Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Articles in PresS. J Neurophysiol (February 3, 2016). doi:10.1152/jn.00971.2015 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Title: Primary motor cortex neurons classified in a postural task predict muscle activation patterns in a reaching task Running Head: M1 prediction of movement from postural classification ETHAN A HEMING1, TIMOTHY P. LILLICRAP2, MOHSEN OMRANI1, TROY M HERTER3, J. ANDREW PRUSZYNSKI4,5,6, STEPHEN H. SCOTT1,7,8 1. Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada 2. Google DeepMind, London, Kings Cross, UK 3. Department of Exercise Science, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, USA 4. Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Western University, London, ON, Canada 5. Robarts Research Institute, Western University, London, ON, Canada 6. Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON, Canada 7. Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada 8. Department of Medicine, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada 17 18 Corresponding Authors: 19 Ethan Heming: [email protected] 20 Stephen Scott: [email protected] 21 22 1 Copyright © 2016 by the American Physiological Society. 23 24 Abstract: 25 Primary motor cortex (M1) activity correlates with many motor variables making it difficult to 26 demonstrate how it participates in motor control. We developed a two-stage process to separate the 27 process of classifying the motor field of M1 neurons from the process of predicting the spatiotemporal 28 patterns of its motor field during reaching. We tested our approach with a neural network model that 29 controlled a two-joint arm to show the statistical relationship between network connectivity and neural 30 activity across different motor tasks. In rhesus monkeys, M1 neurons classified by this method showed 31 preferred reaching directions similar to their associated muscle groups. Importantly, the neural 32 population signals predicted the spatiotemporal dynamics of their associated muscle groups, although a 33 sub-group of atypical neurons reversed their directional preference suggesting a selective role in 34 antagonist control. These results highlight that M1 provides important details on the spatiotemporal 35 patterns of muscle activity during motor skills such as reaching. 36 37 38 Keywords: 39 • primary motor cortex 40 • motor fields 41 • neural network model 42 • electromyography 43 2 44 INTRODUCTION The motor system precisely controls the activity of muscles to generate smooth and accurate 45 46 motor actions. For example, goal-directed reaching involves agonist muscle activity to propel the hand 47 toward the goal and antagonistic muscle activity to decelerate and stop at the goal (Marsden et al., 1983; 48 Wierzbicka et al., 1986; Flanders et al., 1994). The selection, onset time and magnitude of muscle 49 activity during reaching depends on many factors such as target and initial limb position, arm geometry, 50 and external loads (Caminiti et al., 1990; Karst and Hasan, 1991; Hong et al., 1994; Scott, 1997). Primary motor cortex (M1) plays an important role in voluntary motor functions, such as 51 52 reaching, but its specific role remains debated. The classic dichotomy is whether its activity reflects 53 muscles or movements (Phillips, 1975). The latter suggests that M1’s contribution is to define 54 behavioural goals (e.g. direction and extent of movements) leaving subcortical and spinal structures as 55 the primary source for generating spatiotemporal patterns of muscle activity (Georgopoulos, 1995; 56 Raphael et al., 2010). Indeed, many studies have shown that M1 activity correlates with many high level 57 parameters such as direction of hand motion (Georgopoulos et al., 1982; Toxopeus et al., 2011; Philip et 58 al., 2013), hand movement speed (Schwartz, 1993), and target direction and speed (Johnson et al., 59 1999). 60 The other end of the spectrum is the view that primary motor cortex directly generates 61 spatiotemporal patterns of muscle activity for goal-directed movements (Bennett and Lemon, 1994; 62 Scott, 1997, 2003; Cherian et al., 2013). Although the exact patterns of muscle activity for limb 63 movement are only specified at the spinal level, as spinal afferent feedback will influence motor output, 64 the idea is that basic features such as the selection, timing and magnitude of muscle activity are specified 65 by neurons in M1. Indeed, several studies have quantified how M1 activity correlates with the activity of 66 hand and wrist muscles (Evarts, 1968; Humphrey, 1972; Bennett and Lemon, 1996; Kakei et al., 1999; 3 67 Oby et al., 2013) or proximal arm muscles (Murphy et al., 1985; Scott, 1997; Todorov, 2000; Sergio and 68 Kalaska, 2003; Cherian et al., 2013). 69 The lack of strong causal evidence for one of these options indicates that identifying a simple 70 correlation is not sufficient to identify the role of M1 in voluntary motor control. There are many 71 different patterns of muscle and neural activity, and finding arbitrary correlations is relatively easy 72 (Humphrey, 1972). One way to circumvent this problem is to dissociate different movement parameters, 73 such as by making movements with different torques or arm configurations (Fromm and Evarts, 1977; 74 Scott and Kalaska, 1997; Kakei et al., 1999; Sergio and Kalaska, 2003; Cherian et al., 2013). Inevitably, 75 these studies identify that some neuronal activity in M1 can reflect many different features of motor 76 actions. However, the presence of some high level information in M1 does not preclude its role in 77 specifying spatiotemporal patterns of muscle activity. 78 Better support for a role in generating patterns of muscle activity is to separate the process for 79 identifying the portion of the motor periphery associated with a given neuron – its motor field – from the 80 process of relating its spatiotemporal activity to muscles in that motor field. A similar concept is the 81 sensory field of a neuron, which is portion of the periphery to which applying stimuli elicits firing. 82 Identifying a sensory field is relatively straightforward as it requires one to simply apply or present a 83 controlled stimuli. It is less clear how to define motor fields. 84 The synaptic connectivity of neurons that project to proximal limb motor neuron pools can be 85 roughly identified by spike-triggered averaging (Fetz and Cheney, 1978; Cheney and Fetz, 1980, 1985; 86 Kasser and Cheney, 1985; McKiernan et al., 1998). However the variability in STA timing, and the 87 inability to record from all muscles at once, makes it very difficult to describe a complete connectivity 88 mapping from STA connectivity. If the scope of investigation is limited to monosynaptically connected 89 corticomotorneurons (CM cells), the proximal limb has reduced CM cell representation compared to the 4 90 distal limb (Buys et al., 1986; Palmer and Ashby, 1992) and CM cell distribution is limited to the caudal 91 (new) section of M1 (Rathelot and Strick, 2009). Intra-cortical micro-stimulation can be used to identify 92 muscle connectivity by repeated stimulation and stimulus-triggered-averaging (Cheney and Fetz, 1985; 93 Buys et al., 1986), however this technique does not guarantee single-neuron activation. Cheney and Fetz 94 (1985) and Lemon et al. (1987) show increased muscle activity for increased stimulation current and 95 conclude that stimulation likely activates multiple neurons in proximity. 96 The present study used two different behavioural tasks to separate the process for identifying a 97 neuron’s motor field from the process of comparing spatiotemporal patterns of activity between neurons 98 and muscles. First, the motor field of M1 neurons and limb muscles were identified based on their load 99 preference using a posture task consisting of combinations of shoulder and elbow flexion or extension 100 torques (Figure 1A). A neural network model that we used to drive our predictions (Lillicrap and Scott, 101 2013) highlighted that the torque preference of the model’s ‘cortical’ units correlated with the torque 102 preference of their synaptically associated muscle groups. The model predicted that neurons with similar 103 motor fields – defined by the posture task – would show correlated directional preferences in a reaching 104 task. We tested this prediction by examining the directional preferences of monkey M1 neurons during 105 center-out reaching to a range of peripheral targets (Figure 1B). Consistent with our model, we found 106 that the activity of each neuronal population predicted the spatiotemporal patterns of their respective 107 muscle groups. Interestingly, a small subset of the neurons in each group possessed directional 108 preferences during reaching that were opposite to that observed for its associated muscle group. These 109 atypical neurons may be indicative of selective control when the muscle acts as an antagonist during 110 motor skills. 