Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 C 2004) Journal of Chemical Ecology, Vol. 30, No. 6, June 2004 (° CHEMICAL SIGNALING IN A WOLF SPIDER: A TEST OF ETHOSPECIES DISCRIMINATION J. ANDREW ROBERTS∗ and GEORGE W. UETZ Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio 45221, USA (Received August 12, 2003; accepted February 16, 2004) Abstract—Chemical signals from female wolf spiders that elicit exploratory behavior and courtship in males are often assumed to be species-specific, but males of some species court in response to silk cues deposited by closely related heterospecific females. Such is the case with the wolf spiders Schizocosa ocreata and S. rovneri, ethospecies reproductively isolated on the basis of differences in behavioral mechanisms during courtship. We explored whether male S. ocreata and S. rovneri reciprocally discriminate species-specific chemical or mechanical cues associated with female silk by using male behavioral response as an assay. Males were exposed to stimulus treatment categories including silk, washed silk, silk extract, and appropriate controls within conspecific or heterospecific female stimulus categories. Male S. ocreata and S. rovneri did not discriminate between conspecific or heterospecific female stimuli, and courtship intensity was greatest on untreated silk. There were no differences in latency to begin courtship or in rates of courtship behaviors attributed to species origin of silk. However, silk treatment (washed silk, extract) had a significant effect on display and exploratory behaviors (e.g., chemoexplore) in both species. Methanol extraction of female silk successfully removed or inactivated a component necessary to elicit active courtship, but extraction did not significantly reduce exploratory behavior, suggesting that a separate compound may be responsible for releasing this behavior. Together, these experiments support the characterization of S. ocreata and S. rovneri as ethospecies reproductively isolated only by female discrimination of species-specific male courtship, and indicate that chemical, but not mechanical cues associated with silk are critical for eliciting male courtship in both species. Key Words—Behavioral assay, chemical communication, Schizocosa, Lycosidae, wolf spider, species discrimination. ∗ To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected] 1271 C 2004 Plenum Publishing Corporation 0098-0331/04/0600-1271/0 ° P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 ROBERTS AND UETZ 1272 INTRODUCTION Spiders, like most arthropods, use chemical signals to mediate many types of interactions. These interactions may include prey detection, predator avoidance, social recognition, and courtship (reviews in Tietjen and Rovner, 1982; Uetz, 2000; Barth, 2002; Uetz and Roberts, 2002). Male spiders may produce compounds that alter the behavior of conspecifics, either in the context of male/male competition by suppression of activity in other males (Tietjen, 1979; Ayyagari and Tietjen, 1987), or in mating behavior by either releasing female receptivity displays (Ross and Smith, 1979) or inducing a cataleptic state in females prior to copulation (Singer et al., 2000). However, most studies of chemical signaling in spiders have focused on compounds associated with the silk and/or cuticle of females and the role these putative pheromones play in mate attraction and release of exploratory and courtship behaviors (Tietjen and Rovner, 1982; Pollard et al., 1987; Riechert and Singer, 1995; Trabalon et al., 1997; Prouvost et al., 1999; Papke et al., 2001). In wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae), there is evidence that females can attract males with airborne chemical signals (Tietjen, 1979; Searcy et al., 1999), but most chemical signaling appears to be via contact chemical compounds associated with silk. Conspecific female silk and associated pheromones are sufficient to elicit normal courtship behavior in males of many species, even in the absence of all visual and vibratory cues (den Hollander et al., 1973; Tietjen and Rovner, 1982; Stratton and Uetz, 1983; Costa and Capocasale, 1984). While the chemical signals from female wolf spiders that elicit exploratory behavior and release courtship in males are thought to be species-specific (Hegdekar and Dondale, 1969; Tietjen, 1977; Costa and Capocasale, 1984), males of closely-related species will sometimes court the silk of heterospecifics (Kaston, 1936; Hegdekar and Dondale, 1969; den Hollander, 1971; Miller et al., 1998). Such is the case with males of the two wolf spider species used in this study, Schizocosa ocreata (Hentz) and S. rovneri (Uetz and Dondale). These species can be considered “ethological species” (Dobzhansky, 