* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download The demise of number ratios in music theory
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
The demise of number ratios in music theory Pythagoras faces the final curtain Richard Parncutt Centre for Systematic Musicology University of Graz, Austria CIRMMT, Schulich School of Music McGill University, Montreal 17 May 2012 Abstract Medieval music theory was dominated by Pythagorean cosmic numerology. Today there are still musicians, theorists, historians, composers, and psychologists out there who theorize with number ratios. But most intervals have two ratios (Pythagorean and just) that lie within a continuous range of acceptable tunings, so neither is “correct”. Ratios only make psychological sense if the numbers correspond to audible harmonics in complex tones (8:9 is ok but 64:81 is misleading). In fact, musical intervals are approximate psychological distances on a linear, one-dimensional scale that are determined by an aural cultural tradition. The relationship between intervals and ratios is indirect and mediated by music history, performance constraints and tone perception. Every tuning is a compromise among competing criteria: maximizing harmonicity and familiarity, minimizing roughness (beating), optimizing stretch, anticipating voice leading, manipulating expression. Modern performances of early music (should) navigate between just and Pythagorean, as 12-tone equal temperament does. Euler and Leibniz dreamed of a neural ratio detector, but modern research revealed an inextricable mix of temporal and spectral (tonotopic) processing. I will consider future implications for music theory and cognition. Special thanks to composer Graham Hair, Professor Emeritus, Glasgow University and Visiting Professor, Manchester Metropolitan University for ideas and feedback. The major third interval o Pythagorean tuning reflects motion tendencies (leading tone rises) emphasizes difference between major and minor o Just tuning minimizes beats between almost-coincident harmonics Difference: 22 cents; cf. category width of “M3” = 100 cents A rising major scale in 3 tuning systems sound examples • 12-tone equally tempered (12ET) All semitones are equal Works for any mainstream Western music • Pythagorean (“3-limit”) Combinations of P8=1:2 and P5 = 2:3 Occasional problems (enharmonic ambiguities) • Just (“5-limit”) Combinations of P8=1:2, P5 = 2:3, M3=4:5 Fifth between scale degrees 2 and 6 is not 2:3 Must constantly shift scale steps to stay in tune A rising major scale in 3 tuning systems ratios and cents Scale step 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 12ET 200 400 500 700 900 1100 1200 Pythag 8 :9 204 64 :81 3 :4 408 498 2 :3 702 16 :27 128 :243 1 :2 906 1110 1200 Just 8 :9 4 :5 3 :4 2 :3 3 :5 8 :15 1 :2 204 386 498 702 884 1088 1200 Calculating ratios Add intervals - multiply ratios e.g. m7 = P5 + m3 = 3/2 x 6/5 = 9/5 – Pythagorean tuning • combinations of P8 and P5 only • frequency ratios in the form 2n/3m or 3m/2n – Just tuning • combinations of P8, P5, M3 Interval (cents) = log2 (f1/f2) x 1200 log2 (x) = log10 (x) / log10 (2) = ln (x) / ln (2) Most intervals have 2 ratios. Would the real ratio please stand up? interval P1 m2 M2 m3 M3 P4 TT P5 m6 M6 m7 M7 P8 note C C# D D# E F F# G G# A A# B C chr. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 pure/just Pythagorean 1:1 1:1 16:15 256:243 9:8 or 9:10 9:8 6:5 32:27 5:4 81:64 4:3 4:3 45:32 729:512 3:2 3:2 8:5 128:81 5:3 27:16 9:5 or 7:4 16:9 15:8 243:128 2:1 2:1 Some strange ideas of ratio theorists Pythagoras Greek philosopher and mathematician, 6th Century BC Monochord mathematics • P8 is 1:2, the P5 is 2:3, and the P4 is 3:4 • all other intervals by multiplying and dividing • M3 is four P5s minus two P8s, or 64:81. Music of the spheres • planets and stars move according to this kind of math • corresponding to musical notes a cosmic symphony • Pythagoras could hear it! (Tinnitus?) Saint Bonaventure Italian medieval theologian and philosopher, 1221 – 1274 “Since therefore, all things are beautiful and to some measure pleasing; and [since] there is no beauty and pleasure without proportion, and proportion is to be found primarily in numbers; all things must have numerical proportion. Consequently, number is the principal exemplar in the mind of the Creator and as such it is the principal trace that, in things, leads to wisdom. Since this trace is extremely clear to all and is closest to God, it … causes us to know Him in all corporeal and sensible