111 112 113 Experimental Procedures 5 114 115 Subjects and apparatus Five male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta, 6–12 kg, monkeys A-E) were trained to perform 116 reaching and posture tasks while attached to a robotic upper-limb exoskeleton (KINARM; BKIN 117 Technologies, Kingston, Ontario, Canada). This exoskeleton maintained the arm in a horizontal plane at 118 shoulder height permitting motion at the shoulder and elbow, and allowing mechanical torques to be 119 applied at either joint (Scott, 1999). Targets and a dot representing hand feedback location were 120 presented in the plane of the arm via reflection in semi-transparent glass. These experiments were 121 conducted in accordance with the Queen's University Animal Care Committee. 122 123 124 Behavioural Tasks Posture task. This task has been previously described (Cabel et al., 2001; Herter et al., 2007). In 125 each trial, a constant torque was applied to the elbow and/or shoulder. The monkey then stabilized the 126 hand within a central 0.8cm-wide stationary target for at least 3 seconds. Nine constant torques were 127 used, consisting of elbow flexion (EF) or extension (EE), shoulder flexion (SF) or extension (SE), four 128 multi-joint torques (SF+EF, SF+EE, SE+EF, SE+EE), and an unloaded condition (Figure 1A). Torques 129 of magnitude 0.12Nm were used for monkeys A-C,E and 0.32 Nm for Monkey D. Five blocks were 130 presented, each containing the 9 load conditions in random order, for a total of 45 trials. 131 Reaching task. This task has been previously described (Kurtzer et al., 2006b). Monkeys began 132 each trial by maintaining their index finger (white dot) within a central start target (8mm radius). This 133 start target was positioned such that the shoulder and elbow were at approximately 30⁰ and 90⁰, 134 respectively. After a random time period (1.5-2.0s), a peripheral target (12mm radius) then illuminated 135 6cm from the central target. The monkey then moved between the start and peripheral target in 220 to 136 350ms, generating total reach times of ~500 to 600ms when including intra-target acceleration and 6 137 deceleration. Eight such peripheral targets were located around the start target. For monkeys A and B, 138 these were distributed such that they were roughly distributed uniformly in torque space in two 139 arrangements (Figure 1B i,ii). For Monkeys C-E, the targets were uniformly distributed in Cartesian 140 space in two arrangements (Figure 1B iii,iv). In monkey D, some trials were also performed with 3cm 141 reaches (target pattern not shown). Five blocks were presented, each containing the 8 reach directions in 142 random order, for a total of 40 trials. 143 144 145 Data collection Neuronal recordings were obtained from the elbow/shoulder region of primary motor cortex 146 (M1), contralateral to the arm used to perform the behavioural tasks, using standard recording 147 techniques (Scott and Kalaska, 1997; Scott et al., 2001). Activity of these neurons was recorded during 148 both the reaching and posture tasks. 149 We examined the activity of elbow and shoulder flexor and extensor muscles using 150 electromyography during the posture and reaching tasks. In most cases, we recorded through a pair of 151 percutaneous Teflon-coated 50-μm stainless steel wires using standard recording techniques (Scott and 152 Kalaska, 1997). In monkeys A and C, we recorded some muscle activity from chronically implanted 153 bipolar multi-strand electrodes (Scott and Kalaska, 1997; Kurtzer et al., 2006b). Chronic electrode 154 recordings were only selected if they occurred more than a week apart, to minimize redundant data. 155 Recordings were taken from biceps (12 percutaneous, 13 chronic), brachioradialis (7, 11), brachialis (6, 156 6), long head triceps (5, 19), posterior deltoid (8, 10), lateral triceps (8, 3), middle triceps (2, 0), anterior 157 deltoid (3, 11), and pectoralis major (6, 6). To ensure that the percutaneous recordings were not being 158 skewed by the chronic recordings, we compared the mean preferred torque direction (PTD, described in 159 Data analysis) between percutaneous and chronic recordings across muscles, and found no difference 7 160 (paired t-test, t=0.02, p=0.98), and while the average variance in PTD was lower for chronic recordings 161 (3.4º) than percutaneous (7.0º). 162 Joint kinematic data and EMG were recorded at 1 kHz (Monkeys A–C) or 4 kHz (Monkeys D,E). 163 Neuronal firing was binned into 5ms bins, and EMG and kinematic data were down-sampled to 200Hz 164 for data storage. 165 166 167 Data analysis Preferred torque direction. Combinations of shoulder and elbow torques were described in 168 torque space where shoulder torque was represented along the x-axis and elbow torque was represented 169 along the y-axis. Positive torque, in each axis, was defined as flexor torque to oppose joint-extending 170 applied torques, so that shoulder flexor torque was at 0⁰, elbow extensor at 90⁰, shoulder extensor at 171 180⁰, and elbow extensor at 270⁰. A plane was fit to EMG activity or cell firing rate associated with the 172 elbow/shoulder torque in this space.. We used only the EMG activity or cell firing in the last two 173 seconds of hold time to ensure that recordings were from a period of stationary posture. If the plane had 174 a statistically significant slope, the angle of maximal slope was defined as the torque which elicited 175 either a muscle’s maximal activation or neuron’s maximal firing rate. This preferred torque direction 176 (PTD) was measured counter-clockwise from shoulder flexion. 177 Preferred reaching direction. A given reach movement was assigned a reach direction in 178 Cartesian space, increasing counter-clockwise from the positive x-axis. It was calculated by the angle 179 from the origin to the hand position at maximal tangential velocity. EMG activity and neuronal firing 180 rate were integrated around (-50ms to 150ms) movement onset, which was defined as the time when the 181 hand first attained 5% of maximum hand speed. This activity was fit to a plane based on the angle of 182 each reach and the activity during each reach. If the plane had significant slope, the preferred reaching 8 183 direction (PRD) was defined as the angle which had maximal slope on the plane. It described the 184 maximum spatial modulation of either a muscle’s activation or neuron’s firing rate. 185 Spatiotemporal dynamics of reaching. Activities in each trial were aligned temporally on the 186 calculated reach onset time. Baseline activity was removed by calculating the mean activity of each 187 neuron or muscle recording during the initial hold period over all trials and subtracted from all trials for 188 that recording. Activity for each muscle or neuron was then divided by the maximum activity across 189 reaching directions for that recording. Data were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel with a 5ms standard 190 deviation. 191 192 193 Modelling To predict the responses of a neural system with defined motor fields, we implemented a static 194 neural network model similar to that developed by Lillicrap & Scott (2013). . The model consisted of a 195 vector of ‘cortical’ units, z, which controlled 6 muscle groups, u, controlling a two-joint arm in the 196 horizontal plane via linear output weights, w (Figure 2A). Muscle activity was kept positive via the 197 standard sigmoid - that is, u = σ(w · z). Modelled muscle activity generated torques and hand velocities 198 via a function that approximated the biomechanics of a 2-joint revolute arm constrained to move in the 199 horizontal plane. This function was computed by linearizing a dynamic model that included limb 200 geometry, intersegmental dynamics, and mono- and bi-articular muscles with force generation 201 dependent on length and velocity. Muscle tension forces, t, are obtained by element-wise multiplication 202 of muscle activity with linearized F-L/V scaling factors appropriate for the movement direction, i.e., t = 203 H · u. Joint torques are computed via: τ = M t. And hand velocity is determined by the linear 204 transformation, y = G F τ, where F and G are local linear approximations to limb dynamics and the 205 geometric mapping between joint and hand velocity, respectively. This static model was derived as a 9 206 simplified version of a dynamic model which executed reaching movements over a sequence of time 207 steps and in which the network model was connected in closed loop with the arm. One of the findings 208 of this previous work was that a static model based on a linearization of the dynamic version captured 209 the most salient features of the population neural activity (Lillicrap & Scott 2013). The static version 