Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Is Robust Image Formation a Key Innovation? Predictability and Contingency in Macroevolution vs. Predictability versus Contingency in Macroevolution • How likely are certain key actualized adaptations to re-emerge, if we re-ran the tape of life, or if life evolved on other worlds? • What are ‘Good Tricks’ in design space (sensu Dennett, 1995)? – Good Tricks must be more than just adaptations—they must be key adaptations, likely to evolve iteratively and to have substantial macroevolutionary effects. – Dennett (‘95), Dawkins (’04), and Conway Morris (0’3) have all suggested that vision is an excellent candidate for Good Trick-hood • Galis (‘01); Zuker (‘94) and Land & Fernald (’92) suggest vision may be key innovation Question Presented Here: How predictable and / or contingent is the evolution of Vision and other forms of Robust Image Formation? – Do these complex adaptive solutions represent key innovations? What Do I Mean By ‘Robust’ Image Formation? • Robust Image is one which represents the detailed, threedimensional topography of an organism’s surrounding environment, including the spatial arrangement of objects and object features, shapes, textures, depths and distances. • ‘Physical Image’ of an object or stimulus is a functional category that applies to the distribution of a stimulus on a sensory receptor surface • The stimulus ultimately projected onto the senso-receptor array may be chemical (as in olfaction) or energetic (as in vision, echolocation, and electrogeneration). – However, only energetic stimuli provide enough information about the environment for an organism to form what I call a ‘robust image’ – Moreover, apart from vision (which is passive) only active energetic image-formation (pulse emission) is adequate for the task What is a Key Innovation? • Mayr (1963) and Simpson (1953): certain morphological, physiological, or functional complexes play a more significant role than others in directing particular macroevolutionary trajectories • Associated with the origins of higher taxa – Presumably by enabling the anointed lineage to occupy a new adaptive zone with reduced predation pressures (Van Valen, 1970) • Measures: – Diversity – Disparity – Sister Taxa Comparisons vs. Tree Thinking – Ecological Implications For Other Lineages – Convergent (Iterative) Evolution a Statistical Bonus (re-run of tape) Iterative Evolution of Camera-Type Eyes • Camera-type eyes have evolved independently in 5 phyla, including molluska, chordata, annelida, cnidaria, and arthropoda Iterative Evolution of Compound Eyes • The compound eye has also evolved independently in up to 4 phyla, including arthropoda (multiple times), annelida, molluska, and the echinoids (maybe) Image-Forming Eyes and Diversification: An Empirical Investigation Land and Fernald’s (1992) Claim: 96%!!! • Although the convergent origin of the macroscopic arrangements of image-forming eyes occurred in only a handful of the 33 recognized metazoan phyla, these few eye-bearing phyla—namely, Cnidaria, Molluska, Annelida, Arthropoda, and Chordata—account for over 96% of the known species (Land & Fernald, 1992). • Isn’t this strongly suggestive that vision is a key innovation? (No) • Problems with the claim: – First, 3 of the phyla they mention (Cnidaria, Annelida, and Molluska) contain predominantly eye-less species, and hence including those entire phyla in the 96% count is misleading. – Secondly, 2 clades with image-forming eyes—namely the arthropods and vertebrates—account for the overwhelming majority (over 99%) of the 96% of all species. Distribution of Species in 18 Extant Clades w/ Convergent Image-Forming Eyes Clade Species Number Arthropods 839,000 Vertebrates 48,800 Cephalopods 650 Pectinacean Bivalves 410 Prionodontan Bivalves 216 Pontellid Copepods 140 Cypridinid Ostracods 105 Strombid Gastropods 80 Corycaeid Copepods 55 Sapphirinid Copepods 37 Alciopid Polychaetes 31 Heteropod Gastropods 30 Br. Sabellid Polychaetes 27 Cubozoan Cnidarians 25 Eu. Sabellid Polychaetes 18 Laternulid Bivalves 15 Bi. Sabellid Polychaetes 3 Me. Sabellid Polychaetes 1 Diversity Comparison of Extant Image-Forming Clades with their