1970) and have been described as “ethospecies” (Uetz and Denterlein, 1979), that is, species that are isolated reproductively only on the basis of differences in behavioral mechanisms occurring during courtship (den Hollander and Dijkstra, 1974), with no other prezygotic isolating mechanisms (Uetz and Denterlein, 1979; Stratton and Uetz, 1981, 1983, 1986). Schizocosa ocreata and S. rovneri prefer different microhabitats but overlap in geographical range and habitat use (Uetz and Denterlein, 1979; Stratton and Uetz, 1981; McClintock and Uetz, 1996), and are essentially seasonally synchronous (Uetz and Denterlein, 1979; Roberts, J. A., unpublished data). Male S. ocreata do not show evidence of territoriality, and may travel great distances each day during some portions of the breeding season (linear distance of up to 15 m/hr— Roberts, J. A. and Uetz, G. W., unpublished data), increasing chances of encounter P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 CHEMICAL SIGNALING IN A WOLF SPIDER 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 1273 with both conspecifics and heterospecifics. In fact, males allocate much of their time to searching for females in the complex habitat (Cady, 1984). There is no evidence of mechanical or gametic incompatibility, as hybrids of these two species have been successfully produced in “forced” copulation studies in laboratory experiments (Stratton and Uetz, 1986; Orr, M. and Uetz, G. W., unpublished data). The genes that code for courtship behavior do not assort independently so that otherwise viable hybrids suffer behavioral sterility, and no known natural hybrids have yet been identified (Stratton and Uetz, 1986). Females of these species are receptive only to species-specific visual and vibratory courtship cues (Uetz and Denterlein, 1979; Stratton and Uetz, 1981, 1983, 1986; but see Uetz and Roberts, 2002), a finding that is not surprising as females mating with heterospecifics would suffer fitness consequences related to behaviorally sterile offspring. Males of both species have been demonstrated to be equally likely to court both conspecific and heterospecific silk (Uetz and Denterlein, 1979; Stratton and Uetz, 1983), and S. ocreata males court with similar intensity on both silks (Roberts and Uetz, unpublished data), but there has not been a rigorous, reciprocal test of male discrimination of female cues. Male courtship is costly in these species, with recent studies indicating a high energetic expense (Cady, A. B., Uetz, G. W., and Delaney, K. J., unpublished data), and, for S. ocreata, that courtship characteristics important in mate choice also increase predation (Roberts, J. A., Taylor, P. W., and Uetz, G. W., unpublished data). Intraguild predation has a large impact on survival in wolf spiders (Polis and McCormick, 1986; Wise and Chen, 1999), and males making discrimination mistakes may become prey if they court heterospecifics. We predict that if there are detectable differences in female cues associated with silk, there should be differences in the intensity of courtship (i.e., rate of behaviors) for males of both species when exposed to conspecific versus heterospecific cues. In addition, male courtship response is thought to be elicited by chemical compounds associated with silk (contact pheromones), but this has not been confirmed using silk extractions. While unlikely, it is possible that male courtship response in these species could be due to mechanical properties of silk. In this study, we explored whether males are able to discriminate species-specific chemical cues associated with female silk, testing the null hypothesis of no behavioral differences (nondiscrimination). Any significant differences in courtship intensity would suggest species discrimination and could indicate species recognition via chemical or mechanical cues associated with silk. METHODS AND MATERIALS General Methods. The wolf spider ethospecies used in this study are common in deciduous forest habitats of eastern and mid-western North America. P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] 1274 pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 ROBERTS AND UETZ All spiders used in this study were collected as juveniles or subadults in the field and then raised to adulthood under identical laboratory conditions. Juveniles and adult females of these species are morphologically indistinguishable. Adult males of each species can be identified by the presence/absence of large tufts of bristles found only on the forelegs of S. ocreata. As the experiments described here required prior knowledge of species identity, all individuals were collected from unique species-pure localities surrounded by the zone of sympatry. Schizocosa ocreata