210 also has the benefit of being easier to optimize, analyze, and understand. Parameters for the limb 211 biomechanics were derived from published work on monkey limb and muscle characteristics (Cheng 212 and Scott, 2000; Singh et al., 2002; Graham and Scott, 2003). We optimized z to solve analogues of the posture and reach tasks while keeping the square of the 213 214 neural and muscle activities small. For the reaching task, the model captures movement initiation. In 215 this case z* is found by minimizing the difference between target and actual hand velocity for a given 216 movement, 217 − = − , while keeping unit and muscle activity small; that is, ∗ = = + ‖ ‖ + ‖ ‖. For the posture task, the model captures the steady state condition 218 during which the joint torques are countered and the arm is at zero velocity. In this case z* is found by 219 minimizing the difference between applied and actual joint torques, 220 and muscle activity small; that is, 221 and β are set to 1e-6. Importantly, for a given simulation, the elements of the matrix w were drawn 222 randomly from a normal distribution (σ = 0.05) and were unaltered during optimization. This meant that 223 a given unit maintained the same relative contribution to each muscle at the periphery across tasks. Unit 224 activity was optimized to generate 16 target torques (posture task) and 16 target velocities (reach task) - 225 both equally distributed about the unit circle. In practice, we used a preconditioned conjugate gradient 226 descent algorithm with back-tracking line searches to find an optimal vector of activity for a given trial. 227 In line with the analysis of biological data, the activity of z and u across all 16 torques and reach 228 directions were plane fit to determine preferred reach and torque directions. ∗ = = 10 − = − , while keeping unit + ‖ ‖ + ‖ ‖. In both cases, α 229 230 231 232 233 RESULTS 234 235 236 Neural network model: Relation of torque preference and motor field We used a static neural network model similar to that developed by Lillicrap & Scott (2013) to 237 examine the relationship between neural connectivity, torque preferences during posture, and directional 238 tuning during reaching. The activation of model cortical units, z, were optimized to generate 239 combinations of elbow and shoulder torque for the posture task and different hand velocities for the 240 reaching task. Though we used a PCG algorithm for the results reported here, the same essential results 241 can be obtained with virtually any gradient based optimization routine. In particular, we find the same 242 results using L-BFGS and stochastic gradient descent (SGD), though SGD takes significantly longer to 243 converge. Given that the optimization we perform is non-linear and high dimensional, we are not able 244 to find a global minimum. Our results are thus based on local minima - but they are robust minima in the 245 following sense: we repeated the simulation 10 times from random initializations of the synaptic weight 246 matrix and found the same characteristic pattern of PTD/PRD distributions in each case. Thus, there 247 appears to be a large family of such minima - all of which produce similar behavioral performance and 248 PTD/PRD distributions. 249 We compared the torque preference of units in the network to its connectivity to evaluate how 250 well its torque preference estimated its motor field. To quantify torque preference, we fit a plane to the 251 activation of each unit across torque combinations in the two-dimensional elbow/shoulder torque space. 11 252 The preferred torque direction (PTD) of a unit was the angle in torque space to which the unit was 253 maximally active. Across simulations, we found that 99.4% of units had a significant (p<0.01) PTD in 254 the posture task. The distribution of the significant PTDs for one training session can be seen in Figure 255 3A. The resulting distribution was bimodal (r=0.31, p<0.001), aligned in much the same manner as the 256 previously reported distribution of M1 neurons (Herter et al., 2007; Pruszynski et al., 2014), with the 257 majority of units related to whole-limb flexion or extension. 258 In such a straightforward model, one might assume that a given unit would always show an 259 identical relationship between its torque preference and its anatomical connectivity given that the unit 260 can only produce torque in a given direction when activated alone. We calculated the motor field 261 preferred torque (MFPTD) direction of a given unit, zi, by multiplying its output weights (motor field), 262 wij, with the vector of preferred torque direction of each output unit in the posture task, uj, and then 263 vector summating. It is important to note that the preferred torque direction of each output unit is not the 264 simple direction of force production for that unit. Due to the redundant force profiles of the biarticular 265 muscles and the dynamics of the limb the preferred direction of an output unit rotates away from its 266 simple force production direction (Lillicrap and Scott, 2013). We found a circular correlation (r=0.86, 267 p<0.001) between the unit’s MFPTD and its preferred torque direction (PTD) in the posture task (Figure 268 2B), however the relationship was not perfect. To see how different connectivity patterns might be 269 influencing this relationship, we looked at the degree to which a unit co-activated opposite muscle 270 groups together (co-contraction). We normalized each set of weights from a unit to its muscle groups, 271 multiplied these by their respective MFPTD vectors, and vector averaged them. The length of the 272 resulting vector for each unit was used as an indicator of co-contraction, with a short vector indicating 273 more co-contraction. The average difference between PTD and MFPTD was 22⁰. Units with low co- 274 contraction (MFPTD vector length > 0.4, 9.1% of units) had an average difference of 7⁰ (r=0.99), 12 275 whereas units with high co-contraction (MFPTD vector length < 0.1, 11.7% of units) had an average 276 difference of 62⁰ (r=0.40). This suggests that the statistical dispersion in the relationship between torque 277 preference and anatomical connectivity is caused by those units with stronger synaptic connections to 278 antagonist muscles. 279 280 281 Neural Network Model: Relation of torque and reaching direction We used our model to predict the relationship between preferred torque direction and reaching 282 activity for units in the neural network model. Reaching activity was described by a preferred reaching 283 direction (PRD) – the direction in Cartesian space toward which a reach movement would elicit 284 maximal unit activity. Across all simulations, we found that 98.7% of units had a significant PRD (plane 285 fit p<0.01). The distribution of significant PRDs for one training session of units can be seen in Figure 286 3B. The bimodality (r=0.52, p<0.001) towards the top-left and bottom-right that emerges is stable across 287 simulations. The distribution was consistent for static and dynamic networks (Lillicrap and Scott, 2013) 288 and previous observations on M1 neurons (Scott et al., 2001). 289 Novel in this study, we compared PTD and PRD for each model unit which had both a 290 significant PTD in the posture task and a significant PRD in the reaching task (98.1%). As can be seen 291 in Figure 4A, there is a consistent relationship between the PTDs and PRDs, where two clusters emerge 292 due to the interaction of the bimodalities noted in each distribution. Figure 4B displays a histogram of 293 the difference of PTD and PRD angles for all units, with the presence of a mean systematic shift of 152⁰ 294 (circular correlation, r=0.84, p<0.001). This angle roughly corresponds to the shift in coordinate frames 295 between torque and hand space, used to define unit responses in the posture and reaching tasks, 296 respectively. This rotational shift was stable across all ten simulation sets (150⁰-154⁰, circular SD=1.2⁰), 297 indicating that the result is robust. There was some dispersion in the relationship between a unit’s PTD 13 298 and PRD (circular SD=27⁰). Even though individual units had different PRD and PTD tuning across 299 simulations, both the population distribution, and the relation between a given unit’s PTD and PRD 300 remained statistically the same. 301 302 303 Non-Human Primate Recordings: Muscle recordings We recorded and analysed the EMG activity of 9 muscles spanning the shoulder and/or elbow 304 (151 suitable recordings) in 5 macaque monkeys in posture and reaching tasks. In the posture task, a 305 monkey’s arm was maintained in the horizontal plane while combinations of flexor and/or extensor step 306 torques were applied to the shoulder and/or elbow (Figure 1A). Recordings were made while the 307 monkey held their hand stationary at a central position and countered these torques. Figure 3C shows an 308 example of pectoralis major activity during the posture task. As previously reported (Kurtzer et al., 309 2006a), this shoulder flexor, modulated its activity for torques applied at the elbow in addition to loads 310 at the shoulder and, as a result, the preferred torque direction (PTD) for this muscle did not lie at 311 shoulder flexion (i.e. 0⁰) line, as one would expect if the muscle activity reflected its anatomical action. 