded graphemes,
in a left to right fashion (for example, /b/ /o/ /t/ while point to “b,” “oa,” and “t.”)
3. Name letters as tutor points to letters.
4. Close eyes and picture the word in the “mind’s eye.”
5. Keep eyes closed and spell the word out loud.
6. With pencil, open eyes and write word or with computer, open eyes and point to
letters on an alphabetical grid; then tutor points to the letter on keyboard and child
presses key.
7. Compare spelling to target.
8. If incorrect, tutor points out where difference lies, then pervious steps are
repeated.
Summary/Other Resources (Gordon, Vaughn, & Schumm, 193)