Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
LONG-TERM ADJUSTMENT AFTER EXTREMELY . CHALLENGING EVENTS IN A SAMPLE OF METNAMESECANADIAN SENlORS by Stephanie Griffiths B.A. Hons., Mount AHison University, 1997 Thesis Submitted in Partiai Fulfilment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts in the Department of Psychology O Stephanie Griffiths, 2001 SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY Al1 rights reserved. This work rnay not be reproduced in whole or part, by photocopy or other means, withaut permissionof the author. The author has granted a nonexciusive licence aiiowing the National Liof Canada to reproduce, loan, distniuîe or sel copies of this thesis in microform, paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantiai extracts fiom it may be printed or othenivise reproduced without the author's permission. L'auteur a accorde une licence non exclusive permettant à la Bîbliothéque nationale du Canada de reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou vendre des copies de cette thèse sous La forme de microficheJfilm, de reproduction sur papier ou sur format dectronique. L'auteur conserve la propriété du droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés ou autrement reproduits sans son autorisation. iii ABSTRACT The current study describes long-term outcomes in a sample of 64 Vietnamese-Canadian seniors related to the Vietnamese civil war, refugee migration, and life in Canada. Each participant completed questionnaires assessing: demographic features, civil war experiences, refugee experiences, protective factors (sense of preparation for the conflict, social support, and immigration experiences), and current distress (depression, nnxiety, negative changes in ouilook) and well king (Me siitisfaction, personnl and familial happiness, positive changes in outlook) ouicornes. Resul ts revealed two general patterns of adjustrnent. The firsr pa~tern wüs charücterized by a generally high level of current life sütisfiiction und current persona1 and familial happiness despite high levels of rill types of civil war experiences. Additioniilly, being married, being older, and having more children in the farnity was associated witb higher current happiness. Lower current happiness, on the other hand, was related IO being single and reporting a smüller funily size. The second general pattern of adjustment reflected witnessing and expriencing fewer civil war events, higher emotional distress during the war, more refugee experiences and higher current depression. Hriving fewer children in the hmily was also associated with higher current depression. Lower current depression was related to wiinessing more war experiences and being older. These patterns of adjustment reflect the general well-being of this sarnple of Vietnamese-Canadians, while also accounting for the distress outcomes related to the extrernely challenging process of refugee migration. The relationships between different demographic and protective factors and well king outcomes in the sample ?re also discussed. DEDICATION For the Vietnamese-Canadiüns who so generausly gave of their time, experience, and wisdom for this project. 1 admire your strength and courage. I would like to thank Dr.Soma Ganesrtn for his invaluable efforts and insights, for his enthusiasm about the study, and far making the effort to help me. i appreciate it greritly. And ihank you Regsn, for introducing us in the first place. Thanks io my senior supervisor, Dr. Marilyn Bowman for her patience and optimism throughout the duration of this project. 1 feel very lucky to have benefited from her iinwnvering cornmitment Io her students. Lastly, 1 wiint to thank my hmily and tnends from the boitom of my hem for their love and support - D, you are such an important and wonderful source ofjoy in my life. TABLE OF CONTENTS .. Approval .............................................................................................. ..II Dedication .............................................................................................. iv Acknowledgements .................................................................................... v Table of Contents ...................................................................................... vi List of Tobles ........................................................................................... ix List of Figures ........................................................................................... x INTRODUCTION .................................................................................... 1 1 Long term adjustment after chnllenging life events ................................................ Refugee experiences ........................................................................... 2 Individual di fferences in reactions to stress .........................................................3 Adaptive cognitive strategies ...........................-............................................. 3 Social support ......................... ................................................................4 Sense of coherence ..................................................................................... 5 Intervening experiences ............................................................................... 6 . Culture and experience ................................................................................ 1 Culture and communic~tingdistress .................................................................7 PTSD and distress ............................................................................. 8 Modifying our understanding of coping with challenging evenis ...............................9 Measuring adjustrnent after chrillenging events ..........................................9 Rationale of the current study ............................................................... 10 METHOD ............................................................................................ -11 Choice of Vietnamese sample ....................................................................... 11 Participant selection .......................................................................... 11 vii Participant chuacteristics ................................................................... 12 Measures of exposure to very challenging events ................................................12 Measures of types and perceived severity of expenences .......................................13 Mesure of protective factors ........................................................................14 .......................................................................... 14 Perceived social support ...................................................................... 14 Immigration experiences ..................................................................... 15 15 Measuring long term distress outcomes ............................................................. Measures of long term well-being outcornes ......................................................16 Research questions .................................................................................... 18 18 Quality of long term outcome ............................................................... The association between challenging events and outcornes ............................18 Proteçtive factors ............................................................................. 18 Data analysis ........................................................................................... 19 Multivariate analysis ......................................................................... 19 Sense of preparation RESULTS ............................................................................................. 22 Missing data ........................................................................................... 22 -22 Genenl approxh 10 analysis ....................................................................... Characteristics of participants ........................................................................ 23 Characteristics of wanime experiences ........................................................... -25 Proteciive factors ..................................................................................... 28 Sense of preparation ......................................................................... 29 Perceived level of social support ...........................................................29 Immigration rxperiences ....................................................................29 Distress and resiliency outcomes ................................................................... 30 ...........31 Reiationship between wartime experiences and well-being outcomes ........................33 Redundancy index .......................................................................... -34 Redundancy space ........................................................................... 34 Interpretation of two dimensions ...........................................................35 Relationship between exposure to different wartime expenences and well-king ............38 Gender and age ............................................................................... 47 Factors affecting the relationship between civil war experiences and well-being 48 Other background variables and outcomes ................................................ Protective factors ............................................................................. 49 DISCUSSION ........................................................................................S O Overall positive adjustment given frequency and type of experiences ........................50 52 Relationship between wartime experiences and outcomes ....................................... 52 Types of wartime events and outcomes ................................................... ......*.....52 Wartime experiences and well-being outcomes .......................... . .54 Wanime experiences and distress outcomes ............................................. Stress and anxiety ................................................................... 55 Individual difference variables ..................................................................... .56 . . Gender and well-being ......................... ....................*.......**..........* .56 Age and well-being ................................................................... 57 Family size and well-being ..................................................................58 Protective factors ............................................................................ .59 Limiiations of the current study ....................................................................-61 Sample size and characteristics ............................................................. 61 Choice of measures ......................................................................... 61 . ....................................................62 Translation ............................. Clinical implications .................................................................................. 62 REFERENCES .......................................................................................64 Appendix 1: Question assessing wartime experiences ...........................................76 Appendix 2: Questions assessing levels of protective factors ...................................77 Appendix 3: Examples of outcome measures ......................................................78 Appendix 4: One-wny Analysis of Variance for age and gender differences ..................80 Appendix 5: Histograms for sense of preparedness. perceived support. and immigration experiences. respectively .............................................................82 LIST OF TABLES Table 1: Description of participants ........................................................... 23 Table 2: Frequencies for additional demognphic features of participants ...............24 Table 3: Cumulative frequency of exposure to wartime events ............................26 Table 4: Self-reported frequency of distressing reactions to experiences ................27 Table 5: Correlations between exposure to wartime events and self-reported reiction to wartime experiences ..................................................................28 Table 6: Tüble 7: Self-ratings on pratective F~ctors.................................................. -29 Distress and resiliency outcome measiires .........................................31 Table 8: Correlaiions berween exposure and distress and resiliency outcomes .........32 Table 9: Rrlationships between wartime experiences and disiress and well-being outcornes ............................................................................. 33 Table 10: Eigenvalues .......................................................................... 35 Table II: Correlaiions of wartime experience. distress. and well-being outcomes with redundancy dimensions ..............................................................35 Table 12: Dimensional loadings of variables from wür and refugee experience and current outcomes ..................................................................... 38 Table 13: Means of background and protective variables on redundancy dimensions ...39 Table 14: Age differences in curren t happiness and depression ............................47 Tlibie 15: Point-biserial correlations between age. depression. and happiness ...........48 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 : Redundancy analysis of war expeiences. distress and well-being ...........37 Figure 2: Gender and age within the redundancy spxe Figure 4: ................................... 40 Marital and religious status within the redundancy space .....................41 Educational history within the redundüncy space ..............................42 Figure 5: Number of children within the redundancy space ............................. -43 Figure 6: Civil war and refugee expenences within the redundaticy space .............44 Figure 7: Proiective factors within the redundancy spice Figure 3: ................................. 