312 Shifts across all muscles (Figure 3E) showed stereotypical clustering of preferred torque directions - one 313 in the elbow-extension/shoulder-flexion quadrant and the other in the elbow-flexion/shoulder-extension 314 quadrant (Kurtzer et al., 2006a). 315 In the reaching task, NHPs made fast center-out reaches to peripheral targets arranged around the 316 starting position (Figure 1B). Figure 3D shows an example of pectoralis major muscle activity during 317 reaching. Maximal activity in the example was observed towards the top left target reflecting the need 318 for a large shoulder flexor torque to accelerate the arm towards that target (Graham et al., 2003). In the 319 bottom right direction, an activity peak can be seen about 200 ms later related to an antagonist burst to 320 decelerate the arm. EMG activity was integrated over a 200 ms period starting 150 ms before movement 14 321 onset (grey bar), to capture the activity associated with initial acceleration. Across all muscles, PRDs 322 (128 significant muscle samples) were skewed towards one of two directions (Figure 3F), one centred 323 around 110⁰ and one around 280⁰ (Kurtzer et al., 2006a). 324 As with the novel model analysis, we compared PTD and PRD for each muscle which had both a 325 significant PTD in the posture task and a significant PRD in the reaching task (N=122). A strong 326 consistent relationship between the PTDs and PRDs can be seen in Figure 4C where two clusters 327 emerge: one (bottom left cluster) that includes the shoulder extensors, the elbow flexors and all bi- 328 articulars and the other (top right) that includes the elbow extensors and shoulder flexors. Figure 4D 329 displays a histogram of the difference of PTD and PRD angles for all muscles. There is a systematic 330 shift of 152⁰ (circular r=0.87, p<0.001), very similar to that obtained for the network model. 331 332 333 Monkey M1 recordings We recorded and analysed the activity of 540 M1 neurons in 5 monkeys in the posture and 334 reaching tasks, with the same epochs as for muscle activity. Of these, 373 showed a significant PTD in 335 the posture task (example neuron in Figure 3G) and 424 M1 neurons that showed a significant PRD 336 during the reaching task (example neuron in Figure 3H). As seen in Figure 3I and 3J, the distributions of 337 PTDs and PRDs for all M1 neurons showed stereotypical bimodalities as have been previously reported 338 (Scott et al., 2001; Kurtzer et al., 2006a). 339 As with muscles, we compared PTD and PRD for each neuron which had both a significant PTD 340 and PRD (N=314). Figure 4E shows two distinct clusters when comparing the PTD and PRD of M1 341 neurons: one cluster with a PRD at 301⁰ and a PTD at 140⁰ (bottom left cluster), and another cluster 342 with a PRD at 123⁰ and a PTD at 325⁰. The relation between the two preferred directions of M1 neurons 343 is clearly shown in Figure 4F as a 151⁰ shift (circular r=0.43, p<0.001) between PRD and PTD. Cluster 15 344 locations were maxima of cell preferred directions convolved with a 2D windowed Gaussian distribution 345 (σ = 10⁰). This shift and clustering is very similar to both the units of the network model, and the muscle 346 recordings. Unlike the muscles and network model, a notable portion of M1 neurons show PTD-PRD 347 differences not located near the diagonal of the plot. The result is a relatively low PTD-PRD correlation 348 (r=0.43 cells, 0.84 model, 0.87 muscles) and large variance in their shift values (circular SD=61⁰ cells, 349 27⁰ model, 24⁰ muscles). We estimated whether these variances were significantly different by sampled 350 the PTD-PRD difference values with replacement to create distributions of variances. The distribution of 351 PTD-PRD variance for neurons had no overlap with either the muscle or model distributions. The 352 muscle and model distributions of variance shared a 30% overlap. 353 354 355 Prediction of patterns of muscle activity from M1 population signals Our first step to predict muscle activity during reaching was to classify the M1 neurons based on 356 their similarity to muscle groups during a postural load task. Figure 5 highlights the distribution of PTDs 357 of muscle for shoulder and elbow muscles. We defined four ranges in torque space representing flexors 358 and extensors at each joint: elbow flexors (90⁰ to 135⁰), shoulder extensors (135⁰ to 180⁰), elbow 359 extensors (270⁰ to 315⁰), and shoulder flexors (315⁰ to 360⁰). The four torque groups captured 75% of 360 motor cortical neurons: 55 neurons were classified as elbow flexor neurons, 71 as shoulder extensor 361 neurons, 54 as elbow extensor neurons, and 68 shoulder flexor neurons. When applied to our network 362 model, these groupings captured 60.8% of units, with 14.5% to 15.6% of units in each group. 363 Figure 6 displays the distribution of PRDs for each muscle group, the associated M1 neurons, 364 and associated network units. Note that these distributions represent vertical slices, 45⁰ thick, of the 365 PRD-PTD relationship shown in Figure 4E. The preferred reaching direction distributions for each 366 group of muscles were unimodal, with shoulder flexors preferring 134⁰ (r=0.85, p<0.001), elbow flexors 16 367 preferring 261⁰ (r=0.83, p<0.001), shoulder extensors preferring 317⁰ (r=0.99, p<0.001), and elbow 368 extensors preferring 98⁰ (r=0.63, p=0.03). Dispersion was relatively small for each muscle group 369 illustrating that muscle within a group had similar reaching preferences. 370 Each group of M1 neurons showed a significant unimodal distribution of PRDs, nearly identical 371 to that of their comparative muscle group (Figure 6). Shoulder flexor neurons showed a preferred 372 reaching direction of 128⁰ (r=0.66, p<0.001), elbow flexor neurons showed a preferred reaching 373 direction of 293⁰ (r=0.32, p<0.001), shoulder extensor neurons showed a preferred reaching direction of 374 313⁰ (r=0.48, p<0.001), and elbow extensor neurons showed a preferred reaching direction of 95⁰ 375 (r=0.35, p=0.002). M1 neurons associated with each muscle group showed a much greater range of 376 preferred reach directions. Watson-Williams tests between muscles and cells showed no difference in 377 means, except a small (33.8 degrees), but significant difference for elbow flexors (SF: F1,72=0.02 378 p=0.65, EF: F1,102=5.12 p=0.03, SE: F1,101=0.13 p=0.71, SF: F1,55=0.01 p=0.92). 379 The torque groupings were also used to classify neural network units in a similar fashion (Figure 380 6). Shoulder flexor units showed a preferred reaching direction of 119⁰ (r=0.92, p<0.001), elbow flexor 381 units showed a preferred reaching direction of 269⁰ (r=0.88, p<0.001), shoulder extensor units showed a 382 preferred reaching direction of 298⁰ (r=0.91, p<0.001), and elbow extensor units showed a preferred 383 reaching direction of 89⁰ (r=0.89, p<0.001). As the neural network units followed the same pattern as 384 both muscles and M1 neurons in distribution, it was not surprising that each torque-based group also had 385 close overlap with the PRD of both M1 neurons and muscles. 386 387 388 389 Muscle-like spatiotemporal dynamics While predicting the agonist burst activity of the spatiotemporal activity was a first step, we further examined whether M1 neurons could predict the full temporal pattern of shoulder and elbow 17 390 muscle activity across directions during reaching. We evaluated the population activity of each group of 391 M1 neurons over time and compared this to the activity of each group of muscles. 392 Illustrations of normalized population activity over time and across reach direction for each 393 group of M1 neurons can be seen in Figure 7A. Clear maximums of activity emerge prior to reach onset 394 in a primary direction for each group. Given that PRD was determined by the epoch of time around 395 reach onset, it is not a surprise that these maximums align with the PRD of each torque-classified group. 