things; and while we learn that things have numerical proportion, we take pleasure in this numerical proportion and we judge things irrefutably by virtue of the laws that govern it.” Itinerarium mentis in Deum, II, 7 Boethius Italian philosopher, early 6th century “But since the nete synemmenon to the mese (3,456 to 4,608) holds a sesquitertian ratio -- that is, a diatessaron -- whereas the trite synemmenon to the nete synemmenon (4,374 to 3,456) holds the ratio of two tones....” Fundamentals of Music, trans., Calvin Bower (New Haven: Yale), IV, ix. Cited by David Whitwell in Essays on the Origins of Western Music Giovanni Battista Benedetti Italian mathematician, 1530 –1590 In a letter to Cipriano de Rore dated from around 1563, Benedetti proposed a new theory of the cause of consonance, arguing that since sound consists of air waves or vibrations, in the more consonant intervals the shorter, more frequent waves concurred with the longer, more frequent waves at regular intervals. Isaac Beeckman and Marin Mersenne both adopted this theory in the next century. (Wikipedia) What we now call a “temporal model” of consonance Johannes Kepler German mathematician, astronomer (1571-1630) Third law of planetary motion: • The square of the orbital period of a planet is directly proportional to the cube of the semi-major axis of its orbit. Aim: • understand the music of the spheres • express planetary motion in music notation (Did he have tinnitus too?) Sources Holton, G. J., & Brush, S. G. (2001). Physics, the Human Adventure. Rutgers. Burtt, E. A. (1954). Metaphysical Foundations of Modern Physical Science. Gottfried Wilhelm Leibniz German mathematician and philosopher (1646-1716) The pleasures of subconscious counting “Die Freude, die uns die Musik macht, beruht auf unbewusstem Zählen.” “Musik ist die versteckte mathematische Tätigkeit der Seele, die sich nicht dessen bewusst ist, dass sie rechnet.” (Letters) Leonhard Euler Swiss mathematician and physicist (1707-1783) “Take for example a clock, whose aim is to mark divisions of time; we will like it most if … the parts are laid out and combined in such a way that all contribute to indicate time with exactitude. … where there is perfection, there is necessarily also order.” “…the degree of softness of ratio 1:pq, if p and q are prime numbers … is p+ q - 1." Tentamen novae theoriae musicae ex certissimis harmoniae principiis dilucide expositae (1731) (A attempt at a new theory of music, exposed in all clearness according to the most well-founded principles of harmony) Johanna Kinkel German composer and writer (1810 –1858) “As we wonder what it is that grips us with foreboding and delight in Chopin’s music, we are apt to find a solution that might appear to many as pure fantasy, namely that Chopin’s intention was to release upon us a cloud of quarter-tones, which appear now as phantom doppelgänger in the shadowy realm within the intervals produced by enharmonic change. Once the quarter-tones are emancipated, an entirely new world of tones will open to us.” Acht Briefe an eine Freundin über Clavier-Unterricht (1852) Clarence Barlow composer of electroacoustic music “Harmonicity” of an interval = “simplicity” of its number ratio …which depends on “digestibility” of the numbers …which depends on their prime factors Systematic enumeration of all possible frequency ratios within an octave with relatively high harmonicity In order of interval size: 1:1, 15:16, 9:10, 8:9, 7:8, 6:7, 27:32, 5:6, 4:5, 64:81, 7:9, 3:4, 20:27, 2:3, 9:14, 5:8, 3:5, 16:27, 7:12, 4:7, 9:16, 5:9, 8:15, 1:2. Two essays on theory. Computer Music Journal, 11, 44-59 (1987) Laurel Trainor (Music) Psychologist, McMaster University “Effects of frequency ratio simplicity on infants' and adults' processing of simultaneous pitch intervals with component sine wave tones” (abstract) Effects of frequency ratio on infants' and adults' discrimination of simultaneous intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23 (5), 1427-1438 (1997) Ross W. Duffin Dept of Music, Case Western Reserve U, Cleveland OH “The fifth and fourth of 12ET aren’t bad, being out of acoustical purity by only about one-fiftieth of a semitone, but the major third is where 12ET fails the harmonic purity test. 12ET major thirds are extremely wide – about oneseventh of a semitone wider than acoustically pure 5:4 major thirds. (…) This interval is the invisible elephant in our musical system today. Nobody notices how awful the major thirds are. (…) Asked about it, some people even claim to prefer the elephant. (…) But