45 Adjustment after challenging events 1 INTRODUCTION From early occupation by the Chinese, to colonization by the French, ta civil war and the intervention of the United States, the Vietnarnese people have often k e n compelled to live under extremely challenging circumstances. It was following the Ametican in!srvention in the war between North and South Vietnam that the psychological effectsof coping with such evenis were explored more fully. A substantive body of resewh has ken developed using the large population of Americin vetenns from the Vietnam War, and has contributed psychological rnodels of coping. Studies have further extended the scope of investigation to explore negative outcomes in civilian traumas, and some have even explored civilian reactions to wartime conditions. There have been comparatively few empirical studies explonng the reactions of the Vietnamese people themselves to the events of the civil war, and even fewer studies exploring long-terrn adjustment after such events. Long term adjustment after challenging life events The largest body of research pertaining to extremely stressful life events involves combat veterans - from World War il, the Korean War, and especially the Vietnam War. Some authors have reported a strong negative influence of level of combat experiences on well-being, citing a significantlyhigher probability of lifetime adjustment problems in male veterans who experienced moderate to heavy combat exposure, as compared with those veterans who did not (Spiro, Schnurr & Aldwin, 1994). Adjustment after challenging events 2 The perception of long term impairment in veteran populations resulting directly h m intense stressors is comrnon. Recent research concerning aging and well-being, however, suggests an important role of individuai resilience factors in the integration of extremely challenging events (Taft, Stem, King & King, 1998; Fontana and Rosenheck 1994; Engdahl, Harkness, Eberly, Page & Belinski, 1993). It is interesting to consider whether the role of resilience variables translates to civilian experiences with severe stressors. Refugee expenences Although the population of Amencan combat veterans is probably the most extensively studied sample of individuals coping with extremely challenging events, research using other groups has also made important contributions to out understanding of adjustment to such events. For instance, Mollica et al. (1990) reported that 700,000 Indochinese refugees had setiled in the United States in the previous 15 yeius. Refugee populations must adjust to a different constellation of challenging events than combat veterans, and often must do so in a different cultural and linguistic milieu. Exposure to torture, displacement, and migration stress al1 pose additional significant coping challenges (Holtz, 1998). It is further suggested that chronic exposure to such events may lower the threshold for th:. development of psychiatric symptoms through exacerbating premorbid conditions or contributing new pathology (Deering, Glover, Ready, Eddleman & Alarcon, 1996). The body of research showing significant individual variability in reactions to extremely challenging life events, however, suggests an important role for individual variables in the coping process. Adjusment after challenging events 3 Individual dierences in reactionsto stress There are a numkr of studies indicating that the interaction of personality, socid, and community variables results in strong individual differences in reactions to the "same" objectively stressful event (Joseph, Williams & Yule, 1995). Recent studies have identified specific predictors of adjustment in male and female Vietnam vetenns. For example, hardiness, defined by an intemd locus of control, a positive attitude towards change, and a cornmitment to life goals, was one personal resource that has k e n suggested to exert a significant mitigating effect on negative outcomes after combat exposure (Taft, Stem, King & King, 1999; King, King, Fairbank, Keane & Adwüms,1998). Different combinations of protective factors, that contribute to the ability of the individual to cope with stress, are often labeled "resilience" factors. This is a genenl term reflecting the adaptive resources of the individual, or the 'process of, capacity for, or outcome of successful adaptation despite challenging or threatening circumstances" (Masten, Best & Garmezy, 1991; 425). The constnict of resilience is multifaceted, and cesearchers have identified a number of protective factors that demonstrate individual variation in iheir effect on outcomes after challenging experiences. Adaptive cognitive strategies Some theoretical approaches define resiliency in t e m of the effects of cognitive style on the coping process. Authors suggest that appraisals of challenging events as less stressful and the use of acceptance and reframing strategies (rather than avoidance and Adjustment after challenging events 4 denial) in caping are assoçiated with improved adjustment after challenging events (Major, Richards, Cozzarelli, Cooper & Zubeck, 1995). A study of repatriated prisoners of war suggested that the use of self-isolaiion, wishful thinking, and self-blarne were al1 associated with poor long-tenn outcomes (Fairbank, Hansen & Fitterling, 1991). The cognitive resources that may encourage successful coping with stress can and do differ between individuals. This is important to Our understanding of responses to trauma, because if individuals can and do Vary in the way they fmme events. then adaptive cognitive strategies may be encouraged or acquired dunng the adjustment period. Social support Level of social support alsa shows individual variation, and has an intuitive value for those coping with stress. Social support can be considered according to objective measures, but most studies seem io rely on subjects' self-ntingsof perceived quality. Interestingly, individual differences in responses to a challenging event were relaied to differences in perceived of social support (Spasojevic, Heffer & Snyder, 2000, Mcûonald, Chamberlain, Long & Flett, 1999). Resemhers have found that resilience to distress in inner city American children at high risk for negative outcomes was partIy predicted by level of perceived support from an adult attachment figure (Masten, Best & Garmezy, 1990).A study of Tibetan refugee children found that perceived support from an adult attachrnent figure was rissociated with better adjustment to migration (Servan-Schreiber, Lin & Birmaher, 1998).Studies of former prisoners of war have found that higher levels of Adjustment after chdlenging evenis 5 perceived family and social support tendedto mitigate negative outcornes (Tail, Stem, King & King, 1999; Engdah1 et al., 1993). In cultures where social networks are particularly important, perceived social support has been identified as a buffer against negative outcomes following such challenging events as refugee migration after civil war (Blair, 2000). For instance, one study of asylum seekers fmrn a range of ethnocultural backgrounds found that of al1 current self-reported problems, loneliness, and boredom were most frequently reported and were highly related to rates of depression (Silove, Sinnerbrink, Field, Manicavasagar & Steel, 1997). Other research has suggested that the increased adaptive demands posed by stressful events do not necessarily tûx sources of social or emotional support beyond caplicity. Indeed, perceptions of social support may instead be increased in more stressful situations (Zemore & Shepel, 1989).Interestingly, other researchers have found that premorbid clipacity for interpersonal relationships (object-relatedness) was assaiiated with improved resilience against negative outcomes in a sample of combat vetemns (Kishon-Barash, Midlarsky & Johson, 1999).Taken together, these studies suggest that rather than straining existing social support resources, stressful events may insteiid lead some individuais to engage these resources in themselves and others. Sense of coherence Antonovsky (19731defined a sense of coherence as the extent to which an individual perceives that Iife events are sornewhat comprehensible,controllable, and Adjustment after challenging events 6 meaningful. Ying and Akutsu (1997) found that a sense of coherence predicted adjustment in Southeast Asian refugees, md suggested that this pmtective factor may vary between cultures. A recent study of Tibetan refugees who had endured torture showed a remiirkable lack of anxiety and depressive symptoms (Holtz, 1998).This was suggested to be partly a function of an existing sense of preparation for their flight from Tibet. Resexch involving Tibetan refugee children has also associated religious beliefs with improved coping after their long and rirduous joumey (Servan-Schreiber, Lin & Bimher, 1998). Some resexchers have suggested that extremely challenging events have a destructive influence on the resiliency normally associated with spiritual beliefs (Wilson & Moran, 1998). Accoràing to other studies, however, a sense of spirituality was suggested to enhance coping if it could improve one's ability to attribute a meaning or reason to events (Holtz, 1998). Given their histories, some cultures may be more prepared for challenging events, and may be better equipped to attribute a sense of meaning to these events. Intewening experiences In studies of various refugee populations, migration experiences have been found to predict well-being. Immigration procedures involving detention and legal proceedings tended to be associated with intensified distress and poor coping in El Salvadoran refugees (Michultk- Blanchard & Kalous, 1998). Amongst Tibetan refugees and survivors of torture, heightened anxiety was significantly related to stressful events during the joumy h m Tibet to India (Holtz, 1998). Adjustment after challenging events 7 A study of Cambodian refugees resettled in the United States found h t , in addition to wartime and refugee experiences, recent stressors (for example, financial problems) were also related to current distress (Blair, 2000). Resettlement stress and loss was found to predict depression in a simple of Khmer refugees, and the level of exposure to violence in the country of resettlement was more strongly predictive of negative outcorne than were wartime experiences (Berthold, 1999; Sack, Seely & Clark, 1997). Thus, in addition to premigration factors, different aspects of pst-migration challenges also contribute to outcomes (e.g., Nicholson, 1997). Culture and experience Although research has suggested that culture may have a fundamental effect upon one's psychological and physical experience of life events, it has only recently been considered in models of coping at'ter challenging events. Cultunl identification may influence the constnial of events, subjective qualities of experience, and adjustment after trauma. For instance, according to self-reports of Afncan-American Vietnam veterans, their most disuessing experiences involved identification with Vietnamese soldiers as a racial out-group (Green, Gme, Lidy & Leonard, 1990). Culture and communicating distress Culture modifies not only o u perception of events, but also the expression of our reactions to those events. Lee and Lu (1989) suggested that difficulties in adjusunent after Adjustment after challenging events 8 challenging events have been overlooked in many Asian-Amencans because of a relucance to access mental health care professionals. Somatic expressions of distress (Kirkmayer & Young, 1998) tend to be regarded ris the nom for Asian populations, though this finding is equivocal (Mollica et al., 1992). Our expectations or perception of another culture may lead to preconceived ides about the expression of distress following challenging life events. We may underestimate or overestimate a population's ability to cope with life-threateningevents. For instance, ment follow-up studies of Khmer adolescent refugees have found a remarkable lack of functional impairment, despite a history of life-threatening experiences (Berthold, 1999; Siick, Him & Dickason, 1999). PTSD and disîress The most commonly accepied conceptualization of reactions of distress following very challenging events involves the assessrnent of the prevdence of pst-traumatic stress disorder (WSD). The construct of PTSD was developed mainly as a result of concem about the large American Vietnam veteran population, and entered the DSM-IIIin 1980. Acwrding to a meta-analysis of studies assessing outcome in civilian samples, exposure to challenging events may increase susceptibility to a specvum of anxiety and depressive symptoms not included in the diagnostic category of PTSD (Brown, Fulton, Wilkeson & Petty, 2000). Research exploring the validity of PTSD as a cross-culturai construct has been equivocal, particuliuly in light of the fact that major categones of PTSD symptomatology are sometimes found to present without evidence of functional impairment (e.g. Berihold, 1999). Adjustment after challenging events 9 Modifying our understanding of coping with challenging events An adequate model of adjustment after extremely chaiienging events requires expansion in two geneml areas. Firstly, it is essential that we describe the breadth of reactions to such events. Although the bulk of research in this area is concemed with the identification and treatment of negative outcornes, it is important to recognize the potential for these events to exert a positive effect on well-king. Secondly, we need to develop an appreciation of the adaptive resources of individuals, in terms of their resilience to stress, or the interaction of their unique constellation of protective factors with life events. Examining long-term Juicomes may provide a perspective broad enough to attciin these goals. Mmuring adjusîment afer challenging events We have a limited, negatively biased understanding of the coping pmess foilowing very challenging events, and so our assessrnent tools tend to fomalize our preconceptions of coping after such events (e.g., Gerardi, Keane & Penk,1989). The rnajority of studies involving self-report merisures do not allow individuals to report any well-king outcornes. It is thus logical that those who have had negative, but not exclusively negative experiences, would be categorized as suffering from distress, although in reality they may have undergone both positive and negative changes as a result of their experiences. Our assessrnent tools are overly sensithe to negative outcomes after trauma,but not sensitive enough to the breadth and variation in outcomes. Adjustment after challenging events 10 RPdiorsPlc of the cumnl shrdy Based on the prernise of current negative bias in the understanding of the natureof extremely chdlenging events and subsequent adjusmnt, this study was designed to explore the differences in the quality and cmelates of well-king and distress outcornes after vety challenging events in a community sample. This study is exploratory, seeking to describe the bmdth of responses in the ability to integrnie and cope with trauma, and to reveal the operation of individual differences. Adjustment after challenging events 1 1 METHOD Choie of Vietnamese sample A substantial body of research has developed exploring the combat experiences of American veterans of the war in Vietnam, but descriptions of the experiences of the Vietnmese people in that civil war have ben comparatively rare (Lee & Lu, 1989).The Vietnamese people have had varying, but typically high levels of challenging experiences in their country of origin (Nicholson, 1997). Some studies have remarked on the resilience of this population as compared with other Southeast Asian ethnic groups, such a Cambodians and Hmong-Laotians (Fawzi, Pham, Lin, Nguyen, Nga, Murphy & Mollica, 1997; Mollica, Wyshak, Lavelle, Truong, Tor & Yang, 1990). Vietnamese people may have found their experiences to be particularly distressing, but may also have higher levels of iidaptive resources and well-king than cunent theories would assume. They might, therefore, provide useful information regarûing ihe importance of certain resilience resources for well-being outcomes. PartiEipcinï seleciion Through a Vietnamese psychiainst, 1contacted a senior citizen's organization of Vietnamese-Canadians. 