396 Qualitatively, the overall patterns of activity are similar between M1 and their corresponding muscle 397 groups. Given that each group seemed to have a similar profile of activity across time but for different 398 reach directions, we computed 2-dimensional correlations between each M1 group and all muscle 399 groups (Figure 7B) and found high degrees of correlation (shoulder flexor r=0.82, elbow flexor r=0.74, 400 shoulder extensor r=0.74, elbow extensor r=0.75). Each muscle group’s spatiotemporal activity was 401 predicted best by its respective M1 neuronal group, with latencies of maximal correlation that show M1 402 activity precedes muscle activity (shoulder flexor -30ms, elbow flexor -70ms, shoulder extensor -30ms, 403 elbow extensor -65ms). Figure 7C displays the aggregate of all four groups highlighting how specific 404 shifts in the timing and magnitude of agonist muscle activity before movement is preceded (average 405 latency -35ms) by similar spatiotemporal patterns in M1 (Scott, 1997). 406 We also investigated the spatiotemporal output of the subset of M1 neurons that appeared to have 407 an atypical, opposing PRD-PTD relationship. We decomposed the aggregate M1 activity by selecting 408 neurons that were within 30⁰ of the expected relationship (around the 151⁰ PRD-PTD), and those that 409 were opposite to these. As can be seen in Figure 8A and 8B respectively, both sets of neurons seemed to 410 have bursts of activity; however, the atypical M1 neurons had later temporal characteristics for their 411 burst (Figure 8C), which occurred in the opposite reaching direction. A cross-correlation of the 412 maximum aggregate activity showed that the atypical activity burst occurred 50ms later than the typical 18 413 agonist burst. Interestingly, the atypical neurons’ agonist-direction activity (solid red line) appeared to 414 be larger with a longer duration than the regular population’s antagonist-direction activity (dashed black 415 line), lasting well after the reaching movement was completed. Thus across the population, the preferred 416 direction of these atypical cells was aligned with its motor field after movement when maintaining the 417 hand at the peripheral target. 418 19 419 420 421 DISCUSSION Several studies have shown correlations between M1 activity and EMG in monkeys (Evarts, 422 1968; Humphrey, 1972; Fetz et al., 1989; Bennett and Lemon, 1996) and cats (Drew, 1993; Drew et al., 423 2002). However, given the diversity of EMG activity across muscles during a motor action, one can 424 likely find that a neuron’s activity will correlate with at least one of them. Here we show these 425 correlations remain when classification and prediction are separated. We classified M1 neurons into one 426 of four distinct motor fields (elbow flexor, elbow extensor, shoulder flexor, and shoulder extensor) using 427 a static load task. We then generated a population signal from each group and predicted the 428 spatiotemporal activity of the corresponding muscle during a dynamic reaching task. The preferred 429 direction of most neurons matched the directional preference of muscles with the same motor field. 430 Interestingly, a small proportion of neurons had preferences in the opposite direction. 431 A receptive field of a sensory neuron can be defined as the part of the body to which a stimulus 432 elicits activity by this neuron. These can be easily defined by repeated application of stimuli and finding 433 the parts of the body to which firing of a neuron correlates. It is implicitly assumed from this that a 434 neuron that fires in relation to a stimulus is caused by that stimulus. A related concept is a motor field, 435 which can be defined as the portion of the body associated with a given motor-related neuron. While the 436 target innervation, or muscle field, of some neurons, such as corticomotoneurons, can be identified by 437 spike-triggered averaging of EMG (Cheney and Fetz, 1980, 1985), this does not necessarily define the 438 portions of the body to which the neuron fires. Even when limiting to CM cells which have 439 monosynaptic connections to motoneuron pools, Griffin et al. (2015) showed how directional tuning, or 440 motor field, of some CM cells does not necessarily align with the muscle field, or connectivity. As well, 441 CM cells represent only a small fraction of corticospinal neurons that predominantly target the distal 20 442 musculature in primates (Buys et al., 1986; Palmer and Ashby, 1992), and are localized mainly in the 443 caudal portion of M1 or “new M1” (Rathelot and Strick, 2009). More directly, a motor field can be 444 defined based on whether a neuron is active when the animal performs a motor action with a given body 445 part, as we have done in the present study. However, unlike sensory fields, it is not necessarily true that 446 there is a causal link between the neuron’s activity (motor field) and motor output of that body part. 447 We examined this relationship using a relatively complex artificial neural network with 1000 448 neural units to observe the relationship between a unit’s connectivity and activity relative to the motor 449 periphery. The model displayed a strong similarity between the unit’s torque preference (PTD) and the 450 activity preference of its motor field (MFPTD) during the postural load task. However, the directional 451 preferences were not identical as there was a mean difference of 22 degrees. Thus, in general, the 452 activity of the unit during a task was a good indicator of its synaptic connectivity to the motor periphery. 453 This was more accurate for the units that had strong unidirectional connectivity without strong synaptic 454 connections to antagonist muscles. 455 The presence of PTD-MFPTD variability in the artificial neural network parallels previous 456 experimental work demonstrating a statistical relationship between the muscle field of CM cells (defined 457 by STA, Fetz and Cheney, 1978) and the associated patterns of activity of forearm and hand muscles 458 during a precision grip task (Bennett and Lemon, 1994). In that study, the observed correlations were 459 relatively modest, the highest of which was 0.69. Some of this variability may be explained by task- 460 dependant variation in the connectivity using STA (Buys et al., 1986). However, our network model 461 showed that a more complex pattern of synaptic connectivity may contribute to a lower overall 462 correlation value. Ultimately, our model suggests that a perfect correlation will not exist between muscle 463 fields and motor fields as the neuronal activity reflects not only the activity of target muscles, but also 464 the activity of the rest of the network (Lillicrap and Scott, 2013). 21 465 Our model also demonstrated that the preferred pattern of activity relative to the motor periphery 466 remains relatively constant across motor behaviours. In other words, if a unit was maximally active 467 when the elbow flexors were maximally active for postural loads, it will tend to be active when the 468 elbow flexors are maximally active during reaching. A fixed motor field is consistent with the 469 observation that neural responses can be predicted when loads are combined at the shoulder and elbow 470 (Gribble and Scott, 2002) and are similar for transient and sustained loads (Herter et al., 2009; 471 Pruszynski et al., 2014). It is also consistent with several studies that highlight how the reaching 472 directional preference of neurons and muscles both tend to rotate similarly with applied curl fields 473 (Cherian et al., 2013), changes in arm geometry (Scott and Kalaska, 1997) or start position (Caminiti et 474 al., 1991; Sergio and Kalaska, 1997). Again, there is some dispersion in the directional preference across 475 tasks or conditions, likely reflecting how unit activity is influenced by factors beyond its motor field. 476 Similar methodology to the present study was used by Sergio et al. (2005) to examine M1 activity 477 between isometric force production and a dynamic reaching paradigm. Their results show similar 478 patterns of correlation between isometric force and reaching preferred direction which support the 479 notion of M1 motor fields that persist across tasks. Here we show this specific correlation between the 480 activity of M1 neurons and its motor field. 481 It is important to recognize that a fixed motor field does not mean that a neuron is always active 482 when its associated muscles are active during voluntary motor actions. It has been shown that neurons 483 strongly active during precision grip were much less active for power grip that required more muscle 484 force (Muir and Lemon, 1983). As well, load representations can change between posture and 485 movement, but their directional preference remains relatively constant (Kurtzer et al., 2005) and can be 486 only active when the muscle is used for a specific phase of movement (Griffin et al., 2015). Even when 487 neural activity reflects features such as the direction of the spatial goal, these neurons likely remain 22 488 associated with a specific body part such as the wrist (Kakei et al., 1999) or elbow/shoulder (Sergio and 489 Kalaska, 2003). 