I’m here to shake those people out of their cozy state of denial. It’s the acoustics, baby: Ya gotta feel the vibrations. “ How equal temperament ruined harmony (and why you should care). London: Norton, 2007 (pp. 28-29) Ross W. Duffin “Some writers have also cast doubt on whether singers, especially, could sound frequencies to the kind of tolerances necessary to achieve intentional tuning choices. Performances and recordings by modern early music groups, such as the Hilliard Ensemble recording used below, refute that supposition utterly in my opinion, presenting highly trained voices with minimal vibrato and a level of accomplishment in tuning accuracy that Seashore, Barbour and their colleagues probably never imagined.” Just Intonation in Renaissance Theory and Practice. Music Theory Online (2006) Robert (Bob) Fink Saskatoon, Canada “Historically, the ear has preferred simple ratios as harmonious, and complex ratios have been avoided or considered noisy or dissonant ... For example, two notes on the piano right next to each other have a complex ratio (play them together to hear this) and these kind of ratios cause ‘beats’ -- a kind of repetitive ‘wowwow-’ effect, which is physically measured as unpleasant to the ear.” On Music Origins, greenwych.ca/natbasis.htm (N.B. this is NOT the Robert Fink at Dept. of Musicology, UCLA) Kurt Haider Institut für Musiktheorie und harmonikale Grundlagenforschung, Wien • eine mathematische Strukturwissenschaft … die historisch auf die Pythagoreer, PLATON (427-347 v. Chr.) und die Neuplatoniker zurückgeht • spätestens seit JOHANNES KEPLER (1571-1630) als empirische Wissenschaft • die harmonikale Naturphilosophie führt die Struktur der Naturgesetze auf ganzzahlige Proportionen zurück • die Reduktion auf das Messbare und Quantifizierbare, wie sie GALILEO GALILEI (1564-1642) und JOHN LOCKE (1632-1704) vornahmen, (wird) aufgehoben • durch die Intervallempfindung der ganzzahligen Proportionen werden nun qualitative Parameter wie Form, Gestalt oder Harmonie wieder Gegenstand der Wissenschaften kurthaider.megalo.at/node/49 The more “rational” opposition Aristoxenus “Harmonics” (4th Century BC; pupil of Aristotle) “Mere knowledge of magnitudes does not enlighten one as to the functions of the tetrachords, or of the notes, or of the differences of the genera, or, briefly, the differences of simple and compound intervals, or the distinction between modulating and nonmodulating scales, or the modes of melodic construction, or indeed anything else of the kind.” “we must not follow the harmonic theorists in their dense diagrams which show as consecutive notes those which are separated by the smallest intervals [but] try to find what intervals the voice is by nature able to place in succession in a melody” Macran, H. S. (1902). The harmonics of Aristoxenus. London: Oxford UP. Jean-Philippe Rameau (French composer and theorist, 1683 -1764) Tried to explain major and minor triads using ratios: • major triad 20:25:30 (4:5:6) • Mm7 20:25:30:36 • minor triad 20:24:30 (10:12:15) • m7 25:30:36:45 But then resorted to other means: “Obviously there can be no complete chord without the fifth, nor consequently without the union of two thirds which form the fifth; for all chords should be based on the perfect chord that results from this union.” Treatise on Harmony (1722), transl. Philip Gossett (1971) Rameau ‘s later work was based on the harmonic series - not ratios. Hermann von Helmholtz German physiologist and physicist, 1821-1894 “Even Keppler (sic.), a man of the deepest scientific spirit, could not keep himself free from imaginations of this kind … Nay, even in the most recent times theorizing friends of music may be found who will rather feast on arithmetical mysticism than endeavor to hear out partial tones” (p. 229). On the Sensations of Tone as a Physiological Basis for the Theory of Music, 1863; 4th ed. transl. A. J. Ellis (Helmholtz theorized with ratios too…) Carl Dahlhaus German musicologist, 1928-1989 “Whereas in the ancient-medieval tradition number ratios were considered to be the foundation or formal cause of consonance, in modern acoustics and music theory they paled to an external measure that says nothing about the essence of the matter. … In the music theory of the 18th and 19th Centuries, the overtone series is the natural archetype of the interval hierarchy upon which rules of composition are founded. (But it was difficult to explain autonomy of the minor triad and the dissonant character of the fourth.) The surrender of the Platonic idea of number meant nothing less than the collapse of the principle that had carried ancient and medieval music theory; theory lost its object of contemplation.” C. Dahlhaus (Ed.), Einführung in die Systematische Musikwissenschaft (1988) A short history of Western music theory Central question and motivation: Why is (Western) music meaningful and emotional? 