1distributed questionnaires to the membership at their monthly meetings, some of which were returned to the director of the organization, and then to myself over a period of nine months. 1 pmided al1 questions in both English and Vietnamese. 1 informed those who chose to participate of their right to discontinue the study at any time without penalty, and that the psychiaûist would be available to them if the Adjustment after challenging events 12 study elicited any distress. The study was qproved by the Simon Fraser University Ethcs Review Cornmittee. P d m tc~MC&&~S The sample for the current study consisted of 64Vietnamese Canadian seniors, with an average age of 65 years. Sixty-seven percent of the sample was male, 72% were manied, and they reported an average of 10 yem of education in Vietnam. Given that the participants were contacted through a senior's social group, they may represent a segment of relatively physically healthy and high-functioning Vietnamese-Canadian elderly. Measures of exposure to very challenging events Work with Indochinese American samples (Mollica et. al., 1992, 1990) has suggested that current adjustment to challenging experiences needs to be measured not only with culturally sensitive instruments, but also with ri consideration of the effects of intervening experiences. For instance, subsequent stress during migration and resettlement may exacerbate the effects of initial wartime experiences, while successful settlement may mitigate these experiences. The current sample has endured the civil w x , refugee camps, or resettlement in Canada, so it is important to include possible effects of these experiences. Three questions assessed whether participants experienced wartime events (and for how long), whether they migraied as refugees,and whether they spent time in a refugee camp. Adjustrnent afier challenging events 13 Measures of types and perceived sevetity of experiences Mollica et al. (1992) suggested h t the fiequency of certain types of challenging events must be assessed in temis of the capacity in which they were experienced. They hypothesized that witnessing, experiencing, or hearing about distressing events, al1 have different effects on subsequent adjustment. Pre-tcsting with a comprehensive instrument thnt had k e n designed to measure the wartime experiences typical of Southeast Asian populations (Harvard Trauma Questionnaire; Mollica et ai., 1992) suggested that some individuals in the curreni sampb could not remember specific experiences with enough accumcy for an empirical study depending on such memories. Some of those who were capable of remembering their experiences with relatively greater accuncy reported experiencing displacement, starvation, confinement, npe, and torture. They reportai witnessing torture, npe, and murder of family members. Others, in contrast, reported few or no life-threatening experiences during the civil war. The lack of reliability of memory for specific events suggested that it was more appropriate to distinguish the different levels of distressing experiences in only b m d tem. Structured to be consistent with the Harvard Trauma Questionnaire, five genenl questions assessed the perceived severity of distressing experiences during the civil war (Appendix 1). Participants were asked to rate the frequency of distressing events as they were experienced, witnessed, or heard about on a 1-5 scale where one represented "none" and five represented "very many.'*They were also asked to rate the level of physical and Adjustment after challenging events 14 emotional distress associated with those civil war experiences on a 1-5 scde where one represented "none" and five reptesented "severe." Measures of protectivefactors A number of other questions have been fonnulated and included on an explontory bais, stemming from findings of related research. Sense of prepmîion Sense of coherence (Antonovsky, 1973; Ying & Akutsu, 1997). or a sense of preparation (Holtz, 1998) for challenging events has been suggested to mitigate negative outcomes. Participants were asked whether they had anticipated the events of the civil wu, whether they could attribute a reason/cause to the wiu, and whether they could see one now, looking back (Appendix 2). They rated each of these ihree questions on a 1-5 scale, where one represented "no, not at dl", and five represented "yes, very much so." Perceived social supporî The central importance of a support network has been suggested to contribute to resilience and weH-king (Sewan-Schreiber, Lin & Birmaher, 1998; Masten & Garmezy, 1991; Cowen, Wyman, Work & Parker, 1990). In light of the importance of the extended family and traditions of commund interdependence in Asian cultures (Blair, 2000), it was important to measure the perceived value of different sources of support in coping with the evenis of the Vietnam Wu. Four questions were included to assess the level of perceived social support from family, community, and spiritual faith during the war, and from ohers in Canada upon immigration (Appendix 2). Participants nted supportiveness for each Adjustment after challenging evenis 15 question on the same 1-5 d e , where one r e p n t e d "no,not at all", and five represented "yes, very much m." Immigraiion experiences Reseiuchers have rilso identified the important effect of subsequent migration experiences on disial outcornes for immigrants and refugees (e.g., Holtz, 1998).Of particular interest is the effect of immigration to Canada on long-tem outcomes. Participants were asked to evalusite whether immigration helped them to cope with what happened during the civil wat, whether the immigration process was difficult, whether it was perceived as the only choice, whether it was difficult to find employment here, and whether the choice hm been regretted (Appendix 2). Ratings were completed on the same 1-5 scale of severity, where one represented "no, not at all", and five represented "yes, very much so." Maures of long tarm distress outcornes Given the lack of consensus in the literature regarding the types of mure specific distress responses asociated with challenging events (Shdev, Freedman, Pei, Bandes & Srihar, 1997), it seemed irnponant to include a masure rismsing current levels of general depression and anxiety as most representative of current distress. The Indochinese Hopkins Symptom Checklist - 25 item version (1987) has been previously translated into Vietnamese (Indochinese HSCL-25;Appendix 3) and haç been well received by Southeast Asian patients (Smith-Fawzi, Murphy, Pham, Lin, Poole & Mollica, 1997). The instrument Adjustment after challenging events 16 includes eleven items assessing anxiety symptoms, and fourteen items assessing depressive symptoms. In clinical settings, the Indochinese HSCL-25 has been found effective in measuring symptom levels by virtue of its neutrd tone and design, matching tenns to feelings that could not be expressed as easily in open-ended interviews (Mollica et al., 1992). Symptoms are self -rated with respect to how troubling they are for the individual, ranging from "not at di"(1) to "extremely" (4). It is a stnightforwrvd measure, where the total score for each of the two scales is averaged across the number of items on each scale, and a cutoff score of 1.75 is suggested to denote clinically significant symptoms. Measures of long-term weU-being outcornes As opposed [O measures of distress, assessments of well-king and positive change arising from challenging events are relatively rare. A 1988 Greek ferry accident spumd Joseph et al. (1993) to develop the Changes in Outlook scale to assess changes in existentid beliefs d e r Me-altering or life-threatening experiences (Appendix 3). Twenty-six items include simple statements reflecting both positive and negative statementsabout such topics as one's faith in human nature, the value of relationships, and regretting the occurrence of the challenging events. Individuals are asked to rate their agreement with each statement on a scale nnging h m "strongly disagree" (1) to "strongly agree" (6). Measures of intemal consistency were estimated ai .83 for the Positive changes d e , and .90 for the Negative changes scale. In order to determine concurrent validity, Joseph, Williams and Yule (1993) comparai the Changes in Outlwk Scale with measures of negative adjustment (feelings of Adjustrnent after challenging events 17 depression, weakness, and hupelessness). They administered it with the Impact of Evem Scde (LES), the General Health Questionnaire, the Rosenberg self-esteern scde, a justworld scale, a rneasure of level of crisis support, and a measure of locus of control. lnkrestingly, there was an association between more severe perceptions of the event as reported on the ES and more reported positive changes in outlook. In the Greek feny sample, the relationship between reported positive and negative changes was not an inverse function, raising the possibility that ssiressful events may elicit both negative and positive changes. The sale has not k e n previously used with Asian cultural groups, but was chosen for the study because it allows for a range of responses after an especially stressful event, measuring both positive and negative changes in existentid beliefs. Two sets of questions were used to mesisure satisfaction with life and happiness. The Satisfaction with Life Survey (Diener, Emmons, Larsen & Griffin, 1985) hss proven reliable and valid ris a quick rating of personal satisfaction with life (Appendix 3). Pmicipiints nted dl âŹive satisfaction stiitements on a 1-5 scale where one represented "strongly disagree" and five represented "strongly agree."The total score is assumed to reflect generai life satisfactionand has ken found to show convergent validity with other masure of quality of life and satisfaction (Clark, Bormann, Cropanzano &James, 1995; Pavot & Diener, 1993). Participants were also mked to rate their current happiness on the generat questions including; the happiness of their children, their grandchildren, their family, and the importance of their family's happiness on a 1-5scrile where one represented "not happy at di",and fiverepresented "very happy" (Appendix 3). The total score an the five items is assumed to reflect gened happiness of oneself and important significant oihers. Adjustmnt after challenging events 18 Research questions Given pnor research findings, it is possible to genemte a number of general researcli questions relüted IO the current study. QuaMy of lorrg term uirtcome Firstly, as reported in pmvious reseûrch (Joseph, Williams & Yiile, 1995), it is likeiy that challenging events elicit both positive and negative outcomes. Given that the current study is designed to measure both positive and negative outcomes, it is likely that botfi will be reported. Given the inclusion of a community sample, considerable positive ouicornes mriy be reparted, rissuming participants have had a chance to integrate wartime events. The assocaàtion between challe~igirigeveiits anà outcomes Second,cumnt mdels tend to assume that outcomes ;ire most strongly and directly related to the frequency and severity of challenging events (Solomon & Davidson, 1997). As the relritionship between events and outcomes may also be iit'fected by ri number of variables with individual difterences, we might expect the correlations between events and outcomes to be low. The correlations between subjective experiences and outcomes, however, may be higher, as subjective experiences moy be affected by individual differences in personal resources, beliefs, and temperament. Adjustment after challenging events 19 Mective fmors Finally, as reported in studies of resilience to stressfui events, protective moderating factors significantly predict individud adjusmnt (Masten, Best & Gamezy, 1990). Some of the salient variables contributing to well-being in refugee or immigrant simples have been identified as sense of preparation for the events, perceived level of support, and quality of immigration experiences. Self-rritings for these variables may show that they affect the relationship between experiences on outcornes. Data analysis Descriptive statistics were genemted for most variab!es. Standard bivariate correlation techniques were used to explore the relationship between specific pairs of variables. They were interpreted in the folIowing way: magnitudes less than 0.25 represented a srnall correlation, 0.26 to 0.39 represented ;imedium correlation, and over 0.4 represented a large correlation. One-way Andysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used to determine group differencesfor specific variables. Banfermni corrections were applied to ANOVAs exploring gender differences across subsets of variables (such as the predictor set and dependent set). Otherwise, the fiunilywise e m r for post-hoc contnsts was set at 0.05. MultivPriaie analysis Given the limited interpretability of bivariate correliitions, redundancy analysis was used to assess the relationship among differeni subsets of variables. This particular anaîytic technique was deemed more appropriate hm traditional multiple regression techniques because of the large number of variables of interest and the relatively small sarnple size. As Adjusiment after challenging events 20 with canonical correlation analysis, redundancy analysis involves maxirnizing the correlation between linear composites of variable sets (Bilodeau & Brenner, 1999; Cliff, 1987). Unlike cannonical correlation analysis, which assumes symmetncal composites, redundancy analysis is an asymmetrical correlation technique. This allows us to define a directional hypothesis, in other words, we can specify subseis of predictors and dependent variables (Israels, 1984). A directional hypolhesis is presumed in the cumnt study, with wartime experience variables construed as predictors, and disîress and well-king outcornes viewed as dependent variables. As explained by Wollenberg (1977) then, redundancy analysis describes composites of predictors that mriximize the explained variance in composites of the dependent variables, in addition to accounting for the relationships within multivariate dependent sets. The composites tepresent the dimensions of overlap between two variables sets. The overlap in these two sets is quantified by the redundancy index, which is the average squared multiple correlation of the dependent variables with