were collected from the Cincinnati Nature Center, Rowe Woods, Clermont County, Ohio, and the University of Cincinnati Benedict Nature Preserve, Hamilton County, Ohio. Schizocosa rovneri were collected from the Ohio River flood plain at Sandy Run, Boone County, Kentucky. Individuals were visually isolated from other spiders in opaque plastic containers (deli-dish, 10-cm diam), fed (as appropriate for their size/age) assorted collembolans, fruit flies (Drosophila), or two–three 10-day-old cricket nymphs (Acheta domesticus) twice weekly, and supplied with water ad libitum from a cotton wick suspended in a water reservoir below the container. All spiders were maintained at room temperature (22–25◦ C) with stable humidity and a 13:11-hr light:dark photoperiod. Males and females were unmated and ranged from 1 to 4-wk postfinal molt at the time of the study. Collection of Silk and Extraction of Chemical Cues. Female S. ocreata deposit dragline silk as they walk, so in treatments where female silk was necessary for study, it was collected by placing single females on filter paper (Fisherbrand 90-mm diam) in a glass petri-dish (90-mm diam) allowing each to locomote for 24 hr and deposit dragline silk. Females were not used on the same day they had been fed to reduce contamination from feces (Ayyagari and Tietjen, 1987). Fecal material is quickly absorbed by filter paper allowing any fecal stains to easily be excised prior to chemical extraction or use in experiments. Filter paper was handled with stainless steel forceps, and stains were excised by using a stainless steel dissecting blade. Any filter paper disk with three or more fecal stains was not used in this study. To reduce the likelihood of contamination, silk collection equipment was cleaned in Alconox detergent and rinsed in double deionized water and 70% ethanol between filter paper manipulations. Preliminary behavioral assays were conducted with S. ocreata to identify an effective solvent for extraction of chemical cues associated with female silk. We exposed males (5 min) to three stimulus treatments including untreated silk (N = 10) and silk treated in 3-min extractions with pentane (N = 12, Fisher Scientific, HPLC grade) or methanol (N = 10, Pharmco Products, Inc., HPLC grade). Each of these solvents has been used previously to successfully extract behaviorally active chemical components associated with spider silk (Ayyagari and Tietjen, 1987; Trabalon et al., 1997). We used total duration of active courtship (jerky tap) exhibited by male S. ocreata on washed silk as a measure of extraction effectiveness. Methanol was the best solvent, and this result was consistent with a previous P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 CHEMICAL SIGNALING IN A WOLF SPIDER May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 1275 study in which male S. ocreata pheromone compounds were extracted (Ayyagari and Tietjen, 1987). For the collection of silk, 120 females (60 of each of the two ethospecies) were used. The female stimulus treatments for the S. ocreata and S. rovneri discrimination experiments were prepared as follows; (1) Untreated Silk– filter paper disks containing silk (and associated chemical cues) of conspecific or heterospecific females were used directly to elicit male behavior, (2) Washed Silk– following silk collection, filter paper disks with bound silk were soaked in 50 ml of methanol for 30 min, removed from the solvent bath, and allowed to air-dry for 30 min, and (3) Silk Extract– filter papers were soaked in 50 ml of methanol for 30 min, discarded, and the remaining solvent allowed to completely evaporate on a clean piece of filter paper for 30 min. Previous experiments involving the extraction of silk for behavioral assays have used extraction periods ranging from 15 min (Jackson, 1987) to 180 min (Suter and Renkes, 1982). Ayyagari and Tietjen (1987) found that a 15-min extraction in only 10 ml of solvent was sufficient to remove behaviorally active components from the silk of male S. ocreata, but we chose 30 min in 50 ml of solvent (enough to completely submerge the filter paper and silk) as a more conservative estimate to ensure more complete extraction (Suter, R. B., personal communication). The Untreated Control (4) treatment (filter paper alone) was prepared by duplicating the silk collection treatments but without female silk, including placing each filter paper in a glass petri-dish for 24 hr. All prepared filter paper disks were used in behavioral trials within 4 hr of completion of the preparation. Behavioral Assays. We used male behavioral response to chemical and/or mechanical stimuli as an assay of male discrimination of female species identity. Behavioral trials were conducted during