1. 2. 3. 4. Pythagoras, medieval theorists Renaissance theory Scientific revolution Empirical psychology Stages 2, 3 and 4 gradually moved away from Pythagoras A short history of Western music theory Pythagoras (6th Century BC) Middle Ages Musical intervals are number ratios • Ratios are the “meaning of life, the universe and everything” (Douglas Adams) • All intervals are combinations of 1:2 and 2:3 Three kinds of “music” • musica mundana — order of the cosmos • musica humana — human-cosmic harmony • musica instrumentalis — instrumental music Boethius (6th C. AD) agreed in De Musica A short history of Western music theory Renaissance theorists Ratios can also include factors of 5 just M3 (4:5) preferred to Pythagorean (64:81) • • • • Ramos de Pareja (1482) Gioseffo Zarlino (1558) Giovanni Battista Benedetti (1585) etc. (see Ross Duffin in MTO, 2006) Attempt to explain the sonority of omnipresent “triads”? NB: No physical measurements of live performance! A short history of Western music theory Late scientific revolution (18th-19th century) Musical intervals are audible relationships between partials in harmonic complex tones • 4:5 is distance between 4th and 5th harmonics Consonance has two aspects: • harmonicity (Rameau, Stumpf) • roughness (Helmholtz) Basis of intervals and consonance: • physics of the natural environment • physiology/psychology of perception James Tenney’s “Consonance-Dissonance Concepts” CDC concept Tenney’s definition CDC-1 CDC-2 melodic affinity monophony pitches in common sonority of early isolated dyads polyphony roughness or pitch salience? CDC-3 clarity of lower 14th C. voice pitch salience of (lower) melody CDC-4 property of individual tones in chord dependence of roughness on amplitude of individual tones CDC-5 smoothness or 19th C. roughness historical period 18th C. possible perceptual account roughness of whole sonority Only CDC-1 depends directly on ratios A short history of Western music theory 20th century experiments on intonation Summary of many experiments on intonation in performance of tonal Western music: • 12ET generally preferred • Pythagorean tends to be preferred over just – especially for rising leading tones • Just intonation may be preferred for slow, steady tones without vibrato Many empirical studies! Authors: Duke, Fyk, Green, Hagerman & Sundberg, O’Keefe, Loosen, Karrick, Kopiez, Nickerson, Rakowski, Roberts & Matthews... Johanna Devaney et al. (in prep.) with Ichiro Fujinaga, Jon Wild, Peter Schubert, Michael Mandel Participants: professional Task: sing an exercise by Benedetti (1585) to illustrate pitch drift in just intonation Main results: • Intonation close to 12ET • Standard deviation of pitch is typically 10 cents (!) • Small drift in direction of Benedetti’s prediction Ross W. Duffin “ Renaissance performers would have preferred solutions that favor just intonation wherever and whenever possible … deviations from it would have been momentary adjustments to individual intervals, rather than wholesale adoption of temperament schemes” Just Intonation in Renaissance Theory and Practice. Music Theory Online (2006) Thomas Kuhn’s “paradigm shift” or scientific revolution Paradigm • • Entire landscape of knowledge and implications in a discipline Universally accepted Long process of change • • Gradual increase in number of anomalies crisis Experimentation with new ideas intellectual battles Features of change • • Old and new are incommensurable Shifts are more dramatic in previously stable disciplines Examples Physics: Classical mechanics relativity and quantum mechanics Psychology: Behaviorism cognitivism Music theory: Math & notation performance & perception Enharmonics Are they related to ratios? Assuming that G♯ is M3 above E, and A♭is M3 below C: In Pythagorean tuning, G♯ > A♭ Because 64:81 is more than 400 cents In just tuning, G♯ < A♭ because 4:5 is less than 400 cents No clear relation between enharmonics and ratios! Enharmonics an example Can we play this chord “like” a dominant 7th or “like” an inverted augmented 6th? Or did Chopin simply make an innocent two-alternative forced choice? And surely other issues of interpretation are much more important? Enharmonics origin