the predictors (Israels, 1984).Redundancy analysis then decomposes this index into more f;uniliiu eigenvalues. The correlation of individual variables with each dimension can be interpreted in a sirnilar manner to loadings derived in factor analysis or pnnciple components andysis (Wollenberg, 1977). Thus, with a sample size of less than 100, only correlations beyond +/.30 will be considered statistically meaningful (Dillon & Goldstein, 1984). If the first two redundancy composites account for a suitable proportion of variance, variable loadings cm be plotted as vectors. Logically, longer vectors correspond to larger loadings (Israels, 1987). The location of the categories of pertinent demographic and Adjustment after çhallenging events 2 1 protective factors within the redundancy space can aiso be plotted to evaluate their relationship to the predictors and dependent variables. When categorical vuiiibles are ploned in redundancy space, confidence circles must be added to data points in order to evaluate whether group differences are significant. The squared distance for projections of caiegorkal variables into the redundancy space follows a chi-squared distribution on 2 degrees of freedom (Lem, Morineau & Warwick, 1984). Confidence circles for the present demognphic and protective factors can thus be colculated uring the chi-squared critical point set at the 95' prcentile (5.99). When divided by the sample size for each group, the radius of the confidence circle is defined. The significance of group differences for demographic and protective factors with respect to the redundancy composites can be evaiuated through obsewing the overlüp between confidence circles. Circles with considerable overlap suggest thai their ongins are not significantly different from one another, whereas circles with no overlap suggest significantly different origins. Adjustment after challenging events 22 RESLJLTS Missing data Out of twenty-nine variables, seven participants had missing data for one variable because they did not provide an estimate of the duration of their experience of civil war events. Given that this variable was inçluded for solely descriptive purposes, these participants were excluded only from analyses of that variable. G e n e d approach to analysis Given the paucity of standardized instruments assessing refugee experiences, many of the questions in the study were exploratory in nature. As such, descriptive anaiyses were most appropriate for the majority of the items completed by participants. The results section wili first ptesent a description of the sample, followed by descriptions of measures of civil wrir and refugee experiences. Subsequent tables describe participant self-ratings on questions assessing protective factors as well as distress iuid resiliency outcomes. The correlations between wiutime experiences and these outcomes are presented. Lastly, multivariate analyses of the relationships between wartime experiences, protective factors, and distress and resiliency outcomes are presented. Adjustment dter challenging evenis 23 Characteristlcs of the participants Sixty-fourVietnarnese-Canadians completed the package of questionnaires, and were included in the study. Descriptions of the participants are shown in Table 1 and Table 2. There were no significant age or gender differences with respect to groups on other demographic variables (one-way Andysis of Variance (ANOVA), see Appendix 4). Table 1: Descri'kn of partici'nts. Variable N Mean SD Min Max Age (Y-) 64 65.1 7.2 44 81 Children 64 4.0 2.6 O Il Educational level (years in Vietnam) 64 10.3 5.4 O 20 Educational Ievel (years in Canada) 64 0.2 0.6 O 3 Time in refugee camp 64 (yem) 1.1 1.3 O 4 57 Time in wartime conditions (months) 16.1 9.2 2 40 The average age of the sample (65.1 years, S.D. = 7.16) indicated an appropriate age gmup for the assessment of long-term adjustrneni after very challenging events that = 1.3) of occurred in Vietnam miuiy years ago. Reports of an average of 1.1 years (S.D. intemment in a refugee camp, and 16.1 months (S.D. =9.2) enduring aversive events Adjustment after challenging evenu 24 related to the civil war also suggests that many participants have encounted such chdlenging events. Table 2: Fmquenciesfor additional dernogrophicfeatures of participants. Variable N Percent of Sample (rounded) Gender Males Females 43 21 67 33 46 7 2 9 72 11 3 14 39 9 4 2 10 61 14 6 32 17 12 2 50 27 19 3 24 39 38 61 Maritu1 Status Mamied Single Divorced Widowlwidower Reiigious Afiliation Buddhist Christian Ancestor Worship Other None Occupational Szat~rs Civilian Soldier Civil servant Panmilitary Language Status Fluent in 2+ Ianguages ESL training in CDA 3 16 Most of the participants were men (67%). most were mmied, and most reported a spiritual practice of some type (84%). Adjusunent after challenging events 25 Participants reported whether or not they were exposed to any or dl of three types of wartime events. Table 3 reflecis the cumulative frequency of exposure to three kinds of aversive events, those related to: living under conditions of civil war, refugee migration, and living in refugee camps. There were no significant age differences in exposure to these différent kinds of events (see Appendix 4). An exiunination of gender differences in exposure to the three kinds of events showed significani differences only in self-report of living under conditions of civil w u (F = 41.68, p < 0.001). OvemII, 77% of the male participants, and only 10% of the fernale participants reporîed this type of exposure. Adjustment after challenging events 26 Table 3: Cumulativefiequency of exposure to wartrhe ewn& Variable N Percent of Sample (rounded) ho expositre: 16 25 One type of exposure only: Lived under aversive wartime conditions only: Migrated as a rafugee only: Spent tirne in a refugee camp only: Two types ofexposure: Wutirne conditions and refugee migration: Wartime conditions and refugee camp: Refugee migration and refugee camp: 12 O O 1 1 12 Al1 rhree types of evenrs: 22 34 Total: 64 100 The variation in exposure reported by participants is notable, such that 25% reported no exposure, 22% had experienced iwo of rhe three types of events, while 34% reported exposure to al1 three types of evenis. Participants described the level of distress experienced during events related to the civil war, shown in Table 4. They were asked to rate the frequency of distressing events that they heard about, witnessed, and experienced. They were then asked to rate the levels of physicd and emotional distress associated with those experiences. AI1 ratings used the same 1-5 scale, where 1 represented "none", and 5 represented Very many" distressing events or "very strong" physical and emotional distress. There were no significant age or gender Adjusment after challenging events 27 diffetences between the Enqucncy of self-reprted distresshg cxperiences across these çive questions (ANOVA Tables in Appendiic 4). Table 4: se^-repoifedjkquency of distressing nacw tu exptriences N Mean SD Min Max Distressing events heiird about 64 4.0 0.7 2 5 Distressing events witnessed 64 3.7 0.8 1 5 Distressing events Experienced 64 3.0 1.2 1 5 Physical distress 64 1.6 0.9 1 5 Emotional distress 64 3.5 1.O 1 5 Variable Civil war As one would predict, participants report4 a higher frequency of distressing events that they heard about than disnessing events h t they witnessed or experienced. Of interest, self-reported emotiona! distress elicited by the events exceeded self-reported physical distress. The relationship between participants' self-report of exposure to the three kinds of wartirne experiences and self-reported frequency of distressing experiences during the war was explored with point-biserial correlations (Table 5). Adjustment after challenging events 28 C o m W n s between exposure to wartime events and se&repooried remlion to watüme exphences. Variable Living under wartime conditions Refugee migration Living in a refugee camp Distressing evenb heard about 0.18 O. 10 Distressing events witnessed 0.23 0.0 1 Distressing events experienced 0.18 -0.09 -0.06 Physical distress 0.25* O. 18 0.15 0.35** 0.33** Emotional distress 0.36** Note: * Q c 0.05 (2 - tailed) ** p < 0.0 1 (2 tailed) 0.05 - In terms of responses to these different wartime experiences, there was a medium correlation between emotional distress and al1 types of experiences, while living under conditions of civil war showed a medium correlation with only physical distress. Protective Factors Participants rated themselves on questions related to three different protective or resiliency factors, as shown in Table 6. No age or gender differences were found on any cornparisons using one-way Analyses of Variance, so data for both genders were combined. Adjustment after challenging events 29 Table 6: Serf-rotliiggs on proleclivef~turs. Mean SD Min M; Sense of preparaiion 64 9.6 3.3 3 15 Perceived support 11.1 3.3 4 18 Variable N 64 Sense of preparahgon Three questions (each rated on a 1-5 scale) assessed the sense of preparation for the civil war felt by participants. High scores (to a maximum of 15) indicated îhat the participant reprted a sense of prepamtion for the war, and an understanding of its occurrence. The average score over the three questions of participants was 9.6 (S.D. = 3.3). Pemeived lewl of social support Participants rated the perceivecl level of support h m such sources as family, community, and spiritual faith on four questions (each rated on a 1-5 scale for a maximum of 20). Higher scores indicated a stronger sense of socid support dunng the civil war and subsequent migration. The average score over the four questions of participants was 1 1.1 (S.D.= 3.3). lnrntimon experiences Five questions (each rated on a 1-5 scala for a maximumof 25) assessed the quality of participants' experiences in their immigration to Canada; higher scores indicated more positive migration experiences. The participants' average score over the five questions of was 15.9 (SD. = 3.2). Adjustment after challenging events 30 It is interesting that the majority of participants' seif-reportecl protective factors fell above the rnidpoint of the scales (frequency histograms are found in Appendix 5). Distress and resiliency outcornes To evaluate both positive and negûtive aspects of current adjustment, psirticipanffi mted thernselves on a number of q u ationnaires. Anxiety and depression were assessed - using the Hopkins Symptom Checklist 25 items (HSCL-25, Moilica et al., 1990).The Changes In Outlook Scale (CIO, Joseph, Williams & Rule, 1993) measured both negative and positive changes in philosophical beliefs since the civil war, while the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS,Diener, Emmons, Larsen & Griffin, 1985), and five genetal exploratory questions assessed self-mted personal and familial happiness. An examination of age differences for the six different outcome variables showed differences only for happiness. In particular, older participants tended to provide higher niings of happiness. There were no significant gender differences on any distress and resiliency uutcomes (see Appendix 4), so these outcome measures for genders are combined in Table 7. Adjustment after challenging events 3 1 T& 7: Distress and resiliency outcome measures. Variable N Mean SD Min Mis HSCL-25 (anxiety) 64 1.5 0.4 1.O 2.4 HSCL-25 (depression) 64 1.5 0.5 1.O 3.7 CIO (positive) 64 4.1 0.7 1.9 6.0 CIO (negative) 64 2.1 0.8 1.O 5.1 SWLS 64 17.5 4.2 5 25 General happiness 64 21.0 3.6 10 25 The average self-reported rating for current miety symptoms of 1.5 (S.D. = 0.4), was of a sirnilar magnitude as the average mting for current depressive symptoms of 1.5 (S.D. = 0.5). It is significant that boih measures of distress fell below the suggested clinical cutoff of 1.75 (Mollica et al., 1992). It is also important thai a paired samples -test indicated significantly more positive than negative changes in outlook (r = 16.68, Q < .001). High scores on both the SWLS and broad questions assessing personal and familial happiness indicated generally positive current adjustment. Relationship between exposure to different wartime experiences and weU-king Participants pmvided self-ratings for exposure to three kinds of challenging life events, and for both distress and resiliency outcomes. Point-bisenal correlations were Adjustment after challenging evenu 32 calculated to explore the nature of the relationships between these exposures and outcomes re!ated to current well-king (Table 8). Cmlalions h e e n exposure and distress and resiliency outcomes. Variable Living under wartime conditions Refugee migration Living in a refuge CmP HSCL -25 (Anxiety) HSCL - 25 (Depression) CIO (positive) .15 .O3 O . 1 CIO (negative) -.O9 .25* .26* -.44** -.44** Genenl happiness -.O2 Noie: * p < 0.05 (2 - tailed) ** p < 0.01 (2 - tailed) Medium correlations between self-report of both refugee migration and time spent in a rebgee camp and negative changes in outlook were noted. Additionally, there were large negative correlations with these two exposures and current persona1 and familial happiness. Of note, having lived under wartime conditions was not a significant correlate of current well-being or distress. Adjustment ;ifter challenging events 33 Relstionship between wartime experiences and distress and weU-being outcomes In order to evaluate the relaiionship between wartime experiences and current wellking outcomes, Spearman's &J was calculated (see Table 9). Table 9: ReMonships beîween wariime expmfences ami dkiwss and well-being outcomes. Variable Heard about Witnessed (events) (events) Experienced Physical (events) distress Emotional distress .22 .31* .26* .5 1** .32* HSCL-25 Depression .10 .Il .IO .36** .27* CIO Positive -.O8 -.O4 -.16 -.14 .O4 CIO Negative -.12 -.13 -.12 .24 .14 SWLS -.16 -.22 -.31* -.25* Happiness -.16 -.17 -.14 -.O8 -.22 -.a** HSCL-25 Anxiety Note: * p c 0.05 (2 - tailed) ** p < 0.01 (2 - tailed) Distress reiictions, more than piuticular t y p of wartime expenences, were associated with distress and well-being outcomes. There was a large correlation between physical distress during the war and current anxiety symptoms, a medium correlation with current depressive symptoms, and a medium negative correlation with satisfaction with life. Emotional distress dunng the war showed a large negative correlation with cumnt happiness, moderate correlations wiih cumnt anxiety and depression, and a moderate Adjustment after challenging events 34 negative conelation with satisfaction with life. Self-report of distressing events witnessed and experienced showed medium correlations with current anxiety. This pattern suggests a significant relationship between both emotional and physical distress elicited by wartime events and level of current distress, in ternis of anxiety ;tnd depression. Hearing about wartime experiences showed no significant correlations with cumnt distress and well-being outcomes, and changes in outlook (CIO) were not