the period of peak activity for males (1000–1600 hr, Roberts, J. A. and Uetz, G. W., unpublished data), in clear plastic containers (13 × 7 × 7 cm) that provided a visual arena suitable for videotaping. Filter paper disks were handled only with fine, stainless steel forceps during all stages of pre- and posttreatment manipulation. Each disk was cut in half with stainless steel dissecting scissors, and both halves were placed into the arena no more than 5 min prior to the start of a trial. Males were placed gently onto the filter paper stimulus from above and videotaped (RCA Autoshot VHS video camera, Model CC4352) for later behavioral analysis with a software package for the detailed analysis of behavioral data (The Observer , version 4.1). The plastic arenas, dissecting scissors, and forceps were cleaned prior to each trial with a Kimwipe and 70% ethanol to remove any trace of silk and pheromone deposited in previous trials and were allowed to air-dry. A total of 160 males (80 of each species) were chosen randomly from the laboratory population and assigned to one of four stimulus treatment groups (described above) within the species categories S. ocreata or S. rovneri (a two-factor ANOVA with treatment and species as main effects and individuals as replicates). P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 ROBERTS AND UETZ 1276 TABLE 1. ETHOGRAM OF MALE BEHAVIORAL ELEMENTS FOR THE WOLF SPIDERS Schizocosa ocreata AND Schizocosa rovneria Schizocosa ocreata Jerky tap Tap Schizocosa rovneri Bounce Shared behaviors Chemoexplore Leg raise Grooming Locomotion Stationary a Behavioral Active courtship characterized by a combination of jerky forward walking motion and tapping of the forelegs against the substrate, and often ending with the chelicerae striking the substrate. One or both forelegs are raised off the substrate and then lowered, lightly striking the substrate. While stationary, all legs are contracted simultaneously causing the ventral surface of the body to strike the substrate. Active exploratory behavior where the anteriolateral surfaces of the pedipalps are brushed on the surface of the substrate in rapid succession. One or more legs are raised off the substrate above parallel with the body and then lowered without striking the substrate. This is a combination of “arch” (Stratton and Uetz, 1986) and all other leg waving. The legs or pedipalps are drawn between the chelicerae and/or lateral pairs of legs are brushed together rapidly. Walking with no other elements expressed. Motionless with no other elements expressed. elements have been adapted from Stratton and Uetz (1986) and Delaney (1997). Shared and species-specific male behavioral elements were scored for each trial (Table 1). The behaviors critical in discrimination and courtship in these species are chemoexplore (both species), jerky tap (male S. ocreata), and bounce (male S. rovneri) (Stratton and Uetz, 1986; Stratton, 1997; see ethogram, Table 1), and results for these behaviors are presented for each species. Two parameters were determined for each key behavior; (1) Total Number – a count of the number of bouts (or events) of each behavior during the trial period, and (2) Latency – the time from introduction of the male into the arena to the initiation of a behavior (a score of 300 sec was used if a behavior was not seen during the 5-min trial period). The active courtship element of S. rovneri (“bounce”) occurs as an event (an instantaneous behavior with each occurrence having a duration less than 1 sec; measured in terms of rate) and not a behavioral state (behavioral elements with a measurable duration), so parameters including a term for duration of behavior could not be calculated for S. rovneri courtship and were not calculated for either species. Each parameter scored was square root transformed for analysis, and for each species a separate, Bonferroni-corrected critical value (α = 0.025) was used in significance tests of the ANOVA results to account for multiple comparisons (Shaffer, 1995). Post hoc analyses of the preliminary assay results and pooled data from the behavioral assays, where appropriate, were made P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 CHEMICAL SIGNALING IN A WOLF SPIDER 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 1277 with Tukey–Kramer HSD tests (Zar, 1999). All statistical analyses were performed with JMP version 4.02 (SAS Institute). RESULTS Preliminary Assay. Total duration of male courtship behavior on washed silk varied significantly with extraction treatment (ANOVA, F2,29 = 9.4, P < 0.001). Male S. ocreata behavior on female silk washed in pentane was not significantly different from untreated silk, but behavior was significantly reduced