Non-Visual Sister Groups * * adapted from De Queiroz (1999) Image-Forming Clade Diversity (Species No.) Non-Visual Sister Clade Arthropods 839,000 Eucoelomate Protostomes Vertebrates 48,815 Cephalochordates Diversity (Species No.) More or Less Diverse 71,000 + 23 + 40,000 - Cephalopods 650 Gastropods Pectinacean Bivalves 410 Anomiacean Bivalves 25 + Prionodontan Bivalves 216 Mytilacean Bivalves 175 + Corycaeid Copepods 56 Tuccid Copepods 1 + Sapphirinid Copepods 37 Sabelliphilid Copepods 107 - Alciopid Polychaetes 31 Eteonine Polychaetes 141 - Cubozoan Cnidarians 17 Scyphozoan Cnidarians 200 - Laternulid Bivalves 15 Periplomatid Bivalves 28 - Megalomma Polychaetes 1 Demonax Polychaetes 8 - Is Vision a Key Innovation? Above Data Suggests: – Neither image-forming eyes in general nor in particular contexts (active lifestyles) correlate strongly with differential patterns of diversity. – On their face, these results appear to suggest that while image-forming eyes confer local adaptive benefits given the surprising number of independent (polyphyletic) origins, such increases in fitness do not seem to translate into adaptive radiations. – But: • only measured net speciation • Because neontological, addresses only long-term patterns of diversification not spatio-temporally localized macroevolutionary effects. • visual acuity (minimum resolvable angle) (but see arthropods) Paleontological Comparison of the Diversity of Visual Clades for first ~88 (my)* *adapted from data drawn by de Queiroz, (2002) and Benton (1993) Clade by Order of Appearance Mean No. Families for first ~88 my Fossil Arthropods 73 Vertebrates 16 Cephalopods 31 Pectinacean Bivalves 2 Prionodontan Bivalves 3 Laternulid Bivalves 2 Heteropod Gastropods 4 Strombid Gastropods 2 Extant Cubozoan Cnidarians 4 Pontellid Copepods 2 Corycaeid Copepods 2 Sapphirinid Copepods 2 Alciopid Polychaetes 2 Littorinid Gastropods 2 Br. Polychaetes 2 Eu. Polychaetes 2 Bi. Sabellid Polychaetes 2 Megalomma Polychaetes 2 Incumbent Advantage Hypothesis • Two major gaps (i.e. major radiations) in the distribution of the mean numbers of families: (1) Between the arthropods and all other groups, and (2) Between the arthropods, vertebrates and cephalopods and all other visual clades. • This supports the Incumbent Advantage Hypothesis – The early acquisition of a key innovation and its subsequent radiation may competitively dampen (or exclude) any future diversification in connection with the novel acquisitions of the trait – Especially plausible in vision • Once initial active predation evolved, remaining clades resorted to more inert or torpid modes of predator evasion (e.g. bivalves, echinoderms etc. Vision and the Cambrian Explosion • Explosive increase in diversity and disparity (morphospace occupation) (Foote & Gould, 1992) • Somewhat controversial (see Briggs et al. 1992) • Trace Fossils Show Rapid Burst in Ecological / Functional Complexity (Conway Morris, 1998a/1998b). • Uncontroversial [1st Eye Appears in Trilobitidae 544 mya/ C.E.] • Introduction of Vision-Supported Active Predation (Parker, 2004) • Garden of Ediacara Arm’s Race (McMenamin & McMenamin, 1990) Result: (1) Advanced eyes in 2 other major clades, (2) hard-parts, (3) complex ecological strategies Right: Eyed Arthropods and soon-to-be-eyed Vertebrates (Pikaia) What Do I Mean By ‘Robust’ Image Formation? • Robust Image is one which represents the detailed, threedimensional topography of an organism’s surrounding environment, including the spatial arrangement of objects and object features, shapes, textures, depths and distances. • ‘Physical Image’ of an object or stimulus is a functional category that applies to the distribution of a stimulus on a sensory receptor surface • The stimulus ultimately projected onto the senso-receptor array may be chemical (as in olfaction) or energetic (as in vision, echolocation, and electrogeneration). – However, only energetic stimuli provide enough information about the environment for an organism to form what I call a ‘robust image’ – Moreover, apart from vision (which is passive) only active energetic image-formation (pulse emission) is adequate for the task Echolocation • Echoic Capabilities have