correlateci with any rneasures of wartime experiences. Scatterplots of experience ratings and outcome measures fail to show strong linear relationships for most of the correlations between wartime experiences and outcome measures, with the previously mentioned exceptions. Redundancy index The redundancy index was .236, indicating that 24% of the variance in outcome measures (changes in outlook, anxiety, depression, life satisfaction,cumnt personal and familial happiness) could be accounted for by linear dependence on the predictor set (hearing about, witnessing, experiencing civil w u events, experiencing physical and emotional distress). Redundancy spce According to the eigenvalues presented in Table 10, the first two dimensions sumrnarize 63%of the overlap between the two variable sets. Adjustment &er challenging events 35 Table 10: - - Dimension 1 Eigenvalue 0.44 The loadings of wutirne experiences and outcome varinbles on redundancy dimensions are included in Table 11. Table 11: Correlations of wam*meexpience, dimss and well-being outcornes witir redundancy dimensions. Variable L d i n g on first redundancy dimension dimension Physical distress during war Emotional distress during the war No experience with events of civil war Exprienced events Heard ribout events Cumnt happiness Witnessed events Experiencing events of civil war Did not migrate as a refuge Did not spend time in a refugee camp Positive Changes in Outlook Negative Chimges in Outlook Satisfaction With Life Anxiety (HSCL-25) Depression (HSCL-25) .86 .73 - -.57 .56 .56 .45 -38 .35 -33 -.3 -1 -.29 Loading on second redundancy -.10 .30 -.15 -.56 - -.29 -.12 -.56 .O3 -.45 -.43 -.25 -.27 .22 .O4 .O 1 .O7 .18 .31 -.16 Adjusant after challenging events 36 A plot of these loadings presented graphically in Figure 1. The predictors plotted in redundancy spacc are: 1) level of civil wat events heard about, 2) level of civil war events witnessed, 3) level of civil wac events experienced, 4) physicai distress during the civil war, and 5) emotional distress during the civil war. The dependent variables included in the redundancy analysis are: 1) negative changes in outlook (CIO),2) positive changes in outlook (CIO).3) current anxiety, 4) current depression, 5) Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS),and 6) cumnt happiness. Adjustment aftet chaiienging events 37 Adjusünent after challenging events 38 Variables with high loadings on the fmt dimension were: types of wartime experiences (hearing about, witnessing, and experiencing wartime events), physical distress and emotional distress during the war, and current happiness. Variables with high loadings on the second dimension were: refugee experiences, witnessing and experiencing wartime events, emotional distress during the war, and current depression. These two most dominant patterns are presented in Table 12. Dimensional loadings of variablesfronr wrrr and rcfugee experience and current outcornes. First dimension Second dimension Wartime experience high civil war experiences high hearing about war events high witnessing war events high experiencing war events high physical distress during conflict high emotiond distress during conflict high refugee experiences Outcome variables high current happiness high emotional distress during conflict low witnessing war events low experiencing warîirne events high =fugee experiences high current depression Factors affecting the relationship between civil war experiences and weU-beiig Means on the First and second dimension were calculated for the categories of background variables that might be expected to affect the relationship between predictors and outcome variables. Self-ratingson proteciive factors were split into low, moderate, and high levels for sense of preparation and perceived social support, and low and high levels for immigration experiences. The means for al1 caiegoncal variables are included in Table Adjustment alter challenging evenis 39 13. Graphicd representations of the means of demographic and protective variables in - redundancy space are found in Figures 2 7. Table 13: Mmm of backgmumi and prorective YlViObIes on duRdartcy dimensions. Variable Means on tirst redundancv dimension Means on second redunhncv dimension Demographic variables: Male Femlr: Age 60 yervs or less Age 61-70 yerus Age 7 1-80 yem Mamed Single Religious affiliation No religious affiliation 5 y e m or less education 6- 10 yem of education 1 1- 15 yem of education More than 15 y m of education Less than 2 children Less than 6 children Six or more children Proteetive factors Sense ofpreparation: b w sense of preparation Modemte sense of prepmiion High sense of prepmtion Perceived support durittg conflic?: Low perceived support Modemte perceived support High prceived support Qualiîy of immigration e.rperiences: Paor immigration experiences -.54 Positive immigration experiences .13 -.12 Adjustment after challenging events 40 Adjustment after challenging events 41 Figure 4: Educational history within the iedundancy space. -0.7 Dimension 1 Adjustment afler challenging events 43 Adjustment after challenging events 44 Figure 7: Protective factors within the redundancy space. 0.8 low imm high sense prep 0.8 mod social support low social support u 0 . 6 mod sense Dimension 1 Adjustment after challenging evenis 46 The background variables whose categories were clearly separated on the first dimension were: gender, maritai status, age, educational history, and number of children. Roteciive factors whose levels were ciearly delineated on the first dimension included sense of preparation, perceived sacial support, and immigration experiences. Specifically, lower levels of al1 civil war experiences together with lower current happiness was associated with king female, single, older than 70 years of age, having less than five years of education, and more than six children. They were also related to reponing a low sense of prepmtion for the conflict, low perceived support, and p r immigration experiences. Higher civil wiu experiences and higher cumnt happiness, on the other hand, were related to modemte perceived support. Background variables whose categories were clearly separated on the second dimension were: age, educational history, and number af children. Protective factors whose levels were clearly delineated on the second dimension included a sense of preparation, and perceived social support, In particular, witnessing and experiencing more civil war events and reporting lower current depression was associated with being older than 70 years of age, having a modente educational history (6-10 y m ) , having more than six children, a modente sense of preparation, and low perceived support during the conflict. Reporting having witnessed and experienced fewer wûrtime events, more refugee experiences, and higher current depression, on the other h d , was related to reporting fewer than three children and having an educational history of more than fifteen years. WhiIe redundancy malysis provides a global understiinding of the patterns of relationship between two sets, it does not provide detailed information about the Adjustment after challenging events 47 relationship between pairs of variates. In following sections, results from such detailed analyses will be pmvidd. Genàer and age The correlation between gender and age was not significant, so their relationships with the outcornes of current happiness and depression were assessed independently. There were gender differences in self-reported happiness (j= -2.2, p = 0.03),where women reported significantly higher levels of cumnt happiness than men. This finding seems to contradict Figure 2 showing fernale gender corresponding to lower scores on the sarne dimension showing high scores for current happiness. It should be noted, however, that high scores on experiences during the civil war had the highest loadings on the first factor, and women reported significantly fewer civil war experiences than men. tt may be that the mean for femole gender on the first redundancy variate, in reflecting their distribution relative to the pattern of variables loading on the first dimension, actually speaks to their low scores on distressing wartime experiences, nther than lower current happiness. A one-way ANOVA (Table 14)showed an age effect for both depression and happiness. Point-biserial correlations are included in Table 15. Table 14: Age difletences in cutrent happiness and depression. Variable Curreni happiness Current depression F-value P-level of significance Adjustment after challenging events 48 Increasing age was assaciated with significantly higher current happiness and significantly lower current depression. Cornparhg group means using Tukey's HSD technique indicated significantly lower current happiness for those 60 years of age or younger compared with the those aged 61 to 70 and those over 70, but no significant differences between the two older groups. This seerns contrary to the low mean for those over 70 on the first dimension, which is characterized by a high loading of current happiness. As with gender, it is likely the case that the mean for those over 70 reflects their self-reported pattern of wartime experiences, specifically less physical distress during the civil war (which had a pnrticularly high loading on the fint dimension). Depression was significantly higher for the younger group when comprired with the two older groups, but again no significant differences emerged between h e two older groups. Table 15: Point-biserial comkibns between age, depression, und happiness. Variables Happiness Depression Age 0.57** -0.34** Note: ** p < 0.0 1 (2 - tailed) Despite a large positive correlation between age and happiness and a medium negative correlation between age and depression, the estimated effect sizes were modest (0.36 for happiness and 0.16 for depression). M e r imckpund vuriuùles and outcornes There were significant differences in current happiness according to marital status, with those who were rnanied neporthg greater happiness (t = 2.1, p = 0.04). A one-way ANOVA also showed significantdifferences in current happiness associated with number Adjustment after challenging events 49 of children (F= 5.8, p = 0.01), with those reporting more children also reporting higher current happiness. Post-hoc contrasts showed significantly lower current happiness reporied by individuals with less than three children, but no significant differences in current happiness between groups having three to five or more than five children. Although the point-bisenal correlation between number of children and current happiness was moderate ( r = 0.36, p < 0.01). the estimated effect size (0.14) was small. Further analyses of variance showed no significant relationships between family size and depression, marital status and depression, and level of education, happiness, and depression. Mective factors A one-way ANOVA showed significant differences in current happiness associated with social support during the conflict (F = 3.8, p = 0.03). Post-hoc contrasts suggested a counterintuitive pattern, where hose reporting low perceived social support during the conflict showed significantly higher cwrent happiness than those reporting modente social support during the conflict. Differences in current happiness between those reporting higher social support during the conflict and those reporting low and moderate social support were not significant. It should be noted, however, ihat the point-biseriai correlation between social support and happiness (-0.21)iuid the esiimated effect size (0.09) were small. Andysis of variance showed no significant differences in happiness associated with sense of preparation or immigration experiences, and none of the protective factors were associated with significant differences in current depression. Adjustment after chailenging events 50 DISCUSSION Overall positive adjustment &en frequency and type of experiences The self-reported frequency of distressing events experienced by participants during the civil war, as well as the physical and emotional distress elicited by those events, was variable. The nature of these experiences was not specifically assessed, although pre-testing with a more detailed questionnaire suggested many would be considered life-threatening (for example, witnessing others king murdered). The frequency of these types of selfreported experiences for many individuals would be congruent with commonly assumed precursors for long term distress outcomes following extremely challenging events (Mollica et al., 1992). Contrary to this assumption, however, the participants in the sample reported a genenlly high level of genenl satisfaction with life, and high current personal and familial happiness. Furthemore, on a measure of changes in philosophical beliefs following expenences in the civil war, participants reported significantly stronger positive than negative changes. Rather than discounting the bulk of research suggesting high levels of distress following extremely challenging events (e.g., Nicholson, 1997; Mollica et al., 1990), these findings broaden the scope of our understanding of the coping process. They are largety consistent with resettlement resemh with Vietnamese populations in Norway showing that "it is possible to cope with important and complex îasks in exile...