on silk washed in methanol. Schizocosa ocreata, Chemoexploratory Behavior. The total number of bouts and latency of chemoexploratory behavior both varied significantly with stimulus treatment, but there was no effect of species (conspecific or heterospecific source of silk) on male behavior, and no interaction between factors (Figure 1). After pooling the species data for each treatment group, we found that males exhibited the most bouts of chemoexploratory behavior on untreated and washed silk, with frequency of bouts significantly reduced on silk extract, and lowest in the control treatments (Figure 1A). Latency to chemoexplore was shortest on untreated silk followed by washed silk, then silk extract, and longest in the control treatment (Figure 1B). Schizocosa ocreata, Courtship Behavior. As in the data for chemoexploratory behavior, the total number and latency of courtship bouts in S. ocreata varied significantly with stimulus treatment, and there was again no significant effect of the source of silk (species) and no significant interaction (Figure 2). The number of bouts of jerky tap was highest on untreated silk, significantly reduced in response to washed silk, and not different from untreated control for the extract treatment (Figure 2A). Males began actively courting on untreated silk faster than on any of the other stimulus treatments (Figure 2B). Schizocosa rovneri, Chemoexploratory Behavior. For S. rovneri, both the total number and latency of bouts of chemoexploratory behavior varied with stimulus treatment, and while there was no effect of species for either parameter, the interaction was significant for total number of bouts of chemoexplore behavior (Figure 3). This interaction, presumably due to response in the silk extract treatment where the species difference is not consistent with the pattern of differences in other treatments, precluded a post hoc analysis of number of bouts of chemoexplore behavior by treatment stimulus (Figure 3A). Latency to chemoexplore was shortest on untreated silk, intermediate on washed silk and silk extract, and longest for the control treatment (Figure 3B). Schizocosa rovneri, Courtship Behavior. The total number of bounce bouts was significant by treatment, as was the latency of bounce behavior, with no effect of species or interaction term for either measure (Figure 4). Males exhibited more P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 ROBERTS AND UETZ 1278 FIG. 1. Schizocosa ocreata chemoexploratory behavior. For male spiders exposed to each treatment within the conspecific (S. ocreata) or heterospecific (S. rovneri) species categories; (A) mean total number of behavioral bouts (+SE) (ANOVA, treatment: F3,72 = 40.2, P < 0.001a ; species: F1,72 = 0.8, P = 0.365; interaction: F3,72 = 0.4, P = 0.748), and (B) mean latency (s) of behavioral bouts (+SE) (ANOVA, treatment: F3,72 = 64.5, P < 0.001a ; species: F1,72 = 0.0, P = 0.829; interaction: F3,72 = 0.7, P = 0.560) (a Indicates significance using Bonferroni-corrected critical value α = 0.025 to account for multiple comparisons). Shared letters above the bars indicate no significant difference between treatment categories by Tukey–Kramer HSD post hoc analysis (with species category data pooled). bounce bouts on untreated silk than on any other treatment (Figure 4A), and latency to begin bounce behavior was shortest on untreated silk and not different from untreated control for the other treatments (Figure 4B). DISCUSSION According to Dobzhansky (1970), “ethological species” are those in which there is a breakdown in some component of mutual attraction between the sexes in heterospecific pairings that prevents reproduction. In spiders, at least, species that meet this definition have been called “ethospecies” (den Hollander and Dijkstra, 1974; Stratton and Uetz, 1981, 1983, 1986). Stratton and Uetz (1981) presented P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 CHEMICAL SIGNALING IN A WOLF SPIDER May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 1279 FIG. 2. Schizocosa ocreata jerky tap behavior. For male spiders exposed to each treatment within the conspecific (S. ocreata) or heterospecific (S. rovneri) species categories; (A) mean total number of behavioral bouts (+SE) (ANOVA, treatment: F3,72 = 33.8, P < 0.001a ; species: F1,72 = 0.0, P = 0.999; interaction: F3,72 = 0.1, P = 0.933), and (B) mean latency (s) of behavioral bouts (+SE) (ANOVA, treatment: F3,72 = 31.6, P < 0.001a ; species: F1,72 = 0.0, P = 0.887; interaction: F3,72 = 0.2, P = 0.923) (a Indicates significance using Bonferroni-corrected critical value α = 0.025 to account for multiple comparisons). Significant differences indicated as in Figure 1. evidence of reproductive isolation between