of spelling conventions Why do conventions of enharmonic spelling exist? • Primarily to facilitate reading in performance How do we facilitate reading? • Minimize the number of symbols (clutter) • Minimize the cognitive load by increasing the chance of recognizing familiar patterns How do we do that? • Use key signatures for common (stable) tones • Notate common intervals in consistent ways 12 semitones P8 not A7 or d9 7 semitones P5 not AA4 or d9 4 semitones M3 not AA2 or d4 This is a hierarchy: P8 is more important than P5 etc. Enharmonic spellings are often compromise solutions Enharmonics Why they exist Guido’s diatonic staff has big advantages! • Graphic: • Compact: • Efficient: Illustrates melodic contour 7 rather than 12 vertical positions per octave Common pitches easier to notate Enharmonic equivalents exist because: • A non-staff pitch can be notated two ways • Scribes, composers had a “two-alternative forced choice” Don’t exaggerate importance of enharmonics! Don’t reify enharmonics! Accuracy of interval sizes How accurately can we perceive intonation? Geringer & Madsen (1981). Discrimination between tone quality and intonation in unaccompanied flute/oboe duets. Psychology of Music. 480 music and nonmusic graduate and undergraduate subjects listened to 24 oboe and flute duets. Variables: • good/bad tone quality • ET or one instrument shifted by 50 cents Task: • Intonation ok? • Timbre ok? Result: Even musicians could not discriminate tone quality from intonation This presumably applies also to Renaissance theorists! Accuracy How accurate are interval ratios in practice? Uncertainty of interval sizes in middle register • Just noticeable difference – for successive pitches: about 2 cents (many studies) – for simultaneous pitches: about 2 cents (audible beats) • Added performance uncertainty for singers – say: 3 cents for vocal jitter – all voices have it! (old more than young) • Combined uncertainty √(22 + 32) ≈ 4 cents A very conservative estimate for ideal conditions! Much greater in most music. Accuracy Estimating the uncertainty of a given interval Is the interval among first 10 harmonics? Yes: • If small and unambiguous, + 4 cents (larger: stretch?) • If ambiguous, halve the difference between 2 possibilities No: Add uncertainties of simpler intervals (P8s and P5s) Accuracy An example How uncertain is the size of the M2 interval? Method 1: Direct from the harmonic series Ambiguous: 8:9 (204 cents) or 9:10 (182 cents)? 193 + 11 cents Method 2: Add/subtract intervals M2 = 2xP5 - P8 Uncertainty of P5 and P8 is about 4 cents each (could be larger for P8 due to stretch of up to 10 cents!) Combination of 3 intervals: add variances Square root of sum of squares √(42 + 42 + 42) = √48 ≈ 7 cents Categorization of perceptual parameters Color e.g. range of wavelengths of the color red – “nature”: • physiology of rods and cones – “nurture”: • mapping between color words and light spectra Speech sounds e.g. range of formant frequencies of vowel /a/ – “nature”: • vocal tract resonances near 500 and1500 Hz – “nurture”: • learned formant frequencies of each vowel Experiment on categorical perception of musical intervals (Burns & Campbell, 1994) P1 m2 M2 m3 M3 P4 TT P5 m6 M6 m7 M7 P8 Stimuli: Melodic intervals of complex tones; all ¼ tones up to one octave Participants: Musicians Question: name the interval using regular interval names (semitones) How the ear acquires pitch categories …from the multimodal distribution of intervals in performed music F F#/Db G The ear is bombarded with interval sizes varying on a continuous scale, (i) in complex tones (between partials) and (ii) in music (between fundamentals). Some intervals are more common than others, so categories crystalize. These psychological categories are the ORIGINAL, REAL “musical intervals”. Distribution of interval sizes Which do we find in the empirical data? pure Pythagorean Bimodal distribution with tendency toward • pure (M3 = 386 cents) or • Pythagorean (M3 = 408 cents) NO: We never find this Normal distribution sd ≈ 20 cents + 1 sd = acceptable tuning + 2 sd = pitch category YES: We generally find this What influences intonation? Real-time adjustment of tone frequency in performance • Perceptual effects (individual tones) – octave stretch (small intervals compressed*) – beating of coinciding partials • Cognitive effects (musical structure) – preference for equally spaced categories (facilitates categorization and cognitive processing) – less stable tones are more variable in pitch** – rising implication of leading