[even with] painful memories of the p s t and other distressing symptoms" (Hauff & Vaglum, 1997, p. Adjustment after challenging events 51 414). In other long-tem follow-up studies, such as those including Holocaust survivors, reseiuchers have found a sirnilar pattern of current positive adjustment and well-king despite symptorns of distress (Sadavoy, 1997). It is important to consider the possibility that the pattern of adjustment in this sample may be representative of normative long-term responses after extremely challenging events. Some authors hypothesize that coping with such events should be considered fmm the perspective of lifespan development (Spim, Schnurr & Aldwin, 1997). They suggest devoting considenbly more attention to the potentid role of individual difference variables in shaping adjustment after challenging events. The current sample is a community sample, as opposed to the clinical samples often indu&d in research studies of reiictions to challenging events (e.g., Deering, Glover, Reiuiy, Eddleman & Aliucon, 1996).It may be that individual difference variables in community populations who are not seeking treatment services contribute to higher well-king outcomes than are conunonly reported in studies of clinical samples. Additionally, the period of time that has elapsed since exposure to civil war events may have mitigated immediate negative outcomes folluwing those events (Aldwin, Levenson & Spiro, 1994; Fontana & Rosenheck, 1994). Other long-term follow-up studies of American Vietnam vetemns (Spiro, Schnurr & Aldwin, 1997) have described the presence of robust well-king outcomes, contmdicting the common assumption that a linear relationship exists between stressful events and distress outcornes (Berghem, Brmner, Southwick, Charney & Krystal, 1997). Adjustment after challenging events 52 Reiationship between wartbne experienm and outromes There is a complex and interesting relationship between types of wartime events, wartime experiences, and distress and well-being outcomes. Types of warthe events and outcomes In contrast to generally positive adjustment, refugee migration and living in a refugee camp were correlated with lower satisfaction with life and more negative changes in outlook. Although these are not psychiairic symptoms, experiencing less general personal satisfaction with life and holding fewer positive philosophical beliefs may indeed have functional consequences. It might, for instance, affect the ability or willingness of Vietnamese immigrants to make social connections in their new country, which other studies have suggested as an important precursor to positive adjustment (Hauff & Vaglum, 1994).In the redundancy analysis, not king forced to migrate as a refugee, and not spending time in a refugee camp was distributed in a similar manner as lower current depression. This finding is consistent with research suggesting that the process of forced migration as a refugee poses considerable challenges to personal coping resources, above and beyond the challenge of wartime stressors (Blair, 2000; Nicholson, 1997; Sack, Seeley & Clarke, 1997; Silove et al., 1997). Wrutime experiences and well-king outcomes The relationship between the Frequency of chailenging civil war experiences and current persona! and familial happiness suggests a countenntuitive pattern. Higher scores on ail five measures of wartime experiences were related to higher current happiness. In Adjustment after challenging events 53 contrat, lower scores on witnessing and experiencing events of the civil war, but higher emotiond distress during the conflict were related to higher current depression. As has been suggested in other studies, individual difference variables, rather than objective level of exposure to an extremely stressful event, may play an important role in determining long-term outcomes (Taft, Stem, King & King, 1999; Aldwin, Levenson & Spim, 1994). It may be that the level of emotional distress experienced during very challenging events is a more useful predictor of long-terni adjustment that any objective measure of exposure to stressors. Indeed, other studies of Vietnamese populations have found that certain especially emotionally distressing experiences (such as tirne spent in forced "education" camps) predicied disiress outcomes (Hauff & Vaglum, 1994). The correspondence between higher levels of al1 types of wartime experiences, greater physical and emotional distress during the wu, and higher current personal and familial happiness could have a number of explanations. in their review of extremely stressful events and the elderly, Weintmub and Ruskin (1999) suggest that successful negotiation of the stressor at the time it occurs is associated with future well-being. In other words, this could indicate an immunization effect, whereby more ment stressors are successhlly negotiated because of coping skills developed during remote experiences, leading to greater happiness. Research reiated to the constructs of both hardiness and sense of coheretice construe stressful events in a more positive light, as challenges to be met by the coping resources of the individual (e.g., Antonovsky, 1973). It may also be the case that irnmigrating to Canada and making great personal sacrifices to ensure the safety of family members may increase individuals' expectations that they would become happy. Some long-term coping studies do suggest a sense of Adjustment after challenging events 54 increased gratitude and an awareness of cunent resources developing as a result of past stresses or deprivations (e.g., Spiro, Schnurr & Aldwin, 1997). It may also be the case that immigrating to Canada and making great persona! sacrifices to ensure the safety of f d y members may increase expectations that they would therefore be happy. As a result, an individual's estimation of the happiness of his or her family members may be elevated because the expectations of familial happiness are higher. In other studies, marital status h a been identifid as a diable predictor of wellking in elderly samples (Weintnub & Ruskin, 1999; Kehn, 1995), and is certainly assoçiated with higher personal and familial happiness in the current sample. These findings, taken together, may reflect the salutogenic effects of family relationships for members of the current sample in their ability to cope with extremely stressful events. Wumorneexpiences and distress oirlcornes Numerous researchers have suggested that frequent and distressing experiences exert negative long-term effects or symptoms (Brown, Fulton, Wilkeson & Petty, 2000, Sack, Seeley & Clarke, 1997). Given that the average self-reportecl levels of anxiety and depression were below suggested clinical cutoff scores, however, actual functional problerns were not indicated for the majority of this sample. The prevalence of clinically significant depressive symptoms (22%) in this m p l e is considerably higher than other estimates of 8- 10% prevalence in community samples of Vietnamese refugees (Hinton, Jenkins, McPhee, Wong, hi,Le, Du & Fordham, 1998).Even so, the high number of selfreported well-being outcornes suggest ihat even in the presence of anxiety and depressive mood symptoms, positive adjustment is possible. Other studies of Viemamese refugees, for instance, show general well-king and coping efficacy even in the presence of symptoms of A d j u s a n t after challenging evenn 55 disbess (Hauff & Vaglum, 1997), This is also consistent with tesearch studies of d e r cultural groups (such as the Khmer) showing that clinically signif~cantpsychiatrie symptoms can be reported without evident functiod impairnent (Sack, Him & Dickason, 1999; Berthold, 1999; Hauff & Vglurn, 1997). The hypothesis that adaptive long-tenn reactions to exüemely stressful events involve both distress and well-being autcomes is not a commonly held tenet in the literature on coping with extremely challenging events, although it is beginning to be addressed. In addition to studies showing b t h distress and well-king outcomes in American veterans (Spiro, Schnurr & Aldwin, 19971, there is a small but signifiant body of litennire that discusses coping with extremely stressful events in ternis of a negotiable challenge to the individual's coping resources. Rather than exhausting an individuai's protective factors, these researchers believe that stressful experiences allow these msources to be realized and enhanced (Park,1998). This highlights the importance of exploring ihe pmess of long- term adjustment afier such events and ensuring that the breadth of outcomes is recognized for each individual. sîress and anxieo In addition, it may be important io rnodifj our undersîanding of those who do show long terni distress outcomes and impaired hinciioning after very challenging events. Such difficulties are typically subsumed by the North American diagnostic taxa of anxiety disorden. Given the orthogonal lmding of outcome mewures of depression and happiness in the current study, however, these outcorne measures may be more useful than anxiety in summarizing long-tem variation in individuai responses to extremely stressful events. It is interesthg to note other researchers have deve1oped depression - happiness d e s as Adjustment after challenging events 56 reptesentative of the continuum of affective responses to life events (McGreal & Joseph, 1993). Resemh including samples of Vietnamese refugees awaiting placement in Vietnsun has found that depression as rneasured by the Hopkins Symptorn Checklist-25 item version is a reliable indicator of distress (McKelvey & Webb, 1997). Furthermore, studies of Vietnamese refugee samples in the United States have highlighted depression as an identifiable and important correlate of post-migration stressors (Hinton et al., 1998). Individuai differerne variables The dishbution of demographic and protective factors in redundancy space provides an interesting description of their relationships with wartime experiences and outcornes. Other studies of coping with extrernely stressful events in Vietnamese samples ~ P suggested S that demogmphic variables exert a significant effat on well-king (Tm, 1993). Gender and well-being Although the bulk of North AmeFicm resemh suggests a much higher prevalence of clinical depression for women than men, this assumption does not appear to generdize across cultures or genentions. An international review of epidemiological studies of depression fwnd ihsit in less developed countries and in older genentions, gender differences in prevalence of depression have not been significant (Culbertson, 1997). In the cumnt siudy, in fact, women reported significantly greater happiness and did not report higher current depression hm men,suggesting that comrnody-held assumptions about gender differences in distress and well-being may not translate effectively to dl siunples. Adjustment after challenging events 57 In fact, other community studies of depression in Viemamese refugee populations have found lower prevalence of depression (Hauff & Vaglum, 1993) as well as other mental health problems (Matsuoka, 1993) for women than men. The authors suggested that fernale gender operated as a pmtective factor because it was associated with an increased propensity for making social and occupational connections with the host population. It may be that female rehgees and immigrants are both more sacially flexible and more immediateiy employable (e.g., as domestic labour) than male refugees, which contributes to well-king. Age and well-being The current study found that those younger than sixty years of age reported significantly less current happiness and significantlygreater current depression than those older than sixty yem of age. Physical health studies are more typical of the researcli literature on the well-king of elderly populations, and a number of studies suggest that with increasing age, decreûsing physical heaith becornes the most significant predictor of well-king (Krirel, 1997). In contrast, the current study replicates the findings of research with other East Asian populations, suggesting that, ai least in soçieties with strong values of filial piety, increasing age can show strong associations with well-being outcomes (Pei & Pillai, 1999). In a hierature review of common cultural thernes of Southeast Asian refugees, kinship solidruity emerged as partîcularly characteristic of these cultures (Frye, 1995). It may be that elderly individuals from these cultures benefrt significantly from strong family relationships. It may also be that bis partidly reflects cohort differences in general adjustment or differences in expectations of satisfaction. As previously mentioned, epiderniological Adjustment after challenging evenis 58 studies of depression suggest increasing prevalence in younger cohorts (Culbertson, 1997). Other studies exploring the prevalence of depression in Vietnamese refugee samples has replicated this finding that increasing age is associaied with lower depression (Beiser & Fleming, 1986). It may be that older generations hold lower expecilitions for happiness, and their current levels of satisfaction are more likely to rneet or exceed those expectations. This may translate into higher self-perceptions of happiness for older generations. Conversely, younger individuals miiy show lower self-peived hiippiness as a result of incongruent expectations for happiness and the mual possibilities within North American society. Altematively, the relationship between age and current happiness may reflect an association between age at the time of the stressful events and future adjustment. Some mearchers suggest that the life experience associated with age itself is a protective factor when coping with extremely stressful events (Weintraub & Ruskin, 1999). According to this theory, ihose who were older when they experienced wartime events might show greater long-term well-being. Family site aiid well-bei~ag This may also be the most intuitive explanation for the higher distress outcornes in a subsample of individuals characterized by family structure including fewer children, which is ftequently assumed to be economically advantageous, and thus a protective factor (McKelvey, Webb & Mao, 1993).The expectations of this particular group may have k e n the highest upon entenng Canada, and as such may have been most likely to be disappointed by the limited opportunities afforded by immigrant status in a new country. Adjustment after challenging events 59 Numerous studies including refugee populations have suggested that resettlement stressors play a predictive role in current adjustment (Nicholson, 1997). This implies that we need to reconsider our assumptions about the protective benefits of certain demographic variables. It may be more important to evaluate demographic variables in terms of the degree of congruence between expectations and probable achievements. For instance, higher soçiĆconomic status rnay not be a protective factor for rehgees because it may result in unrealistic expectations (for instance, pertaining to standard of living) in a new country of resettlement. The finding that a larger nurnber of children in the family is related to higher happiness contmts some research including elderly community-dwelling North American populations, suggesting that family size does not predict self-reported well-king and satisfaction (Kehn, 1995). In other cultural groups, however, particularly those with smng intergenerational ties and frequent contact between young and old rnembers, family relationships are particularly important for psychological well-king (Pei & Pillai, 1999). It may indeed be the case that larger families are a protective factor for elderly Vietnamese, particuliuly when coping with highly stressful events. Proteciivefactors: psychological Given the research evidence supporting the importance of a sense of coherence or meaning for integrating extremely stressful events (Ying & Akutsu, 1987) and for predicting satisfaction in later life (Sagy, Antonovsky & Adler, 1990), it is surprising that ii sense of prepamtion did not seem highly related to positive adjustment. It is most likely that the questions included in this study to assess this constmct were in fact measuring an awareness of possible justifications for the civil war. This sense of meaning may be a more Adjustment after challenging events 60 logical, but less personal, sense of coherence, which appears to be unrelated to distress and well-king outcomes. Similarly, it was surprising that the quality of immigration experiences was weakly related to outcome measures. The bulk of recent