490 While the essential PRD and PTD patterns are quite similar between network model and real 491 neurons, the variability in the difference between the PRDs and PTDs is notably greater in the neurons 492 than in the model. There are many possible sources for this difference in observed variability. First, 493 there is noise in the empirical estimates of the PRDs and PTDs, whereas in the model these quantities 494 can be measured perfectly. As well, the only source of noise in the model, given a fixed and 495 deterministic optimization procedure, is the random initialization of the weights, w. Second, our model 496 assumes that all of the model cortical units directly contributed to driving muscle activity by way of 497 monosynaptic connections. In reality, only a subset of M1 neurons project to the spinal cord, and most 498 of these synapse predominantly on spinal interneurons and can be influenced by spinal processing. As 499 well, we may expect that neurons that do not project to the spinal cord – which may connect to other 500 brain areas (Turner and DeLong, 2000), or recurrently within M1 – to show activity dissociated from the 501 muscles. As well, motor cortical neurons reflect aspects of motor function beyond patterns of motor 502 output, such as correlates of movement kinematics (Kalaska et al., 1989) and behavioural goals (Kakei 503 et al., 1999). Neural activity is also context (Hepp-Reymond et al., 1999), phase (Griffin et al., 2015) 504 and task-dependent (Buys et al., 1986; Kurtzer et al., 2005). These complexities will necessarily 505 influence the relationship between motor cortical activity across postural and movement tasks and likely 506 increase variability in the difference between their PRD and PTD. 507 At the same time, given the complexity of neural processing in M1 it is surprising its activity can 508 predict the patterns of muscle activity. The patterns are not exact however particularly related to activity 509 patterns when the muscles are antagonists. The general similarity between M1 and muscle activity 510 suggests that spinal processing is not dramatically altering descending commands (Lillicrap and Scott, 23 511 2013). While adding an additional network layer to our model to simulate the influence of a spinal cord 512 or other systems may have given us a higher network variability, adding complexity was not the aim of 513 this study. We were not interested in exactly modeling the descending stream from primary motor 514 cortex, but rather to producing testable hypotheses that stem from a system with an unchanging 515 relationship with the periphery. 516 Our analysis focussed on exploring the relationship between M1 neurons and flexor and extensor 517 muscle groups at the shoulder and elbow. However, this does not mean that M1 neurons are associated 518 with all muscles within a given group, nor that they are only associated with a single muscle group. 519 Most certainly M1 neurons synapse onto many motor neurons (directly or indirectly) including those in 520 different muscle groups and muscles that span multiple joints as demonstrated with STA (Cheney and 521 Fetz, 1985; McKiernan et al., 1998; Smith and Fetz, 2009). We find several neurons with load 522 preferences related to combined flexor or extensor loads at the shoulder and elbow, a pattern of activity 523 not observed for any muscles (Kurtzer et al., 2006b). These neurons likely have muscle fields that span 524 both joints. Even those neurons with load preferences associated with a single muscle group, likely have 525 a motor field beyond that group. 526 The idea of a motor field is closely related to the idea of muscle synergies. While the basic 527 definition of a muscle synergy is simply the cooperative action of two or more muscles, some theorize 528 that the motor system simplifies control by only varying a small number of muscle synergies (d’Avella 529 et al., 2006). One prediction from this theory is that there would be a fixed number of motor fields 530 represented in a region like M1. In our postural load task, these synergies should appear as clusters of 531 preferred loads in our M1 sample. In fact, the distribution of load preferences do appear to be clustered 532 into two groups one related to whole-limb flexion and one related to whole-limb extension (Kurtzer et 533 al., 2006a; Herter et al., 2009). However, this bias appears to be due to the presence of bi-articular 24 534 muscles (Lillicrap and Scott, 2013) – in a model with no bi-articular muscles, the distribution of load 535 preferences is relatively uniform with no obvious clustering. The units spanned all possible 536 combinations of control, rather than showing a reduced set of controls. The spanned distribution was 537 mirrored by the distributions of load and reach preferences observed for neurons in M1. As well, studies 538 using STA also highlight diverse connectivity in their cell samples (Smith and Fetz, 2009; Alstermark 539 and Isa, 2012). Thus, there are likely many more motor fields (and muscle synergies) represented in M1 540 neurons than muscles in the body. 541 It was surprising to find atypical cells where the directional preference of the neuron during 542 reaching was opposite to that of its muscle group identified from the postural load task. This sub- 543 population of neurons could not be identified in Scott (1997). In that study, neurons were first divided 544 into elbow and shoulder groupings by their response to passive movement of the arm. Separation into 545 flexor or extensor sub-groups was defined by their preferred direction during reaching, and thus, they 546 would have simply been classified with the antagonist muscle group at that joint. 547 The onset time of this atypical population of neurons was delayed as compared to neurons with 548 directional preferences aligned with the directional preference of their motor field. One possible 549 explanation is that these neurons are preferentially involved in controlling muscle activity when it is an 550 antagonist to decelerate the limb. Drew and colleagues (Drew, 1993; Drew et al., 2002) found that some 551 M1 neurons in cats during locomotion were only active when a specific modification of the muscle 552 activity was required, suggesting that the activity of some M1 neurons could be task-dependent. Task- 553 dependent processing in M1 has also been observed in previous reaching and posture comparisons 554 (Kurtzer et al., 2005). While the latency between atypical and typical activity was much shorter than the 555 latency between the muscular agonist and antagonist, a recent study by Griffin et al. (2015) also showed 556 that cells which were functionally tuned for antagonist activity show activity only shortly after those 25 557 tuned for agonist activity. The antagonist activity was relatively weak in the present study due to the 558 speed of reaching and contribution of friction from the exoskeleton assisting the deceleration of the 559 limb. The potential contribution of these atypical neurons for controlling antagonist muscle activity 560 could be examined by modifying the speed of the movement or inertia of the limb. 561 562 26 563 564 565 566 Acknowledgements 567 568 We would like to acknowledge Kim Moore, Simone Appaqaq, Justin Peterson, and Helen Bretzke for their laboratory and technical assistance. 569 570 Grants 571 572 573 574 This work was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR). E. Heming received a Doctoral Award from NSERC. M. Omrani received a Vanier Doctoral Award from CIHR. J. A. Pruszynski received salary awards from CIHR and the Human Frontier Science Program. S. H. Scott is supported by a GSK-CIHR Chair in Neuroscience. 575 576 Disclosures 577 578 S. H. Scott is associated with BKIN Technologies, which commercializes the KINARM device used in this study. 27 579 References 580 Alstermark B, Isa T. Circuits for Skilled Reaching and Grasping. Annu Rev Neurosci 35: 559–578, 2012. 581 582 d’Avella A, Portone A, Fernandez L, Lacquaniti F. Control of Fast-Reaching Movements by Muscle Synergy Combinations. J Neurosci 26: 7791–7810, 2006. 583 584 Bennett KM, Lemon RN. The influence of single monkey cortico-motoneuronal cells at different levels of activity in target muscles. J Physiol 477: 291–307, 1994. 585 586 Bennett KM, Lemon RN. Corticomotoneuronal contribution to the fractionation of muscle activity during precision grip in the monkey. J Neurophysiol 75: 1826–1842, 1996. 587 588 Buys EJ, Lemon RN, Mantel GW, Muir RB. Selective facilitation of different hand muscles by single corticospinal neurones in the conscious monkey. J Physiol 381: 529–549, 1986. 589 590 Cabel DW, Cisek P, Scott SH. Neural Activity in Primary Motor Cortex Related to Mechanical Loads Applied to the Shoulder and Elbow During a Postural Task. J Neurophysiol 86: 2102–2108, 2001. 