tone; major-minor distinction • Effects of performance – solo versus accompaniment (soloists tend to play sharp) – technical problems or limitations of instruments • Effects of interpretation – intended emotion (e.g. tension-release) – intended timbre (e.g. deep = low) * Rakowski ** Ambrazevicius & Wisniewska, What about quartertones? • Non-western theories of frequency ratios are equally problematic! • Quartertones simply lie between half-tone steps Does the brain have a ratio-detection device? If it did, we might expect: 1. bimodal interval performance and preference distributions 2. low tolerance to mistuning of harmonics in complex tones 3. an evolutionary basis for ratio detection In fact: 1. distributions are unimodal 2. harmonics mistuned by quartertone or semitone (!) are still perceived as part of the complex tone (Moore et al., 1985) 3. environmental interaction depends instead on identification of sound sources via synchrony, harmonicity… (Bregman, 1990) Does the brain have a ratio-detection device? Is pitch perception based on frequency ratios between partials? Decades of pitch research suggest that: • the periodicity of a waveform plays a role within a critical band • temporal processing is less important across critical bands In both cases: no evidence for a ratio-detection device! In general: • Temporal and spectral processing (tonotopic representations) are inextricably mixed in biological neural networks . • Info about intervals is stored in this complex, hidden way, including – distances between audible harmonics in isolated tones – distances between tones in music • The auditory system is very sensitive to such distances! separate foreground & background in everyday sounds (ASA) Imagine a world without interval ratios How could we explain the beauty and power of music without ratios? A world without ratios Psychological foundations Innate ability to accurately perceive pitch distance • Simultaneous: in harmonic complex tones • Successive: between speech syllables Ease of recognizing asymmetric patterns • e.g. diatonic scales, major & minor triads • for those with absolute pitch: character of keys Tone/chord functions depend on vertical/horizontal context • Not on notational conventions Emotion & meaning due to layers of old associations • Not magical mathematical or cosmic connections A world without interval ratios Imagine notating without accidentals… 12 approximately equally spaced pitch categories • Idea is already latent in ancient Greek theory of tones and semitones • inspired many 19-20th century “notation reforms” In “keyboard trigram” notation, • staff lines are groups of 3 black keys • leger lines are groups of 2 black keys A world without interval ratios Implications Freedom to develop theories that • depend on the listener`s subjective experience (the music itself?) • are independent of – other theories – notational conventions A level field for comparing theories • more objective comparison of theories • a fresh look at – history of notation and enharmonic spelling – notational approaches of composers A psychological theory of the consonance of sonorities • Smoothness of complex sounds – peripheral, in ear (Helmholtz) – lack of roughness, superficial • Harmonicity of complex sounds – central, in brain (Stumpf, Terhardt) clarity of harmonic function (Riemann) • Familiarity, culture – central, in brain Roughness of pure tone dyads Plomp & Levelt 1965 nothing to do with ratios Roughness of complex tone dyads Plomp & Levelt (1965) ratios are artifacts of overlapping harmonics Why is the tritone dissonant? Not because of its ratio! A tritone of pure tones sounds quite smooth Dissonance of a tritone of complex tones: • Roughness of almost coincident harmonics • Inharmonicity • Musical ambiguity Harmonics of C3 Harmonics of F#2 The lot So why do we still believe in ratios? We prefer explanations that are: 1. Simple • easy to understand, remember, apply • easy to falsify (like a scientific theory) 2. Quantitative • everyone can use words; maths needs expertise • numbers seem more objective than words • achievement s of science (Newton, Einstein…) 3. Phenomenological • We can experience the theory directly e.g. by retuning a keyboard and playing music Why do we still believe in ratios? We believe enharmonics are intrinsic to tonality • and ratios are linked to enharmonics E.g. musicians associate this music with sharps • but non-musicians listen to the music itself Why do we still believe in ratios? We like music that is… • consistent with our personal