research involving various refugee populations has found that negative immigration experiences contribute to distress outcomes (Silove et al., 1997). It is possible, however, that other factors such as immediaie employrnent problems and financial stressors are more strongly related to distress and wellking outcomes. Such a relationship has been suggested in other studies which show strong associations between ment stresses (within the past year) and major depression in Cambodian refugee populations (Blair, 2000). The curvilinear relationship between levels of social support during the war and current happiness was somewhat surprising. Rather than a linear association between level of social support during the war and current happiness, it appeared that a modente level of social support was most strongly related to moderate current happiness, while low social support was associated with higher happiness. Because most research supports a fairly strong relationship between social support and well-king following very challenging events (Servan-Schreiber, Lin & Birmaher, 1998), it may be important to consider other explanations for the current findings. It is most likely that the relationship between social support and happiness has been made spurious by the high loadings of civil war experiences on the first dimension. It is possible that lower levels of civil war experiences corresponded strongly to a lower need for social support during the war, resulting in the lower mean on the first dimension. Altematively, perceptions of low social support at the time of the conflictcould be associated with more negative perceptions of Vietnam at the Adjustment after challenging events 61 time of the war, and may related to higher current happiness in Canada as a country of residence. Limitations of the current study Sanrple size and chamcteristics Although the pattern of associations between variable sets described in redundancy analysis was suggestive of significant relationships between wartime experiences, protective factors, and current well-being outcomes, further quantitative analyses showed consistently small effect sizes assaciated with al1 of the significant multivariate relationships. Given the large error terms evident in the ANOVA calculations, it is likely that the sample had insufficient power to detect what might have been significant population differences. Given that participants for the study were voluntarily recruited from a senior citizen's group, a certain segment of the population of Vietnamese-Canadian seniors may have been oversampled. Even though a number of participants reported clinically significant symptoms of distress, it may k the case that the current sample represents a particularly healthy population with relatively high well-king. Indeed, the current study may have unintentionally excluded those who, because of limitations or heatth concems, could not participate in the seniors' p u p . Choiee of measures Adjusiment after challenging events 62 The selected variables included in the current study accounted for approximately 24% of the variance in well-king and distress outcomes. It is likely that other variables, such as personality fearures, that would be expected to affect outcome variables, were not included in the study. With the considerable period of time elapsed since the civil war, multiple variables would be expected to affect current adjustment. On one hand, it is important to assess long term responses to stressful events in order to evaluate normative pmesses of integration and coping. On the other hand, the more remote the event, the greater the range of intervening variables likely to impact the coping response. It is difficult to design a study of long - term coping that encompasses the full range of predictive factors, and particularly when measuns must be trandated, some potentinlly important measures rnay be sacnficed for the sake of efficiency. TmnsIrih'on Although measures were either standardized translations, or translated and backtranslated versions of questionnaires, it is possible that minor translation errors may have occurred. It may also be the case that subtle nuances of certain common English phrases or concepts do not iranslate completely into Viemamese, which rnay have changed the implied meaning of certain questions or phrases. Participants reported genenlly high levels of well-being outcomes, despite some clinically significant symptoms of depression. The finding that refugee experiences are associated with higher distress outcomes is consistent with previous research. The Adjusûnent after challenging events 63 relationship between more civil war experiences and strong well-king outcomes, on the other hand, is counter-intuitive. In addition, the association between female gender and higher happiness, as well as incmased age, higher happiness, and Iower depression is not typicrtl of the health and coping Litenture. Both larger family size and being rnarried were also found to be iissociated with higher happiness in the sample. The pattern of well-king of Lhis particular gmup of participants has implications for Our understanding of adjustment and health in the elderly. it is signifiant that this sample has endured exuemely stressful events in the pt,and currently reports high levels of well-king. This my be a more accurate reflection of expectable well-king outcomes in community populations. It miiy riiso reflect the adjustment of a parlicularly healthy, socially active segment of Vietnamese-Candian seniors. In either case, the finding that family size and marital stritus are related to well-king highlights the importance of close social support networks ruid social activities for this popuhtion. This suggests thai we may need to shift the focus of treaiment of distress in elderly Vietnamese-Canadianstowards incorporating family and community networks. It also appem that detnographic variables, such as isge and gender, operate differently thm in Nonh A m r i c m samples. This implies that Our understanding of the ielationship between certain individual characteristics and welI-king rnay need to account for both individd and cultuml differences. In other words, effeçtive treatment may need to recognize and capitalize on the imporiance of specific demographic variables for certain cultures. Adjustment after challenging events 64 REFERENCES Abueg, F.R. & Chun, K.M. (1996) Tnumatization stress among Asians and AsianAmericans. In A.J. Marsella et ai. (Eds.) Ethnocultural aspects of wst-traumatic stress disorder: issues. research, and clinical aamlications(pp. 285-299). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association. Alarcon, R.D.,Deering, C.G., Glover, S.G., Ready, D.J.& Eddieman, H.C. (1997). Should there be a clinical typology of pst-traumatic stress disorder? Australian and New Zealand Joumal of Psvchiatrv. 31(2), 159-167. Aldwin, C., Levenson, M. & Spim, A. (1994). Vulnerability and resilience to combat exposure: can stress have lifelong effects? Psvcholo~vand Aging. 9( 1),34-44. Beiser, M. & Fleming, S.A. (1986). Measuring psychiatrie disorder among Southeast Asian refugees. Psvcholo~icalMedicine. 16,627-639. Berghem, T., Bremner, J.D., Southwick, S.M., Chmey, D.S.& Krystal, J.H. (1997). Neurobiological perspectives on trauma and aging. Journal of Geriauic Psvchiatry, 30( 1), 27-59. Berthold, M.S. (1999). The effects of exposure to community violence on Khmer refugee adolescents. Joumal of Traumatic Stress. 12(31455-471. Bilodeau, M. & Brenner, D. (1999). Them of multiviate statistics. New York: Spnnger-Verlag New York, Inc. Blair, R.G.(2000). Risk factors iissociated with PTSD and major depression among Cambodian refugees in Utah. Health and Social Work. 25( 1123-30. . Adjustment after challenging events 65 Brown, E.S.,Fulton, M.K.,Wilkeson, A. 8t Petty, F. (2000).The psychiatrie sequelae of civilian trauma. Comorehensive Psyhiatry. 41(11 19-23. Caihoun, L.G. & Tedeschi, R.G. (1998). Pos~umaticgrowth: future directions. In R.G. Tedeschi, C.L. Park & L.G.Calhoun (Eds.) Posttraumatic growth: positive changes in the aftermath of crisis (pp. 213-258). New Jersey: Erlbaum Associates. Clark, K.K., Bormann, C.A., Cropnzano, R.S. &James, K. (1995). Validation evidence for three coping measures. JoumaI of Personalitv Assessment. 6332 434-455. Cliff, N. (1987). Analyin~multiviuiate data. San Diego: Harcourt Bmce Jovanovich, Publishers. Cowan, P.A., Cowan, C.P. & Schulz, M.S.(1996).Thinking about risk and resilience in families. In E.M.Hetherington & E.A. Blechman (Eds.) Stress. coping. and resiliencv in children and families (pp. 1-38). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cowan, E., Wyman, P., Work, W. & Parker, G. (1990).The Rochester Child Resilience Project: overview ruid summary of first year Eindings. Develo~mentand P s v c h o p a t h o l o ~193-212. Culbertson, F. (1997). Depression and gender; An international review. American Psvcholo~ist. 52( 1),25-3 1. Davidson, J. (2000). Pharmacotherapy of pst-traumatic stress disorder: Treatment options, long-term foilow-up, and predictors of outcome. Journal of Clinical Psvchiatrv, 6 1(5), 52-59. Deering, C.G., Glover, S.G., Ready, D., Eddleman, H.C. & Alarcon, R.D. (1996). Unique patterns of comorbidity in pttraumatic stress disorder from different sources of trauma. Comorehensive Psvchia-. 37(5), 336-346. Adjustment dter challenging events 66 Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, R. & Griffin, S. (1985).The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personalitv Assessment. 49,7 1-75. Dillon, W.R. & Goldstein, M. (1984).Multivariate analysis: Methods and anplications. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Engdahl, B.E., Harkness, A.R., Eberly, R., Page, W. & Bielinski, J. (1993). Structural models of captivity muma, resilience, and trauma response among former prisoners of w u 20 to 40 years after release. Social Psvchiatrv and Psvchiatric Epidemioloev. 28, lO9-1 15. Fairbank, J.A. ,Hansen, D.J. & Fitterling, J.M. (1991).Patterns of appraisal and coping across different stressor conditions among former prisoners of war with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consultine and Clinical Psvcholoev. - - 59(2),274- 281. Fawzi, M.C., Pham, T., Lin, L., Nguyen, T.V., Ngo, D., Murphy, E. & Mollica, R.F. (1997). The vaiidity of pst-traumatic stress disorder among Vietnamese refugees. Journal ofTnumittic101-108. Flannery Jr., R.B. & Flannery, G.I. (1990).Sense of coherence, life stress, and psychological distress: a prospective methodological inquiry. Journal of Clinical Psvcholoev. 46(4B 4 15-420. Foa, E.B.(2000). Psychosocial treatment of pst-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psvctiiahy. 61(5143-5 1. Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Higgitt, A. & Target, M. (1994). The Emanuel Miller Mernorial Lecture 1992:The theory and pmctice of resilience. Journal of Child Psvcholoev and Psvchiatrv. 35(2), 23 1-257. Adjustment after challenging events 67 Fontana, A. & Rosenheck, R. (1994). Traumatic war stressors and psychiatric symptoms among World War II, Korean, and Vietnam war veterans. Psvchologv and king. 911L 27-33. Frye, B.A. (1995). Use of cultural themes in promoting health among Southeast Asian refugees. American Journal of Health Promotion, 9(4), 269-280. Gerardi, R., Keane, T.M.,Penk, W. (1989). Utility: sensitivity and specificity in developing diagnostic tests of combat-related pi-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Journal of Clinical Psvcholo~v,45(51695-703. Goenjian, A.K., Steinberg, AM.,Najarian, L.M., Fairbanks, L.A., Tashjian, M. & Pynoos, R.S. (2000). Prospective study of pst-traumatic stress, anxiety, and depressive reactions after earthquake and political violence. American Journal of Psvchiatrv, 157(65, 895-91 1. Goldsmith, H.H., Buss, K.A. & Lemery, K.S.(1997). Toddler and childhood temperament: expmded content, stronger genetic evidence, new evidence for the importance of environment. Develo~r!Ientûl~svchomthoiogv.33(61891-905. Green, B.L., Grace, M C ,Lindy, J.D. & Leonad, AC. (1990). Race differences in response to combat stress. Journal of Traumtic Stress. 3(3), 379-393. Hauff, E. & Vaglum, P. (1997). Establishing social contact in exile: A prospective community cohort study of Vietnamese refugees in Norway. Social Psvchiatrv and Psychiairic E~idemiology,32(7), 408-415. Hauff, E. & Vaglum, P. (1994). Chronic posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnamese refugees. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 182(2), 85-90. Adjustment after challenging events 68 Hauff, E. & Vaglum, P. (1993). Vietnamese boat refugees: The influence of war and flight traumatization on mental health on arriva1 in the country of resettlement: A community cohort study of Vietnamese refugees in Norway. Acta Psvchiatrica Scandinavia. 88(3), 162-168. Hinton, L., Jenkins, C.N., McPhee, S., Wong, C., Lai, K.Q.,Le, A., Du, N. & Fordham, D. (1998). A survey of depressive symptoms among Vietnamese-American men in three locales. Journal of Newous and Mental Disease. 186(112677-683. Holtz, T.H. (1998). Refugee trauma versus torture trauma: a retrospective contmlled cohort study of Tibetan refugees. The Joumal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 186(1 12434. Humphreys, L., Westerink, J., Giamtano, L. & Brooks, R. (1999). An intensive treatment program for chronic pst-traumatic stress disorder: 2-year outcome data. Austdian and New Zealand Joumal of Psvchiatw, 33(6), 848-854. Israels, A. (1984). Redundancy andysis for qualitative variables. Ps~chometrika, 49132 331-346. Israels, A. (1987). Redundancy analysis. In Eigenvalue techniaues for aualitaiive d;ita (pp. 179-222). Leiden: DSWO Ress. Joseph, S. & Williams, R. (1995). Psychosocial perspectives on pst-traumatic stress. Clinical Psycholoev Review, 15(6)., 5 15-544. Joseph, S., Williams, R. & Yule, W. (1993).Changes in outlook following disaster: the preliminiuy development of a measure to assess positive and negative responses. Journal of Traumatic Stress. a22 271-279. Adjustment after challenging events 69 Karel, M.J. (1997). Aging and depression: Vulnerability and stress across adulthood. Clinical Psvcholorzv Review. 17(8), 847-879. Keane, T.M. & Wolfe, 1. (1990). Comorbidity in pst-traumatic stress disorder: an analysis of comrnunity and clinical studies. JournaI of A ~ ~ i i Social e d Psvchologv, 20, 1776-1788. Kehn, D.J.(1995). Predictors of elderly happiness. Activities, Ada~tation& Agina 19(3), 11-28. King, L.A., King, D.W., Fairbank, LA., Keane, T.M.& Adams, G A . ( 1998). Resilience-recovery factors in pst-traumatic stress disorder among female and male Vietnam veterans: hardiness, postwar social support, and additionai stressful life events. e o f 420434. Kinzie, D.J., Manson, S.M., Vinh, D.T.,Tolan, N.T., Anh, B. & Pho, T.N. (1982). Developrnent and validation of a Vietnarnese-language depression rating scale. Amencan Journal of Psvchiatrv. 