591 592 593 Caminiti R, Johnson PB, Galli C, Ferraina S, Burnod Y. Making arm movements within different parts of space: the premotor and motor cortical representation of a coordinate system for reaching to visual targets. J Neurosci 11: 1182–1197, 1991. 594 595 Caminiti R, Johnson PB, Urbano A. Making arm movements within different parts of space: dynamic aspects in the primate motor cortex. J Neurosci 10: 2039–2058, 1990. 596 597 Cheney PD, Fetz EE. Functional classes of primate corticomotoneuronal cells and their relation to active force. J Neurophysiol 44: 773–791, 1980. 598 599 600 Cheney PD, Fetz EE. Comparable patterns of muscle facilitation evoked by individual corticomotoneuronal (CM) cells and by single intracortical microstimuli in primates: evidence for functional groups of CM cells. J Neurophysiol 53: 786–804, 1985. 601 602 Cheng EJ, Scott SH. Morphometry of Macaca mulatta forelimb. I. Shoulder and elbow muscles and segment inertial parameters. J Morphol 245: 206–224, 2000. 603 604 Cherian A, Fernandes HL, Miller LE. Primary motor cortical discharge during force field adaptation reflects muscle-like dynamics. J Neurophysiol 110: 768–783, 2013. 605 606 Drew T. Motor cortical activity during voluntary gait modifications in the cat. I. Cells related to the forelimbs. J Neurophysiol 70: 179–199, 1993. 607 608 Drew T, Jiang W, Widajewicz W. Contributions of the motor cortex to the control of the hindlimbs during locomotion in the cat. Brain Res Rev 40: 178–191, 2002. 609 610 Evarts EV. Relation of pyramidal tract activity to force exerted during voluntary movement. J Neurophysiol 31: 14–27, 1968. 611 Fetz EE, Cheney PD. Muscle fields of primate corticomotoneuronal cells. J Physiol (Paris) 74: 239–245, 1978. 28 612 613 Fetz EE, Cheney PD, Mewes K, Palmer S. Control of forelimb muscle activity by populations of corticomotoneuronal and rubromotoneuronal cells. Prog Brain Res 80: 437–449; discussion 427–430, 1989. 614 615 Flanders M, Pellegrini JJ, Soechting JF. Spatial/temporal characteristics of a motor pattern for reaching. J Neurophysiol 71: 811–813, 1994. 616 617 Fromm C, Evarts EV. Relation of motor cortex neurons to precisely controlled and ballistic movements. Neurosci Lett 5: 259–265, 1977. 618 Georgopoulos AP. Current issues in directional motor control. Trends Neurosci 18: 506–510, 1995. 619 620 Georgopoulos AP, Kalaska JF, Caminiti R, Massey JT. On the relations between the direction of twodimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex. J Neurosci 2: 1527–1537, 1982. 621 622 Graham KM, Moore KD, Cabel DW, Gribble PL, Cisek P, Scott SH. Kinematics and Kinetics of Multijoint Reaching in Nonhuman Primates. J Neurophysiol 89: 2667–2677, 2003. 623 624 Graham KM, Scott SH. Morphometry of macaca mulatta forelimb. III. moment arm of shoulder and elbow muscles. J Morphol 255: 301–314, 2003. 625 626 Gribble PL, Scott SH. Overlap of internal models in motor cortex for mechanical loads during reaching. Nature 417: 938–941, 2002. 627 628 Griffin DM, Hoffman DS, Strick PL. Corticomotoneuronal cells are “functionally tuned.” Science 350: 667– 670, 2015. 629 630 Hepp-Reymond M-C, Kirkpatrick-Tanner M, Gabernet L, Qi H-X, Weber B. Context-dependent force coding in motor and premotor cortical areas. Exp Brain Res 128: 123–133, 1999. 631 632 Herter TM, Korbel T, Scott SH. Comparison of Neural Responses in Primary Motor Cortex to Transient and Continuous Loads During Posture. J Neurophysiol 101: 150–163, 2009. 633 634 Herter TM, Kurtzer I, Cabel DW, Haunts KA, Scott SH. Characterization of Torque-Related Activity in Primary Motor Cortex During a Multijoint Postural Task. J Neurophysiol 97: 2887–2899, 2007. 635 636 Hong DA, Corcos DM, Gottlieb GL. Task dependent patterns of muscle activation at the shoulder and elbow for unconstrained arm movements. J Neurophysiol 71: 1261–1265, 1994. 637 Humphrey DR. Relating motor cortex spike trains to measures of motor performance. Brain Res 40: 7–18, 1972. 638 639 Johnson MTV, Coltz JD, Ebner TJ. Encoding of target direction and speed during visual instruction and arm tracking in dorsal premotor and primary motor cortical neurons. Eur J Neurosci 11: 4433–4445, 1999. 640 641 Kakei S, Hoffman DS, Strick PL. Muscle and Movement Representations in the Primary Motor Cortex. Science 285: 2136–2139, 1999. 642 643 644 Kalaska JF, Cohen DA, Hyde ML, Prud’homme M. A comparison of movement direction-related versus load direction-related activity in primate motor cortex, using a two-dimensional reaching task. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 9: 2080–2102, 1989. 29 645 646 Karst GM, Hasan Z. Timing and magnitude of electromyographic activity for two-joint arm movements in different directions. J Neurophysiol 66: 1594–1604, 1991. 647 648 Kasser RJ, Cheney PD. Characteristics of corticomotoneuronal postspike facilitation and reciprocal suppression of EMG activity in the monkey. J Neurophysiol 53: 959–978, 1985. 649 650 Kurtzer I, Herter TM, Scott SH. Random change in cortical load representation suggests distinct control of posture and movement. Nat Neurosci 8: 498–504, 2005. 651 652 Kurtzer I, Herter TM, Scott SH. Nonuniform Distribution of Reach-Related and Torque-Related Activity in Upper Arm Muscles and Neurons of Primary Motor Cortex. J Neurophysiol 96: 3220–3230, 2006a. 653 654 Kurtzer I, Pruszynski JA, Herter TM, Scott SH. Primate Upper Limb Muscles Exhibit Activity Patterns That Differ From Their Anatomical Action During a Postural Task. J Neurophysiol 95: 493–504, 2006b. 655 656 Lemon RN, Muir RB, Mantel GWH. The effects upon the activity of hand and forearm muscles of intracortical stimulation in the vicinity of corticomotor neurones in the conscious monkey. Exp Brain Res 66: 621–637, 1987. 657 658 Lillicrap TP, Scott SH. Preference Distributions of Primary Motor Cortex Neurons Reflect Control Solutions Optimized for Limb Biomechanics. Neuron 77: 168–179, 2013. 659 660 Marsden CD, Obeso JA, Rothwell JC. The function of the antagonist muscle during fast limb movements in man. J Physiol 335: 1–13, 1983. 661 662 663 McKiernan BJ, Marcario JK, Karrer JH, Cheney PD. Corticomotoneuronal Postspike Effects in Shoulder, Elbow, Wrist, Digit, and Intrinsic Hand Muscles During a Reach and Prehension Task. J Neurophysiol 80: 1961– 1980, 1998. 664 Muir RB, Lemon RN. Corticospinal neurons with a special role in precision grip. Brain Res 261: 312–316, 1983. 665 666 Murphy JT, Wong YC, Kwan HC. Sequential activation of neurons in primate motor cortex during unrestrained forelimb movement. J Neurophysiol 53: 435–445, 1985. 667 668 Oby ER, Ethier C, Miller LE. Movement representation in the primary motor cortex and its contribution to generalizable EMG predictions. J Neurophysiol 109: 666–678, 2013. 669 670 Palmer E, Ashby P. Corticospinal projections to upper limb motoneurones in humans. J Physiol 448: 397–412, 1992. 671 672 Philip BA, Rao N, Donoghue JP. Simultaneous reconstruction of continuous hand movements from primary motor and posterior parietal cortex. Exp Brain Res 225: 361–375, 2013. 673 674 Phillips CG. Laying the ghost of “muscles versus movements.” Can J Neurol Sci J Can Sci Neurol 2: 209–218, 1975. 675 676 Pruszynski JA, Omrani M, Scott SH. Goal-Dependent Modulation of Fast Feedback Responses in Primary Motor Cortex. J Neurosci 34: 4608–4617, 2014. 30 677 678 Raphael G, Tsianos GA, Loeb GE. Spinal-Like Regulator Facilitates Control of a Two-Degree-of-Freedom Wrist. J Neurosci 30: 9431–9444, 2010. 679 680 Rathelot J-A, Strick PL. Subdivisions of primary motor cortex based on cortico-motoneuronal cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106: 918–923, 2009. 681 682 Schwartz AB. Motor cortical activity during drawing movements: population representation during sinusoid tracing. J Neurophysiol 70: 28–36, 1993. 683 684 Scott SH. Comparison of Onset Time and Magnitude of Activity for Proximal Arm Muscles and Motor Cortical Cells Before Reaching Movements. J Neurophysiol 77: 1016–1022, 1997. 685 686 Scott SH. Apparatus for measuring and perturbing shoulder and elbow joint positions and torques during reaching. J Neurosci Methods 89: 119–127, 1999. 687 688 Scott SH. The role of primary motor cortex in goal-directed movements: insights from neurophysiological studies on non-human primates. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13: 671–677, 2003. 689 690 Scott SH, Gribble PL, Graham KM, Cabel DW. Dissociation between hand motion and population vectors from neural activity in motor cortex. Nature 413: 161–165, 2001. 691 692 Scott SH, Kalaska JF. Reaching Movements With Similar Hand Paths But Different Arm Orientations. I. Activity of Individual Cells in Motor Cortex. J Neurophysiol 77: 826–852, 1997. 693 694 Sergio LE, Hamel-Pâquet C, Kalaska JF. Motor Cortex Neural Correlates of Output Kinematics and Kinetics During Isometric-Force and Arm-Reaching Tasks. J Neurophysiol 94: 2353–2378, 2005. 