identity • familiar but still interesting • live, because – played by real people (social function of music) – like what we know (familiar) but also different (interesting) – bond with audience members (identity) Any slight retuning of a keyboard is: • similar to what we know (familiar) • but also different (interesting) Why we still believe in ratios? Post hoc ergo propter hoc – a logical fallacy = after this, therefore because of this = since event B followed event A, B must have been caused by A Biological basis in classical conditioning • ||: A = dog hears bell, then B = dog eats food :|| several times • B predicts A salivation Human consequences • • • • A rooster crows. We expect the sun to rise. Causation? A solar eclipse. We play drums. The sun returns. I just-tune my keyboard and enjoy the effect. ratio = causa? cf. Huron`s “fast and dirty response” in Sweet anticipation Why we still believe in ratios? The word itself • “ratio” is related to “rational” - we want “rational” explanations • in fact, “ratio” originally means “reason”! Online Etymological Dictionary etymonline.com © 2001-2012 Douglas Harper Ratio: • 1630s, "reason, rationale," • from Latin ratio "reckoning, calculation, procedure," also "reason," • from rat-, pp. stem of reri "to reckon, calculate," also "think" • mathematical sense is attested from 1660 Conclusions Musical intervals are: • primarily psychological (not physical or mathematical) • approximate (categorical) – there is no ideal tuning • learned from music (an aural tradition) Exact musical interval size can depend on: • • • • • • maximizing musical familiarity maximizing harmonicity minimizing roughness (fast beating) optimizing stretch anticipating voice leading manipulating expression The origin of musical emotion is: • a chain of associations (many forgotten or subconscious) • largely independent from the origin of intervals Conclusions • Advantages of ratio theory: – theory accounts for most prevalent intervals • octave, fifth, fourth, thirds, sixths, sevenths... – theory explains12-tone equal temperament • best approximation to main ratios • Disadvantages of ratio theory: – – – – – two ratios for one interval? which is “correct”? systematic deviations in real performance unclear physiological or psychological basis Intervals are subjective - part of culture Intervals emerge/develop gradually over generations We don‘t need ratios to explain… Music’s meaning, beauty, magic • chains of associations Omnipresence of major and minor triads • harmonicity, fusion, smoothness Closure of harmonic cadences • melodic distances, implied roots Occasional poor tuning of piano accompaniments to solo violins • octave stretch, leading tones, expression Special character of Renaissance vocal music • pitch structure, rhythm, timbre, expression Success of some ratio-based microtonal composition (e.g. Partch ) • Form, development, timbre Definition of music • non-lexical, culturally complex sound-movement patterns Further implications Mathematics is a tool for measuring things - not an aesthetic criterion Pc-set theory is applicable to any kind of music in the chromatic scale Relatively wide major thirds are more or less wide - not “Pythagorean” New scales/tunings e.g. 19ET their chance of success depends on similarity with familiar patterns Implications for performance of Renaissance polyphony Some performers practice hard to approach just tuning - lately with real-time computer feedback. What are they really achieving? Excellent skills in intonation, which improve performance regardless of tuning. x Suppression of natural tendencies to express by manipulating intonation. Monteverdi would not have been impressed. x Construction of an “authenticity” that may never have existed. Music approaching JI has a special character, but without recordings of music as performed in the Renaissance, we can only guess about its intonation. x Exaggeration of the importance of music theory. 20th century theorists read non-existent intentions into minds of 20th-century composers (think of those complex pc-set analyses), so why should Renaissance theorists be any different? They did not know what we know today, namely how difficult it is to perform or perceive different tuning systems in practice. Renaissance theorists were merely trying to explain the sonorous Thanks Graham Hair Composer Professor Emeritus, Glasgow University Visiting Professor, Manchester Metropolitan University The final curtain http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=LL5YrVgZ_vU&feature=related