139(1Oh 1276-1281. Kirmayer, LA. & Young, A. (1998). Culture and somatization: clinical, epidemiological, and ethnographic perspectives. Psvchosomatic medicine. 60,420-430. Kishon-Barash, R., Midliusky, E. & Johnson, D.R. (1999). Altruism and the Vietnam war veteran: The relationship of helping to symptomatology.Journal of Traurnatic SU~SS. 12(41655-662. Krell, R. (1993). Child survivors of the Holocaust - strategies of adaptation. Canadian Journal of Psvchiatrv. 38,384-389. Adjustment aAer challenging events 70 Lebart, L., Monneûu, A., & Wanvick, K.M. (1984). Multivariate descriptive statistical analvsis: Comswndence analvsis and ~ l a t e dtechniaues for l m e matrices. New York: Wiley. Lee, E. & Lu, F. (1989). Assessment and treatment of Asian-American suwivors of m s violence. Joumal of Tmumatic Stress. 2( 1),93- 120. Lewis, C.A., Shevlin, M.E.,Bunting, B.P. & Joseph, S. (1995). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Satisfaction with Life Scale: replication and methodological refinement. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 80( 11304-306. Lyons. J.A. (1991). Stntegies for assessing the patentid for positive adjustment following trauma. Journal of Tmumatic Stress, 4( 1), 93- 111. MacDonald, C., Chamberlain, K., Long, N. & Flett, R. (1999).Post-traumatic stress disorder and interpersonal functioning in Vietnam war veierans: A mediational model. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 12(41701-707. Major, B., Richards, C., Cozzarelli, C., Cooper, ML. & Zubek, J. (1998). Personal resilience, cognitive appraisals, and coping: an integrative mode1 of adjustment to abortion. Journal of Personalitv and Social Psvcholo~v.74132 735-752. Mûsten, A.S., Best, K.M. & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: contributions from the siudy of children w ho overcome adversity.Development and P s v c h o ~ a t h o l 425-444. o~~ Matsuoka, J. (1993). Demographic characteristics as deierminants in qualitative difference in the adjustment of Viemamese refugees. Journal of Social Service Research, 17(3L 1-2 1. Adjustment after challenging events 7 1 McCubbin, H.I., McCubbin, M.A., Thompson, A.I. & Thompson, E.A. (1998). Resiliency in ethnic families: a conceptual mode1 for predicting family adjustment and adaptation. In H.I. McCubbin, E.A. Thompson, A.I. Thompson & J.E. Fromer (Eds.) Resiliencv in Native American and immigrant families (pp. 3-48).Thousand Oaks, C.A.: Sage Publications. McGreiil, R. & Joseph, S. (1993).The depression-happinessscale. Psvcholoe;icd Rewrts. 73, 1279-82. McKelvey, R. & Webb, J. (1997). Comparative levels of psychological distress in a pre-migratory tefugee population. Austnlian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatw. 3 1, 543-548. McKelvey, R. Webb, J. & Mao, A. (1993).Prernigratory risk factors in Vietnamese Amernians. Amencan Journal of Ps~chiritrv,150432470-473. Mghir, R. & Raskin, A. (1999).The psychological effects of the war in Afghanistan on young Afghan refugees from different ethnic backgrounds. International Joumal of Social Psvchiatw. 45( 12 29-36. Michultka, D., Blanchard, E.B. & K;ilous, T. (1998).Responses to civilian wiu expenences: Predictors of psychological functioning and coping. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1 113L 571-577. Mollica, R.F., Caspi, Y.Y., Bollini, P. & Truong, T. (1992).The Hmiud Trauma Questionnaire: validating a cross-cultural instrument for rneasunng torture, trauma, and pst-traumatic stress disorder in Indochinese refugees. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 180(2), 1 1 1- 1 16. Adjustment after challenging events 72 Mollica, R.F., Wyshak, G., Lavelle, J., Tmong, T,Tor, S. & Yang, T.(1990). Assessing symptomchange in southeast Asian refugee suwivors of mass violence and torture. American l w d of Psvchiae. 14711 1 83-88. Mollica, R.F., Wyshak, G.,de-Marneffe, D. & Khuon, F. (1987). Indochinese - versions of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist 25 items: A scmning instrument for the psychiatrie c m of refugees. A m e h J 87-93. Nicholson, B.L. (1997). The influence of pre-migrationand post-ernigration stressors on mentd health: A study of Southeast Asim refugees. Sacial Work Resêarch, Zlll), 19-3 1. Ortega, A.N. & Rosenheck, R. (2000). Posttraumatic stress d i s d e r among Hisprinic Vietnam veterans. Amencan loumal of Psvchiatrv. 157(4), 615-619. Page, W.F., Engdithl, B.E. & Eberly, R.E. (1997). Persistence of PTSD in former prisoners of war. In C.S.Fullerton & R.J. Ursano (Eds.) Post-traumatic stress dimrder: acute and long-tenn resrionses to trauma and disaster (pp. 147-158). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatrie Press. Park, CL. (1998).Implications of posttraumatic growth for individuals. In R.G. Tedeschi, C.L.Park, & L.G. Calhoun (W.) Positraumtic erowth: psitive changes in the aftennath of crisis (pp. 154- 177). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Park, CL,Cohen, L.H. & Murch, R.L. (1996). Assessment and prediction of stressrelatedgrowth. Journal of Personalitv. 6 4 1),7 1-105. Pavot, W. & Deiner, E. (1993). Review of the Satisfaction wiih Life Scale. Psvchotoeicai Assesment. 5(2),164-172. Adjustment after chdlenging events 73 Pei, X. & Pillai, V. (1999). Old age support in China: The role of state and the family. Intemaiional Joumal of Agin~!and Human Develo~ment.49(31,197-212. Sack, W.H.,Him, C. & Dickrison, D. (1999).Twelve-year follow-up study of Khmer youths who suffered massive war trauma as chilàren. Joumal of the American Academv of Child of Adolescent Psvchiatry. 38(9), 1173-1179. Sack, W.H.,Seeley, J.R. & Clarke, G.N. (1997).Does WSD transcend cultural biuriers? A study from the Khmer adolescent refugee project. Journal of the American Academv of Child and Adolescent Psvchiatrv. 36( 1149-54. Sadavoy, J. (1997).A review of the late-life effects of prior psychological trauma. American Joumal of Geriatric Psvchiaîry. 5,287-301. Sagy, S., Antonovsky, A. & Adler, 1. (1990).Explaining life satisfaction in later life: The sense of coherence model and activity theory. Behavior. Health & Aning. 1( 1), 1 1- 25. Servan-Schreiber, D., Lin, B.L. & Birmaher, B. (1998).Prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder and major depressive disorâer in Tibetan refugee children. Journai of the Arnerican Academv of Child and Adolescent Psvchiatry. 37GL 874-879. Shalev, A.Y., Freedman, S., Pen, T., Brandes, D., Sahar, T., OIT, S.P.& Pittman, R.K. ( 1998).Prospective study of pst-traumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma. Amencan Joumal of Psvchiatrv. 155(5h 630-637. Shalev, A.Y., Freedman, S., Pen, T., Brandes, D. & Sahar, T. (1997).Predicting PTSD in tnuma survivors: Prospective evaluation of self-report and clinician administered instruments. British Joumal of Psvchiatrv. 170,558-564. Shalev, A.Y., Bonne, O. &EthWS. (19%). Treatment of pst-traumatic stress disorder: A review. Psvchosornatic Medicine. 58,165-182. Silove, D., Sinnerbrink, I., Field, A., Manicavasagar, V. & Steel, 2.(1997). Anxiety, depression, and PTSD in asylum-seekers: Associations with pre-ernigration trauma and pst-emigration stressors. British Journal of Psvchiatrv. 170C42 351-357. Smith-Friwzi, MC, Murphy, E., Pham. T., Lin, L., Poole, C. & Mollica, R.F. (1997). The validiiy of screening for pst-irautnatic stress disorder and major depression and Vietnamese former political prisonen. Acta Psvchiatrica Scandinavica, 95(2), 87-93. Solomon, S. & Davidsun J. (1997). Trauma: Revalence, impaiment, service use, and cost. Journal of Clinical Psvchiaiw. 58(Su~ol.9 1 5-1 1. Spasojevic, J., Heffer, R.W.& Snyder, D. K. (2000). Effects of pst-traumatic smss and acculturation on marital functioning in Bosnian refugee couples. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1W), 205-217. Spiro, A., Schnurr, P. & Aldwin, C. (1997). A life-span perspective on the effects of rnilitary service. Journal of Geriatric Psvchiatrv. 30( 1),9 1- 128. Spiro, A., Schnurr, P. & Aldwin, C. (1994). Combat related posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in older men. Psycholo~vand Agine. 9(12 17-26. Steiner, A., Raube, K., Siuck, A.E., Aronow, H.U., Draper, D., Rubenstein, L.Z. & Beck, J.C. (1996). Measunng psychosocial aspects of well-king in older comunity residents: performance of four short scales. The Gerontologist. 36( 1 154-62. Taft, C.T., Stem,A.S., King, LA. & King, D.W.(1999). Modeling physical hedth and functional hedth status: The role of combat exposure, pst-traumatic stress disorder and personal resouce attributes. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 12111 3-23. Adjusbnent after challenging events 75 Tedeschi, R.G.& Calhoun, L.G.(19%). The Post-traumatic Growth Inventory: Measuring the positive legacy of trauma. Joumal of Traumatic Stress. 9(3),455-472. Tran, T.V. (1993). Psychological traumas and depression in a sarnple of Vietnamese people in the United States. Health & Social Work. 18(3), 184-194. Wagner, A.W., Wolfe, J., Rotinsky, A., Proctor, S.P. & Erickson, D.J. (2000). An investigation of the impact of pst-traumatic stress disorder on physical health. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13(1),41-55. Weintraub, D. & Ruskin, P.E.(1999). Posttraumatic stress disorder in the elderly: A review. Harvard Review of Psvchiatrv. 7,144-152. Wilson, B.J. & Gottman, J.M.(19%). Attention - the shuttle between emotion and cognition: risk, resiliency, and physiological bases. In E.M.Hetherington & E.A. Blechman (Eds.) Stress, co~ingand resiliency in children and families (pp. 189-228).New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wilson, J.P. & Moran, T.A. (1998). Psychological trauma: Post-tnumtic stress disorder and spirituality.Journal of Psvcholoev and Theology. 26(2), 168- 178. Wollenberg, A.L. (1977). Redundancy analysis: An aitemative for canonical correlation analysis. Psvchometrika. 42(2&207-22 1. Ying, Y. & Aksutu, P.D. (1997). Psychological adjustment of southeast Asian refugees: the contribution of sense of coherence. Journril of Cornmunitv Psvcholo~v.2X21 125-139. Zemore, R. & Shepel, L.F. (1989). Effects of breast cancer and mastectomy on emationril support and adjustment. Social Science and Medicine, 28(12 19-27. Adjustment after challenging events 76 Appenàii 1: Questions awedng wartime experiences 1. How many distressing events did you: Hear about during the conflict in Vietnam? 1 2 3 ne vciy few mmt Wimess during the conflict in Vietnam? 1 2 3 Ilme very rw solne Experience during the conflicf in Vietnam? 1 2 3 none very few YMT 2. How strong was your: Physical distress as a resuli of those events? 1 2 3 mnr ramcwhai s m g Emotional distress as a result of those events? 1 2 3 none wmwhpiymwig 5 vrry smng 5 very stmng Adjustment after challenging events 77 Appendlx 2: Questions assesslngIeveis of protecîive factors Did you feel prepared for what happened in Vietnam? 1 2 3 4 no. noiiuall 5 ~cs."eryprcpiirrJ At the time in Vietnam, could you see a go& reason for the war? 1 2 3 4 y=. no. noi m oll Can you see a reason for that war, looking back? 1 2 3 4 g m d rcaron 5 yes. a v a y g d mrwn no. nnt iu $1 Perceived level of s 5 O very i i m At the time you were in Vietnam, did your family help you to cope with what was happening? 1 2 3 4 5 yu. vrry r w h W. IWW PL uII At the time you were in Vietnam, did your community help you to cope with what was happening? 1 2 3 4 5 no, niiiu $1 p.very mich At the time yau were in Vietnam, did your religion or spiritual faith help you to cope with what was happening? 1 2 3 4 5 yts, vciy much no, nef af dl Did you find enough support from others when you came to Canada? 1 2 3 4 5 ycs, vciy much no.rociu;i11 Immigration exwriences Did your immigration to Canada help you to cope with what happened? 1 2 3 4 5 ycs. vciy mwh no. MC iu dl How difficult was immigration to Canada? 1 2 3 4 Did you feel that immigration was your only choice? 1 2 3 4 4 al1 the timc How easy was it to find employment in Canada? 1 2 3 my ==Y 5 no. 1h;d &r choices yes. it w;r~ihr only choicc Do you regret your immigration to Canada? 1 2 3 5 midificult v a y diffiiulf 5 never 4 5 vcry diflieulf Adjusrment after challenging events 78 Appendix 3: Examples of outcorne measwes Hopkins Symptom Checklist, Indochinese Version (Mollica et d.,1992) Anxiety symptoms: 2. Feeling fearful: 5. Trembling: Depression symptoms: 13. Crying easily: 18. Feeling blue: not at al1 2 a little 3 quiie a bit 1 not at al1 2 a little quite a bit extremely 1 not at al1 2 a little 3 quite a bit 4 extremely 1 not at riIl 2 a litîle 3 quite a bit L 4 extremely 3 4 4 extremely Changes in Outlook Scale (Joseph et al., 1993). Positive chimges: 5.1 value my relationships much more now. 1 2 3 s m g l y âiigme 13.1 have greater faith in human nature now. 1 2 3 5 Negative changes: 9.1fear death very much now. 1 2 6 suongly ü g m rimngly diagicc 3 - % I Y &grce 15.1 desperately wish 1 could tum the dock back before it happened. 1 2 3 4 5 s m g l y lliim The Satisfaction Wirh Life Sirwey (Diener et al., 1985). 1 = Strongly disagree 2 = Disagree 3 = Neither agree nor disagree 4 = Agree 5 = Strongly agree 1. In most ways, my life is close to my ideal. 2. The conditions of my life are excellent. 3.1 am satisfied with my life. 4. So far, f have gdten the important things 1 want in life. 5. If I could Live mv life over. 1 would change almost nofhing. Adjustment &r challenging events 79 Questions assessing current personal and familial happiness. How happy are you now? 1 2 whippy u l How happy are your childien? 1 2 -happy 3 aiail How happy are your grandchildren? 1 2 3 noi happy )idl How would you rate the happiness of your frunily? 1 2 3 noihappyudl How important is your farnily's happiness to you? 1 2 3 na4 imptnîmt ît îII Adjustment after challenging events 80 Appendix 4: One-way Analysis ofVariance tables for age and gender Weronccs Age differences acmss variables Variable group F-value p-level of significance Age vs. Exposure to wartime events Migration ris a refugee Spending time in a refugee Camp Age vs. Events heard about Events witnessed Events experienced Physical distress Emotional distress Age vs. Sense of preparation Perceived support immigration experiences Age vs. HSCL - 25 (anxiety) HSCL - 25 (depression) CIO (positive) CIO (negative) SWLS Happiness 3.91 O.OO* Note: Bonferroni correction applied for ANOVA for each variable group. * p c O.ûû8 Adjustment after chdlenging events 81 Variable p u p Gender vs. Enduring civil w x experiences F-value plevel of significance Refugee migration Refugee camps Gender vs. Events heard about Events witnessed Events experienced Physical distress Emotional distress Ge~idervs. Sense of preparation Perceived support Immigration cxperiences Gender vs. HSCL - 25 (anxiety) HSCL - 25 (depression) CIO (positive) CIO (negative) SWLS Happiness 4.90 0.03 * Note: Bonferroni correction applied for AVONA for each variable group Adjustment after challenging evenis 82 Appendix 5: H i i m for sense of preparedness,perceived support, and immigration experiences, respectively SENSPREP SUPPORT