S. ocreata and S. rovneri based on species-specific male courtship behaviors without a rigorous test of reciprocal chemical cue discrimination. In this study, males of both S. rovneri and S. ocreata responded equivalently with respect to courtship and exploratory behaviors on untreated conspecific and heterospecific female silk cues. It appears that either there are no differences between female signals, or that there are no speciesspecific differences recognizable by males of either species. Therefore, female discrimination of species-specific male courtship appears to be the only component of attraction preventing reproduction in these ethospecies, supporting previous studies (Stratton and Uetz, 1981, 1983, 1986). The results of experiments presented here also suggest that chemical, and not mechanical cues associated with silk are critical for eliciting male courtship in both S. ocreata and S. rovneri (Uetz P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] 1280 pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 ROBERTS AND UETZ FIG. 3. Schizocosa rovneri chemoexploratory behavior. For male spiders exposed to each treatment within the conspecific (S. rovneri) or heterospecific (S. ocreata) species categories; (A) mean total number of behavioral bouts (+SE) (ANOVA, treatment: F3,72 = 25.9, P < 0.001a ; species: F1,72 = 1.6, P = 0.208; interaction: F3,72 = 7.0, P < 0.001a ), and (B) mean latency (s) of behavioral bouts (+SE) (ANOVA, treatment: F3,72 = 45.0, P < 0.001a ; species: F1,72 = 1.7, P = 0.195; interaction: F3,72 = 3.0, P = 0.036) (a Indicates significance using Bonferroni-corrected critical value α = 0.025 to account for multiple comparisons). The significant interaction effect in (A) prohibited a post hoc analysis by treatment category, however significant differences for (B) indicated as in Figure 1. and Denterlein, 1979; Stratton and Uetz, 1983). Even so, while we were able to successfully remove a compound on female silk that elicits male courtship, extracts were not sufficient to stimulate males to court at levels equivalent to untreated silk. There may be more than one potential explanation for this finding. One possible explanation relates to stimulus strength. Ayyagari and Tietjen (1987) extracted behaviorally active components from male silk in S. ocreata, but in that study, the silk of multiple individuals was extracted simultaneously to arrive at a product that would elicit male response. This method has the undesirable side effect of potentially creating a “super-stimulus” (a chemical stimulus far stronger than any that would be encountered in the natural habitat) that could result in atypical behavioral response in stimulated individuals. In an attempt to arrive at P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 CHEMICAL SIGNALING IN A WOLF SPIDER May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 1281 FIG. 4. Schizocosa rovneri bounce behavior. For male spiders exposed to each treatment within the conspecific (S. rovneri) or heterospecific (S. ocreata) species categories; (A) mean total number of behavioral bouts (+SE) (ANOVA, treatment: F3,72 = 23.8, P < 0.001a ; species: F1,72 = 0.9, P = 0.346; interaction: F3,72 = 1.0, P = 0.391), and (B) mean latency (s) of behavioral bouts (+SE) (ANOVA, treatment: F3,72 = 17.3, P < 0.001a ; species: F1,72 = 0.3, P = 0.599; interaction: F3,72 = 0.8, P = 0.512) (a Indicates significance using Bonferroni-corrected critical value α = 0.025 to account for multiple comparisons). Significant differences indicated as in Figure 1. more natural dose levels, we used extracts of the silk of single individuals. It seems likely that the low courtship response in our study may have been due to diffusion of a finite amount of chemical stimulus over a greater area, reducing point-specific concentration to levels below the threshold necessary for release of courtship. Another possibility is that there may be multiple compounds affected differently by solvent type. Ayyagari and Tietjen (1987) used methanol to extract a behaviorally active component from male S. ocreata silk, and in part for this reason methanol was selected as a potential solvent for female chemical compounds in the work presented here. In previous attempts to study potential pheromones associated with the silk of female wolf spiders, several organic and inorganic solvents were found to produce a pattern of inactivation similar to that found using methanol in the current study (Kaston, 1936; Hegdekar and Dondale, 1969). This evidence P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 ROBERTS AND UETZ 1282 is suggestive that male courtship response is elicited by multiple compounds that are differentially extracted in various solvents but