695 696 697 Sergio LE, Kalaska JF. Systematic Changes in Directional Tuning of Motor Cortex Cell Activity With Hand Location in the Workspace During Generation of Static Isometric Forces in Constant Spatial Directions. J Neurophysiol 78: 1170–1174, 1997. 698 699 Sergio LE, Kalaska JF. Systematic Changes in Motor Cortex Cell Activity With Arm Posture During Directional Isometric Force Generation. J Neurophysiol 89: 212–228, 2003. 700 701 Singh K, Melis EH, Richmond FJR, Scott SH. Morphometry of Macaca mulatta forelimb. II. Fiber-type composition in shoulder and elbow muscles. J Morphol 251: 323–332, 2002. 702 703 Smith WS, Fetz EE. Synaptic Linkages Between Corticomotoneuronal Cells Affecting Forelimb Muscles in Behaving Primates. J Neurophysiol 102: 1040–1048, 2009. 704 705 Todorov E. Direct cortical control of muscle activation in voluntary arm movements: a model. Nat Neurosci 3: 391–398, 2000. 706 707 Toxopeus CM, de Jong BM, Valsan G, Conway BA, Leenders KL, Maurits NM. Direction of Movement Is Encoded in the Human Primary Motor Cortex. PLoS ONE 6: e27838, 2011. 708 709 Turner RS, DeLong MR. Corticostriatal Activity in Primary Motor Cortex of the Macaque. J Neurosci 20: 7096–7108, 2000. 31 710 711 Wierzbicka MM, Wiegner AW, Shahani BT. Role of agonist and antagonist muscles in fast arm movements in man. Exp Brain Res 63: 331–340, 1986. 712 713 32 714 Figure 1. Experimental posture and reaching tasks. A. In the posture task, the monkey was required to 715 maintain its hand at a central target while flexion and/or extension torques were applied at the elbow and 716 shoulder. The right panel illustrates the eight combinations of torques and an unloaded condition. B. In 717 the reaching task, relatively fast reaches were made from a central origin to eight peripheral targets. The 718 right panel displays the four different combinations of targets used in the datasets. 719 720 Figure 2. Predictions from the static neural network model. A. A representative schematic of one of the 721 models. Two network units, z, are shown connected to muscle outputs, u, via random weightings shown 722 by the thickness of the lines. The output variables, u, drove mono- and bi-articular muscles on a two- 723 joint arm, as shown. The values of the units in z were optimized to solve analogues of the reaching and 724 posture tasks. B. The relationship between each unit’s preferred torque direction in the posture task and 725 the calculated preferred torque direction of its motor field (defined by synaptic connectivity) in one 726 network of 1000 units. The size of the circle for each unit corresponds to its strength of tuning. Note that 727 the muscles, plotted here for reference, must lie on the diagonal as they correspond with their own motor 728 field. 729 730 Figure 3. Preferred torque and reaching directions. A. Polar histogram of each network units’ preferred 731 torque direction (PTD) in one network. Angles are in torque space (shoulder torque x-axis, elbow torque 732 y-axis). B. Polar histogram of each network units’ preferred reaching direction (PRD) in one network. C. 733 Exemplar activity of pectoralis major in eight shoulder/elbow loaded conditions (unloaded condition not 734 shown) during the last three seconds of in the posture task. The arrow in the centre denotes the muscle’s 735 preferred torque direction (351 degrees). D. Exemplar activity of pectoralis major around reach onset 736 while reaching to the eight targets (set iii in Fig. 1B). The arrow in the centre denotes the muscle’s PRD 33 737 (140 degrees). E-F. Averages of the PTD and PRD of all muscle samples are shown with 95% 738 confidence intervals as shaded areas around each line. G. Activity of an exemplar M1 neuron in the 739 eight shoulder/elbow loaded conditions (unloaded condition not shown) during the last three seconds of 740 in the posture task. The arrow in the centre denotes the neuron’s preferred torque direction (155 741 degrees). H. Activity of an exemplar M1 neuron around reach onset while reaching to the eight targets 742 (Set iii in Fig. 1B). The arrow in the centre denotes the neuron’s PRD (119 degrees). I-J. Polar 743 histograms of all significant PTDs and PRDs across the population of recorded M1 neurons. 744 745 Figure 4. E. Relationship between PTD and PRD (directional preferences must be significant in both 746 tasks) for network units, muscles, and M1 neurons. A. Scatter relationship for network units. B. 747 Histogram of the difference between PTD and PRD for network units, inset shows as polar plot. C. 748 Scatter relationship for muscle recordings. D. Histogram of the difference between PTD and PRD for 749 muscle recordings. E. Scatter relationship for M1 neuron recordings. F. Histogram of the difference 750 between PTD and PRD for M1 neuron recordings. 751 752 Figure 5. Distribution of load preferences for M1 neurons and individual muscles. The number of 753 recordings for each muscle is indicated in brackets beside the muscle name. The four assigned motor 754 fields are shown as vertical shaded stripes with their titles marked at the top. 755 756 Figure 6. Distribution of preferred reaching direction for each muscle group, and assigned M1 neurons 757 and network units. Each distribution is normalized as a percentage of the total number of significant 758 PRDs. The x-axis starts at rightward reach (0⁰) and increases counter clockwise. 759 34 760 Figure 7. Spatiotemporal dynamics of muscle groups and associated M1 neuron populations during 761 reaching. A. Colormaps of the aggregate activity over time (x-axis) and across reach direction (y-axis) 762 of each muscle group and associated population of M1 neurons. Data are smoothed in the y-axis for 763 display purposes. B. Two-dimensional correlation coefficients between the spatiotemporal patterns of 764 muscle and neuron groups. Each graph is one muscle group and the bars show the coefficients between 765 that muscle’s spatiotemporal pattern and the pattern of the four populations of M1 neurons. C. 766 Aggregate activity of all neurons and all muscles were generated by rotating the y-axis of each group so 767 that their directions of maximal agonist activity were aligned with the Agonist direction, and then 768 averaged. 769 770 Figure 8. Aggregate spatiotemporal colormaps of M1 neurons that have similar and opposing PRDs 771 compared to their associated muscle group. A. Colormap of population activity of all neurons whose 772 PRD was within 30⁰ of its assigned muscle group (typical neurons). Aggregate of neural activity was 773 rotated as in Figure 7. B Colormap of population activity of all neurons whose PRD was greater than 774 150⁰ away from that of its assigned muscle group (atypical neurons). C. Spatiotemporal pattern of 775 activation for typical (black lines) and atypical (red lines) neurons when the muscle group acts as an 776 agonist (solid line) and an antagonist (dashed line). 777 778 35 A. Postural contraction against loads B. Monkey reaches to peripheral targets i ii iii iv A. w z 1 u Elb Flex 2 Elb Ext 3 Sho Flex 4 5 Sho Ext 6 Bi Flex Bi Ext N B. Motor Field PTD (°) 360 0 0 Posture PTD (°) 360 A. Model Posture PTD C. EMG in Posture Task E. EMG Posture PTD G. M1 cell in Posture Task I. N=373 Model Reach PRD D. EMG in Reaching Task F. EMG Reach PRD N=125 1au B. 1s 20 sp/s BR Bi Br DA DP PM Tlat Tlong Tmid 1s H. M1 cell in Reaching Task J. M1 Reach PRD N=424 20 sp/s 1au N=133 1s M1 Posture PTD 1s B. A. Neurons (%) Reach PRD (°) 20 0 0 Posture PTD (°) 10 0 360 C. 0 D. Recordings (%) 0 90 180 270 360 270 360 270 360 Difference (°) PRD-PTD 30 179 Reach PRD (°) 152° 15 5 180 25 N=122 152° 20 15 10 5 180 0 Posture PTD (°) 0 360 E. F. 179 0 12 10 Neurons (%) Reach PRD (°) PRD-PTD 25 179 0 180 90 180 Difference (°) PRD-PTD N=314 151° 8 6 4 2 0 Posture PTD (°) 360 0 0 90 180 Difference (°) Elbow Shoulder flexor extensor Elbow Shoulder extensor flexor Model units M1 neurons (373) Biceps (25) Brachioradialis (18) Brachialis (12) Long head triceps (24) Posterior deltoid (18) Lateral triceps (8) Middle triceps (2) Anterior deltoid (14) Pectoralis major (12) 0 45 90 135 180 225 270 315 360 A. shoulder flexor B. elbow flexor D. elbow extensor 30% Muscles M1 neurons Network 20% 10% 0% C. shoulder extensor 30% 20% 10% 0% 0° 360° 0° 360° Preferred reaching direction Preferred reaching direction EMG behavior B. C. 1 Aggregate M1 prediction Antagonist Correlation Shoulder flexor related M1 prediction Reach direction A. 0 Agonist Reach direction Elbow flexor related Correlation 1 0 Aggregate EMG behavior Antagonist Correlation Shoulder extensor related Reach direction 1 0 Agonist Reach direction Correlation 1 Elbow extensor related 0 -1 -1 1 Time around reach onset (s) 1 SF EF SE EE neuron group -1 0 Time around reach onset (s) 1 Agonist Antagonist A. Agonist Antagonist B. -1000 100 Percent max activity C. 1000 0 Time around reach onset (ms) 80 60 40 20 0 -500 0 500 Time around reach onset (ms)