evolved independently at least 5 times in the history of life, including 3 orders of mammals—Chiroptera (bats), Cetaceans (toothed whales), Insectivora (tree shrews)—and two orders of birds Apodiformes (swiftlets) and Caprimulgiformes (oilbirds) • Chiropterans form Robust Acoustic Images of their environment – including the shapes, distances, textures, and spatial orientation of objects. – can distinguish targets separated by distances well under 1mm in three dimensional space. – Bats have a fine range resolution and are able to discriminate range differences on the order of 1 cm at distances up to 240 cm Chiropteran Echolocation as a Key Innovation • One of the most diverse and ubiquitous orders of mammals—nearly 1/4 mammals is a bat. – Also Sheer Biomass! • • Sister Taxa Comparison: • Bats >1000 species vs. Dermoptera (4) or Tree Shrews (20) Microchiroptera (sophisticated echo) Vastly More Successful than Megachiroptera (reduced echo) • Bats found on nearly every landmass except the polar regions and a few tropical islands • Few natural (no specializing) predators [except for R. Brandon] • Radiation at K-T boundary and suddenly appear over the entire globe completely developed (like eyes!) (Darwin) Ecological Contingencies in Chiropteran Echolocation • Primary Bat Niche: Aerial Insect Hawking • Why did it take sophisticated echolocation take so long to evolve? – Contingent on increases in aerial nocturnal pollinating insect densities (Lepidoptera and Diptera) due to angiosperm proliferation in the Cretaceous. • Even so, why did bats develop echo first, before avians? • Best Answer: No aerial insectivorous birds or pterosaurs Phylogenetic Contingencies in Chiropteran Echolocation • Active Biosonar / Robust Acoustic Image Formation requires high frequency signal emission capabilities – This comes at a huge metabolic cost – May be limited to Endothermic Vertebrates with directional sound capabilities (lungs / pharynx) • Bats have reduced cost with the biomechanical coupling of echolocation and powered flight. • Echo-then-Flight, Flight-First, Tandem Theories Phylogenetic Contingencies in Chiropteran Echolocation • Order of Origin: Bat Incumbent Advantage may have excluded Avian Echolocation (c.f. early eyes) • I propose that sophisticated flying, echolocating, and nocturnally aerial hawking insectivorous bats expanded rapidly to fill much of this niche, preventing its occupation by subsequent avian clades who might hit upon the same Good Trick (echolocation). – To use Darwin’s (1859) metaphor, bats have formed a wedge that is jammed so tightly in the economy of nature that no animal has subsequently been able to pry it out. • Rudimentary Echolocation has evolved in Australasian swiftlets (edible nests) and the Neotropical oilbird (1 species). • Do Not use echo to detect / capture prey • Do Not form Robust Acoustic Images. • No substantial macroevolutionary effects Cetacean Echolocation • Water represents another medium amendable to acoustic signaling, and thus it is the only other meta-habitat in which sophisticated active biosonar has evolved. • Acoustic Image acuity / Robustness of dolphins is as good / better than bats • Use Echolocation not only for object detection but also for small, medium, and large-scale navigation by locking onto landmarks. • Use Echolocation not only for object detection but also for small, medium, and large-scale navigation by locking onto landmarks (unless pelagic). • Holistic Representation of Objects • Nearly 100% cross-modal recognition—Echo-to Vision and Vision-to Echo – Echolocation is functionally equivalent to vision. Contingency of Cetacean Echolocation • Sister Taxa Comparison: – Odontoceti is a diverse sub-order, containing 10 families and over 80 species, Mysticeti (baleen whales) are comprised of 4 families and only 14 species. – Complex echolocation, to the extent that it facilitates prey capture, navigation, and social communication may have played a key adaptive role in their relative success. • Key Question: If echolocation is such a Good Trick, Why has it not evolved in other closely related marine mammal taxa, or in any other taxa, for that matter? – Do I hear the ring of contingency? Not necessarily… – Mysticeti or Sirenians? – Why not Pinnipeds w/ similar foraging? (review shows they don’t echo) • Answer: Due to their obligatory amphibious lifestyle (mating, etc.), retained the ability to hear on land /ice (i.e. in air, which has a different impedance than H20). • Their ears are not adapted for exceptional full-time aquatic life necessary for robust echolocation – Only endothermic fully aquatic fish-foraging animal is the cetacean! • What about Fully Marine Reptiles, like Icythyosaur or Mosasaur? (ectothermic) schnauzenorgan Robust Electrical Image Formation Mormyridae • • Electric Organ Discharges: Independently Evolved in 2 grps of Weakly Electric Fish Electric Fovea: – – • The mormyrids have two specialized electric foveae—one in the ‘nasal region’ for long-range guidance and object detection, and the other in schnauzenorgan, a long and flexible chin appendix covered with densely packed mormyromast electroreceptive cells, associated with shorter-range prey detection and discrimination. Like dual fovea in some birds (predator detection /flight and myopic foraging on ground) Objects can alter the electric organ discharge either in waveform or in amplitude, and the fish perceive both in order to assess object properties in multiple dimensions, including the object’s the object’s size, shape, spatial orientation, depth and distance, and complex impedance (passive and resistive components, and capacitance). – Distance measure (via maximal slope) is unequivocal (unlike vision ambiguity), from which size can be positively derived • ‘Color Perception’: The detection of capacitance properties through (e.g.) waveform distortion can be compared to color vision which measures the wavelength of light reflected by an object. • Holistic object perception: trained to receive a positive reward (conspecific EOD) by learning and remembering to choose a metal cube (cylinder, pyramid, elliptical, etc.), they later preferred a plastic cube to a metal cylinder • Alien Aspects of Electrolocation Gymnotiformes Electrolocation as a Key Innovation • Sister Taxa Comparison: – Mormyridae is comprised of over 200 species, and is by far the largest family in its order, as the others—Arapaimidae, Gymnarchidae, Hiodontidae, Notopteridae, Osteoglossidae, and Pantodontidae—all range from 1-5 species • Advantages of Electrolocation • EOD as behavioral isolating mechanisms – Gymnotidae, while also successful (comprised of 5 families, and nearly 200 species), are not nearly as diverse as their highly successful sister order Siluriformes (catfish), which contains 37 families nearly 2,000 species, as well as a much wider geographical distribution. • Nevertheless, electrogeneration is connected to substantial absolute diversification in both of the major taxa in which it has evolved. – However, biogeographical ranges are circumscribed, perhaps due to limited ecological applicability. Robust Image Formation and Complex Social Behavior • Echolocation & Sociality in Cetaceans: – Super-alliances (>400), the largest known stable associations outside of humans. – Social transmission of tool use • Electrolocation & Sociality in Fish – Rather than converging on a single location for hunting, large predatory mormyrids of Lake Malawi (Africa) form cohesive traveling packs that forage as a unit through the cluttered rocky bottom for small cichlid prey. – “Form temporally stable associations [> 1 month] that characterize pack hunting carnivores and cetaceans” (Arnegard & Carlson, 2005). – EOD Synchronization – Individual recognition Pack-Hunting Mormyrids (Lake Malawi, Africa) Other Macroevolutionary Effects of Robust Image Formation • Encephalization Related to Robust Image Formation – Most Sophisticated Robust Image Forming Clades are More Encephalized than their sister taxa (1) Vision-Related Encephalization • • Vertebrates > Echinoids Cephalopods > Bivalves (2) Echolocation-Related Encephalization: • • Dolphins > Mysticeti • Metacognition (delphinids) Bats > Flying Lemurs or Tree Shrews (2) Electrolocation-Related Encephalization: – Mormyrids > non-electric sister families » – Hypertrophied ‘Mormyrocerebellum’ (at huge metabolic cost) Gymnotiformes > Siluriformes