are necessary in combination to elicit courtship. In contrast to the results for male courtship, males exhibited a similar number of bouts of chemoexploratory behavior on untreated silk and silk washed in methanol (Figures 1 and 3). This suggests that the chemical compound(s) that elicits courtship behavior plays only a limited role for release of chemoexploratory behavior. Either there is a separate compound associated with silk that releases chemoexploratory behavior but is not extracted by methanol, or the term “chemo” exploratory behavior may be a misnomer for these species. Wolf spiders are known to have chemosensory sensilla concentrated on the anterior dorsal and lateral surfaces of the pedipalps (Kronestedt, 1979; Tietjen and Rovner, 1982; Barth, 2002), which are actively rubbed on the substrate during exploratory behavior presumably to collect chemical information (Tietjen, 1977; Stratton and Uetz, 1986). In another study of S. ocreata (Roberts, J. A. and Uetz, G. W., unpublished data), total number of bouts of chemoexploratory behavior was similar on the silk of female S. crassipes, a member of the ocreata clade along with S. ocreata and S. rovneri (Miller et al., 1998), but decreased in species outside the clade despite the presence of silk. Together this evidence suggests that a chemical associated with silk elicits chemoexploration, and that this compound may be independent of, or redundant to, the chemical that releases courtship behavior. Of note is the unexpected result for S. rovneri males of greater number of bouts of chemoexploratory behavior on extract of heterospecific silk, compared to conspecific extract (Figure 3A). This suggests that there may be slight differences in the composition or ratios of chemical compounds on the silk of these species, but those differences are not behaviorally relevant on untreated silk. Further chemical analysis will be necessary to fully explain this difference. Acknowledgments—This work was supported by the American Arachnological Society (JAR), the University of Cincinnati Research Council and Department of Biological Sciences (JAR), and the National Science Foundation (IBN 9906446 and IBN 9414239 to GWU). This research was submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for completion of the PhD in Biological Sciences at the University of Cincinnati. We thank the Cincinnati Nature Center, Rowe Woods, and the residents of Sandy Run, KY, for permission to collect spiders on their properties. Voucher specimens are on deposit at the National Museum of Natural History in Washington, DC, and in the personal collection of the corresponding author (JAR). We are especially grateful to J. Rovner (for access to his reference collection), and K. Roberts, A. Cady, C. Harris, J. Hinn, B. Jayne, K. Petren, M. Polak, J. Shann, and S. Walker for their advice and editorial comments. REFERENCES AYYAGARI, L. R. and TIETJEN, W. J. 1987. Preliminary isolation of male-inhibitory pheromone of the spider Schizocosa ocreata (Araneae, Lycosidae). J. Chem. Ecol. 13:237–245. P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] pp1208-joec-487114 CHEMICAL SIGNALING IN A WOLF SPIDER May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 1283 BARTH, F. G. 2002. A Spider’s World: Senses and Behavior. Springer, Heidelberg, Germany. CADY, A. B. 1984. Microhabitat selection and locomotor activity of Schizocosa ocreata (Walckenaer) (Araneae: Lycosidae). J. Arachnol. 11:297–307. COSTA, F. G. and CAPOCASALE, R. M. 1984. Lycocsa carbonelli, sp. nov.: una etoespecie simpatrida, sibilina de Lycosa thorelli (Keyserling) (Araneae, Lycosidae). J. Arachnol. 11:423–431. DELANEY, K. J. 1997. Communication in the contexts of courtship and aggression in two species on wolf spider (Araneae: Lycosidae). MS Dissertation, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati. DEN HOLLANDER, J. 1971. Species barriers in the Pardosa pullata group/Araneae, Lycosidae/. Proceedings of 5th International Arachnological Congress, Brno, pp. 129–142. DEN HOLLANDER, J. and DIJKSTRA, H. 1974. Pardosa vlijmi sp. Nov., a new ethospecies sibling Pardosa proxima (C. L. Koch, 1948), from France, with description of courtship display (Araneae, Lycosidae). Beaufortia 22:57–65. DEN HOLLANDER, J., DIJKSTRA, H., ALLEMAN, H., and VLIJM, L. 1973. Courtship behaviour as species barrier in the Pardosa pullata group (Araneae, Lycosidae). Tijdschr. Entomol. 116:1–22. DOBZHANSKY, T. 1970. Genetics of the Evolutionary Process. Columbia University Press, New York. HEGDEKAR, B. M. and DONDALE, C. D. 1969. A contact sex pheromone and some response parameters in lycosid spiders. Can. J. Zool. 47:1–4. JACKSON, R. R. 1987. Comparative study of releaser pheromones associated with silk of jumping spiders (Araneae, Salticidae). N. Z. J. Zool. 14:1–10. KASTON, B. J. 1936. The senses involved in the courtship of some vagabond spiders. Entomol. Am. 16:97–167. KRONESTEDT, T. 1979. Study on chemosensitive hairs in wolf spiders (Araneae, Lycosidae) by scanning electron microscopy. Zool. Scr. 8:279–285. MCCLINTOCK, W. J. and UETZ, G. W. 1996. Female choice and pre-existing bias: visual cues during courtship in two Schizocosa wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae). Anim. Behav. 52:167–181. MILLER, G. L., STRATTON, G. E., MILLER, P. R., and HEBETS, E. 1998. Geographic variation in male courtship behaviour and sexual isolation in wolf spiders of the genus Schizocosa. Anim. Behav. 56:937–951. PAPKE, M. D., RIECHERT, S. E., and SCHULZ, S. 2001. An airborne female pheromone associated with male attraction and courtship in a desert spider. Anim. Behav. 61:877–886. POLIS, G. A. and MCCORMICK, S. J. 1986. Scorpions, spiders, and solpugids: Predation and competition among distantly related taxa. Oecologia 71:111–116. POLLARD, S. D., MACNAB, A. M., and JACKSON, R. R. 1987. Communication with chemicals: Pheromones and spiders, pp. 133–141, in W. Nentwig (ed.). Ecophysiology of Spiders. Springer, Berlin. PROUVOST, O., TRABALON, M., PAPKE, M., and SCHULZ, S. 1999. Contact sex signals on web and cuticle of Tegenaria atrica (Araneae, Agelenidae). Arch. Insect Biochem. Physiol. 40:194–202. RIECHERT, S. E. and SINGER, F. D. 1995. Investigation of potential male mate choice in a monogamous spider. Anim. Behav. 49:715–723. ROSS, K. and SMITH, R. L. 1979. Aspects of the courtship behavior of the black widow spider, Latrodectus hesperus (Araneae: Theridiidae), with evidence for the existence of a contact sex pheromone. J. Arachnol. 7:69–77. SEARCY, L. E., RYPSTRA, A. L., and PERSONS, M. H. 1999. Airborne chemical communication in the wolf spider Pardosa milvina. J. Chem. Ecol. 25:2527–2533. SHAFFER, J. P. 1995. Multiple hypothesis testing. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 46:561–584. SINGER, F., RIECHERT, S. E., XU, H., MORRIS, A. W., BECHER, E., HALE, J. A., and NOUREDDINE, M. A. 2000. Analysis of courtship success in the funnel-web spider Agelenopsis aperta. Behaviour 137:93–117. STRATTON, G. E. 1997. Investigation of species divergence and reproductive isolation of Schizocosa stridulans (Araneae: Lycosidae) from Illinois. Bull. Br. Arachnol. Soc. 10:313–321. P1: JLS Journal of Chemical Ecology [joec] 1284 pp1208-joec-487114 May 22, 2004 9:35 Style file version June 28th, 2002 ROBERTS AND UETZ STRATTON, G. E. and UETZ, G. W. 1981. Acoustic communication and reproductive isolation in two species of wolf spiders. Science 214:575–577. STRATTON, G. E. and UETZ, G. W. 1983. Communication via substratum-coupled stridulation and reproductive isolation in wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae). Anim. Behav. 31:164–172. STRATTON, G. E. and UETZ, G. W. 1986. The inheritance of courtship behavior and its role as a reproductive isolating mechanism in two species of Schizocosa wolf spiders (Araneae; Lycosidae). Evolution 40:129–141. SUTER, R. B. and RENKES, G. 1982. Linyphiid spider courtship: releaser and attractant functions of a contact sex pheromone. Anim. Behav. 30:714–718. TIETJEN, W. J. 1977. Dragline-following by male lycosid spiders. Psyche 84:165–178. TIETJEN, W. J. 1979. Is the sex pheromone of Lycosa rabida (Araneae: Lycosidae) deposited on a substratum? J . Arachnol. 6:207–212. TIETJEN, W. J. and ROVNER, J. S. 1982. Chemical communication in lycosids and other spiders, pp. 249–279, in P. N. Witt and J. S. Rovner (eds.). Spider Communication. Mechanisms and Ecological Significance. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. TRABALON, M., BAGNERES, A. G., and ROLAND, C. 1997. Contact sex signals in two sympatric spider species, Tegenaria domestica and Tegenaria pagana. J. Chem. Ecol. 23:747–758. UETZ, G. W. 2000. Signals and multi-modal signaling in spider communication, pp. 387–405, in Y. Espmark, T. Amundsen, and G. Rosenqvist (eds.). Animal Signals: Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication. Tapir Academic Press, Trondheim, Norway. UETZ, G. W. and DENTERLEIN, G. 1979. Courtship behavior, habitat, and reproductive isolation in Schizocosa rovneri Uetz and Dondale (Araneae: Lycosidae). J. Arachnol. 7:121–128. UETZ, G. W. and ROBERTS, J. A. 2002. Multisensory cues and multimodal communication in spiders: Insights from video/audio playback studies. Brain Behav. Evol. 59:222–230. WISE, D. H. and CHEN, B. 1999. Impact of intraguild predators on survival of a forest-floor wolf spider. Oecologia 121:129–137. ZAR, J. H. 1999. Biostatistical Analysis, 4th edn. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ.