Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
JOURNAL OF MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING 31:1061–1066 (2010) Original Research Exposure to High-Field MRI Does Not Affect Cognitive Function Marc Schlamann, MD,1* Melanie A. Voigt, MD,1 Stefan Maderwald, MS,1,2 Andreas K. Bitz, MS,1,2 Oliver Kraff, MS,1,2 Susanne C. Ladd, MS, MD,1,2 Mark E. Ladd, MS,1,2 Michael Forsting, MD,1 and Hans Wilhelm, MS3 Purpose: To assess potential cognitive deficits under the influence of static magnetic fields at various field strengths some studies already exist. These studies were not focused on attention as the most vulnerable cognitive function. Additionally, mostly no magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) sequences were performed. Materials and Methods: In all, 25 right-handed men were enrolled in this study. All subjects underwent one MRI examination of 63 minutes at 1.5 T and one at 7 T within an interval of 10 to 30 days. The order of the examinations was randomized. Subjects were referred to six standardized neuropsychological tests strictly focused on attention immediately before and after each MRI examination. Differences in neuropsychological variables between the timepoints before and after each MRI examination were assessed and P-values were calculated Results: Only six subtests revealed significant differences between pre- and post-MRI. In these tests the subjects achieved better results in post-MRI testing than in preMRI testing (P ¼ 0.013–0.032). The other tests revealed no significant results. Conclusion: The improvement in post-MRI testing is only explicable as a result of learning effects. MRI examinations, even in ultrahigh-field scanners, do not seem to have any persisting influence on the attention networks of human cognition immediately after exposure. Key Words: neuropsychology; ultra-high-field cognition J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2010;31:1061–1066. C 2010 Wiley-Liss, Inc. V 1 MRI; Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Radiology and Neuroradiology, University Hospital Essen, Germany. 2 Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, University Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany. 3 Department of Neurology, University Hospital Essen, Germany. *Address reprint requests to: M.S., Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Radiology and Neuroradiology, University Hospital Essen, Hufelandstr. 55, 45122 Essen, Germany. E-mail: [email protected] Received March 21, 2009; Accepted November 20, 2009. DOI 10.1002/jmri.22065 Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). C 2010 Wiley-Liss, Inc. V MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING (MRI) has been utilized in medical diagnostics for more than 25 years. In that period, more than 300 million MRI examinations have been performed (1), and currently more than 50,000 are performed daily (2). Very few critical incidents have occurred in this time, and these were mostly caused by nonobservance of common precautions, for example, examination of patients with implants not suitable for MRI (3). Generally, MRI is regarded as a harmless procedure in clinical routine, operating without ionizing radiation and without harmful effects on the human body when the known precautions such as prevention of projectile effects are observed. The increasing dissemination of scanners with field strengths above 3 T has led to new discussions on possible biological effects and damages caused by stronger magnetic fields and/or higher absorption inside the human body of the transmitted electromagnetic radiofrequency (RF) radiation necessary for tissue excitation. Possible impairment or disturbing events to the human brain might be reflected in cognitive deficits. Some studies have already assessed potential cognitive deficits under the influence of static magnetic fields at various field strengths (4–7). However, MR sequences (ie, magnetic gradients and high-frequency electromagnetic fields) were not assessed as part of most of these studies. Hence, possible effects caused by the interaction of the static magnetic field, gradient fields, and RF energy could not be detected. Additionally, the neuropsychological test battery was short and not focused on the most vulnerable cognitive function, which is attention (8–11). A few studies have assessed subtests on attention, among other things, but here also no imaging sequences were performed (12–14). One study performed sequences for sodium imaging at 9.4 T, but the test battery was not strictly focused on attention (15). To help close this gap, the aim of the present study was to evaluate the potential effects of MRI examinations at 1.5 T and 7 T on attention. MATERIALS AND METHODS The local Ethics Committee authorized the examinations as part of fundamental research on high-field 1061 1062 MR. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior to scanning. Subjects and Study Protocol A total of 25 subjects without history of neurological diseases were enrolled in the study. To assure homogeneity of the sample, only right-handed men aged between 20 and 45 years (mean 31.2 years) with comparable intelligence were examined: Intelligence was assessed by means of a short ‘‘paper-and-pencil’’ test (Multiple Choice Vocabulary Intelligence Test). The hereby determined IQ ranged from 104 to 145 (mean 123.3). All subjects had to be native German speakers to ensure equivalent conditions for the neuropsychological testing. MR Examination All 25 subjects underwent one MRI examination at 1.5 T (Magnetom Avanto, Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany) and one at 7 T (Magnetom 7 T, Siemens Healthcare) with an interval of 10–30 days (mean 18.1 days). The order of the examinations was randomized. Twelve subjects were first examined at 1.5 T and 13 started at 7 T. The cranial MR examination consisted of a fixed variety of spin echo and gradient echo sequences (2D and 3D) using the standard 12-channel receive-only matrix head coil at 1.5 T and an 8-channel transmit/ receive head coil (Rapid Biomedical, Wuerzburg, Germany) at 7 T. The sequences at 1.5 T and at 7 T were comparable with respect to maximum gradient strength and gradient slope. The whole-body specific absorption rate (SAR), which was averaged over the entire scan time and all participating subjects, amounted to 0.173 W/kg. This is 8.64% of the corresponding limit in ‘‘normal mode’’ (2 W/kg) or 4.32% of the limit in ‘‘first-level mode’’ (4 W/kg). At 7 T, fewer slices were acquired in the same time to compensate for the higher energy absorption in human tissue at 300 MHz. More relevant might be the local SAR distribution in the head. This information would only be accessible through numerical simulations in dedicated models of each individual head, so that it is not available for this study. All subjects underwent MRI examinations of 63 minutes, one at 1.5 T and one at 7 T in the supine position. The subjects were contacted repeatedly during the MR examination to avoid falling asleep. Neuropsychological Testing Study participants were referred to standardized neuropsychological tests immediately before and after each MRI examination in a separate room outside the scanner and control rooms. In total, four neuropsychological examinations were performed in each subject. Because of the presumed volatility of possible effects, neuropsychological testing was strictly focused on attention. Due to the widespread active neuronal networks that are involved in attention, this Schlamann et al. is considered to be the most vulnerable cognitive function (8–11). A well-validated set of six standardized psychometric tests was performed. Two paperand-pencil tests were followed by four tests at the computer using TAP 2.1 software (Test for Attentional Performance, Psytest Psychologische Testsysteme, Herzogenrath, Germany). All tests were monitored by the same experienced neuropsychologist. To minimize learning effects, a prestudy training session was held in which the complete test battery was explained and practiced once before the actual measurements were obtained. Paper-and-Pencil Tests To measure the subjects’ attention and psychomotor speed, Reitan’s Trail Making Test version B (TMT-B) was performed. Reitan’s Trail Making Test version A (TMT-A) was used to measure the rate of information processing, psychomotor tracking speed, and handeye coordination. In test version A, the task is to connect numbers in a sequence (ie, 1-2-3, etc.) by drawing a line as quickly as possible; in test version B, test numbers and letters have to be connected (eg, 1A-2B3C, etc.). The corresponding time was measured by a stopwatch and recorded. Computer-Based Tests The test ‘‘Working Memory’’ of the TAP software checks the control of information flow and actualization of information in working memory. There is no clear distinction between processes of selective attention and ‘‘working memory’’ (16). This task requires continuous control of the information flow through short-term memory by presenting numbers on the screen that must be compared with previously presented numbers. The repetition of a number within a short interval has to be confirmed by pressing a key. The test ‘‘Flexibility’’ was used to evaluate the orientation flexibility of the focus of attention. Mental flexibility seems to be an important aspect of cognitive functioning, with the extremes of high distractibility or flight of ideas on the one side and perseveration or rigidity on the other. The flexibility of focused attention was tested by a mental alternation between letters and numbers. For testing, two stimuli, one from each of these symbol sets, were presented simultaneously and randomly on the left or the right side of the fixation point. In each subsequent presentation, the target changes from letter to number and back again. The subject has to press a key as quickly as possible on the side of the target (left or right) with the corresponding target, either letter or number. Analysis was performed for the overall test, and subanalysis was performed for ‘‘change’’ and ‘‘no change’’ of the target side in comparison to the side of the preceding target. The test ‘‘Divided Attention’’ was performed to test the ability of dividing attention and concentrating on two tasks. Brain-damaged patients frequently report difficulties in situations where various aspects must concurrently be paid attention to (17). Divided attention performance can be investigated by executing Cognitive Function and High-Field MRI 1063 Table 1 P-values of the Two ‘ Paper-and-Pencil’’ Tests (TMT-A and TMT-B) and the Four Computer-Based Tests (Working Memory, Divided Attention, Incompatibility, Flexibility), Including Subanalyses Test TMT-A 1.5 T TMT-A 7 T TMT-B 1.5 T TMT-B 7 T Working Memory RT 1.5 T Working Memory RT 7 T Divided Attention RT 1.5 T Divided Attention RT 7 T Divided Attention RT 1.5 T Divided Attention RT 7 T Incompatibility RT 1.5 T Subtest P-value Test Subtest P- value Auditory Auditory Visual Visual Compatible 0.242 0.065 0.013 0.032 0.689 0.098 0.539 0.572 0.617 0.021 0.946 Incompatibility RT 7 T Incompatibility RT 1.5 T Incompatibility RT 7 T Incompatibility 1.5 T Incompatibility 7 T Flexibility RT 1.5 T Flexibility RT 7 T Flexibility RT 1.5 T Flexibility RT 7 T Flexibility 1.5 T Flexibility 7 T Compatible Incompatible Incompatible Overall Overall Change Change No change No change Overall Overall 0.061 0.778 0.031 0.732 0.024 0.019 0.764 0.128 0.071 0.054 0.948 The results are based on tests for differences in pre-MRI testing vs. post-MRI testing. RT, reaction time. dual tasks. This is realized by a visual and an auditory task which have to be carried out simultaneously. The visual task consists of crosses that appear in a random configuration in a 4 4 matrix. The subject has to detect whether the crosses form the corners of a square. The auditory task includes a regular sequence of high and low beeps. The subject has to detect an irregularity in the sequence. Analysis was performed for the auditory and the visual part. Finally, the test ‘‘Incompatibility’’ was performed to evaluate the addition of interference through an incompatibility of stimulus and reaction (‘‘Simon Effect’’) (18). Arrows pointing to the left or the right are presented on the left or the right of the fixation point on the screen. The subject has to press a key on the side indicated by the direction of the arrow—independent of the position of the arrow. If the side of the arrow presentation and the side of response are in concordance, the condition is classified as ‘‘compatible,’’ otherwise as ‘‘incompatible.’’ Subanalysis was performed for ‘‘compatibility’’ and ‘‘incompatibility.’’ Reaction times and number of failures of each subtest were determined, yielding 22 variables for analysis in each subject in all neuropsychological sessions. Statistical Analysis All analyses were performed using SPSS software (SPSS 15, Chicago, IL). A P-value below 0.05 was considered to indicate a statistically significant difference. Differences in neuropsychological variables between the timepoints before and after each MRI examination were assessed by use of the Wilcoxon rank test. P-values were calculated and the differences between the results of the 1.5 T and the 7 T examinations were calculated. Before and after the MR examinations the subjects had to complete a questionnaire on personal state to exclude malaise or nausea related to the examination as adulterant factors in neuropsychological testing (19). RESULTS None of the subjects reported malaise after the examinations. The interval between the MR examinations of the 25 subjects at 1.5 T and 7 T ranged from 10–30 days (mean 18.7). Differences in the neuropsychological testing are shown in Table 1 (pre-MRI examination versus post-MRI examination). The Wilcoxon rank test revealed no statistically significant differences in most subtests. P-values varied between 0.013 and 0.948 (Table 2). Only six subtests revealed significant differences between pre- and post-MRI. TMT-B revealed P ¼ 0.013 at 1.5 T and P ¼ 0.032 at 7 T. The subtest ‘‘Divided Attention, visual’’ revealed a P-value of 0.021 at 7 T, the subtest ‘‘Incompatibility, overall’’ revealed a Pvalue of 0.024 at 7 T, the subtest ‘‘Incompatibility incompatible’’ revealed a P-value of 0.031 at 7 T, and the subtest ‘‘Flexibility, change reaction time (RT)’’ revealed a P-value of 0.019 at 1.5 T. Additionally, three borderline cases were noted: TMTA revealed a P-value of 0.065 at 7 T, subtest ‘‘Incompatibility compatible’’ a P-value of 0.061 at 7 T, and subtest ‘‘Flexibility overall ’’ a P-value of 0.054 at 1.5 T. All cases with statistical significance were related to improvements in test performance postexposure. Seventeen of 25 subjects (68%) achieved better results in the TMT-B test following the 1.5 T examinations. Sixteen of 25 subjects (64%) improved in the TMT-B test following the 7 T examination with respect to the pre7 T test. Seventeen of 25 subjects (68%) improved in the ‘‘Incompatibility, overall’’ subtest following the 7 T examination. Seventeen of 25 subjects (68%) had better results in the ‘‘Divided Attention, visual’’ subtest following the 7 T examinations. Seventeen of 25 subjects (68%) had better results in the ‘‘Incompatibility, incompatible’’ subtest after 7 T. Nineteen of 25 subjects (76%) had better results in the ‘‘Flexibility, change’’ subtest after 1.5 T. DISCUSSION With the increasing spread of MR scanners with higher field strengths, the question of possible negative side effects on the human body has moved into the focus of interest. Patients have reported transient side effects of MRI examinations such as metallic taste, vertigo, and dizziness (13,19). There are three different electromagnetic fields affecting the body during an MR examination. First, 1064 Schlamann et al. Table 2 Standard Deviation and Mean Value of the Subtests Test Mean value Standard deviation Test Mean value Standard Deviation TMT-A pre 1.5T TMT-B pre 1.5T TMT-A after 1.5T TMT-B after 1.5T TMT-A pre 7T TMT-B pre 7T TMT - A after 7T TMT - B after 7T Working memory RT pre 1.5T Working memory RT after 1.5T Working memory RT pre 7T Working memory RT after 1.5T Divided attention RT auditory pre 1.5T Divided attention RT auditory after 1.5T Divided attention RT auditory pre 7T Divided attention RT auditory after 7T Divided attention visual RT pre 1.5T Divided attention visual RT after 1.5T Divided attention visual RT pre 7T Divided attention visual RT after 1.5T Incompatibility compatible RT pre 1.5T Incompatibility compatible RT after 1.5T 24.4 48.8 23.2 42.2 24.2 52.8 21.9 45.0 586.9 595.8 591.3 627.2 538.3 541.6 533.0 534.7 721.84 713.56 743.16 711.6 403.8 406.0 4.2 14.4 5.7 9.0 7.3 23.9 6.1 14.9 143.4 145.6 150.8 198.1 82.5 76.5 76.6 69.6 95.7 87.9 97.2 85.2 50.8 55.2 Incompatibility compatible RT pre 7T Incompatibility compatible RT after 1.5T Incompatibility incompatible RT pre 1.5T Incompatibility incompatible RT after 1.5T Incompatibility incompatible RT pre 7T Incompatibility incompatible RT after 7T Incompatibility overall RT pre 1.5T Incompatibility overall RT after 1.5T Incompatibility overall RT pre 7T Incompatibility overall RT after 7T Flexibility RT change pre 1.5T Flexibility RT change after 1.5T Flexibility RT change pre 7T Flexibility RT change after 7T Flexibility RT no change pre 1.5T Flexibility RT no change after 1.5T Flexibility RT no change pre 7T Flexibility RT no change after 7T Flexibility RT overall pre 1.5T Flexibility RT overall after 1.5T Flexibility RT overall pre 7T Flexibility RT overall after 1.5T 415.7 401.7 419.7 418.7 435.1 421.6 410.5 398.0 425.3 411.1 536.7 504.7 533.5 525.8 568.2 542.6 581.8 559.6 557.0 532.3 568.0 550.4 57.9 50.7 58.8 51.7 61.6 53.3 53.5 74.5 57.8 49.8 85.5 100.7 73.3 91.5 107.3 109.4 112.3 97.3 98.6 103.5 97.3 93.5 RT, reaction time/msec. the static magnetic field is responsible for aligning the protons of the body, and thus for the number of protons orientated parallel to B0 for imaging. Higher field strength produces more signal, as the relation between B0 and the aligned spins is linear. Second, the gradient magnetic fields with both spatial and temporal variance are responsible for the spatial encoding of the received signal. A well-known fact is that these gradient magnetic fields can induce neural stimulation (20). Even cardiac stimulation has been described in animal experiments. These effects have been observed in studies both with and without an additional external static magnetic field of 1.5 T (21,22). Third, RF pulses excite the signal-emitting protons. These RF fields generate thermal energy within the tissue. Studies have demonstrated that examinations with a whole-body absorption rate of 6 W/kg averaged over 16 minutes are still tolerated by volunteers (23), although the clinical limit for ‘‘first-level mode’’ is currently lower at 4 W/kg. Our study is the first where neuropsychological testing was combined with the performance of clinical routine MRI sequences at different field strengths, including the application of gradient magnetic field switching and RF exposure in addition to the static magnetic field. Our results indicate that MRI has no persisting measurable effects on human cognition, as attention networks are the most vulnerable parts of cognition and show no effects after MRI imaging even at 7 T. Former Studies Effects of MRI examinations on the human brain were demonstrated very early after the introduction of clini- cal MRI by von Klitzing (24) in 1986. He showed that auditory evoked potentials were altered in a static magnetic field of 0.35 T. These alterations normalized 15 minutes after exposure. Hong and Shellock (25) and Müller and Hotz (26), however, could not confirm these results. Pacini et al (27) reported that cell cultures of human nerve cells show morphological and biochemical changes after exposure to a static magnetic field of 0.35 T for 15 minutes. These results could not be confirmed (28). In the past several years, several studies involving neuropsychological testing have been published on the possible effects of static magnetic fields as well as low-frequency gradient magnetic fields and radiofrequency fields. Chakeres et al (4) performed neuropsychological examinations in 12 women and 13 men after lying in a static magnetic field of 8 T and 0.05 T. The cognitive assessment included measures of learning and retention, verbal fluency (spontaneous word generation), auditory attention, and auditory working memory; consequently, it was broad-based. The collective was not homogeneous and no MRI sequences were performed. Hence, possible effects on human cognitive function caused by synergistic interaction of the static magnetic field, the gradient fields, and RF energy could not be evaluated (4). The authors found no significant differences between pre- and postmagnetic field exposure. Nor were any clinically significant effects in vital parameters found (6). Atkinson et al (15) examined 25 volunteers in a 9.4 T system while performing sodium imaging and in a mock scanner. Before and after the MR examination the subjects had to undergo a neuropsychological test battery that contained some subtests on attention. No effects on cognition were found. This is the only other Cognitive Function and High-Field MRI study to date where MR sequences were performed during the exposure to the static magnetic field. However, the test battery was not strictly focused on attention and the performed sequences are not common in clinical routine, as sodium imaging is not yet an established application. No possible effects of the MR examination were detected. De Vocht et al (29) examined 20 volunteers blinded to the exposure situation in the stray field of a 0 T, 1.5 T, and 3 T system. The volunteers performed a neuropsychological test battery after exposure including tests for the working memory, visual inference, and eye–hand coordination neurological domains. No imaging sequences were performed. Nevertheless, de Vocht et al found some influences on visual and auditory working memory, eye–hand coordination speed, and visual tracking tasks. Kangarlu et al (7) examined 10 subjects before and after resting in a static magnetic field of 8 T for 1 hour. They utilized a Mini-Mental Test and two additional tests to assess language and motor function. This relatively simple test setting revealed no abnormalities. Likewise, no imaging sequences were performed in that study. With the exception of one study, all of these publications have in common that no clinical sequences were performed. Thus, possible synergistic effects of static and gradient magnetic fields and RF fields might remain undetected. In some studies the collective was small and heterogeneous, and the neuropsychological testing was not strictly focused on attention. In our study we hypothesized that if there is an alteration in brain function caused by an MRI examination, a deficit in attention should be detectable. We also strove to detect any possible interactions between the different electromagnetic fields to which a patient is exposed during MRI. For this reason we performed a complete MRI examination and did not expose the subjects only to a static magnetic field as, for example, Chakeres et al (4) chose to do. Any effects detected in our study would of course have to be further investigated to determine whether they are attributable to one of the three fields alone, of if there are indeed synergistic effects. Our study demonstrated that MRI examinations at 1.5 T as well as at 7 T do not seem to have any influence on cognition immediately following exposure. No MR-related effects on attention as the most vulnerable part of cognition were detectable, although our collective was very homogeneous. Only five of the subtests revealed significant differences between pre- and post-MRI exposure, and in each case the difference was related to an improvement in the test performance post-MRI. Thus, the underlying explanation for the significance of these subtests is most likely related to learning effects. Before and after the MRI examinations, exactly the same tests were performed. Thus, learning effects are supported. These learning effects might have been limited by so-called complementary tests, but preliminary examinations showed that these tests led to higher scatter in reaction times, so that these tests were not considered suitable for the number of sub- 1065 jects in this study. Additionally, no test battery with four ‘‘complementary’’ tests exists. Usage of a test battery with two ‘‘complementary’’ tests would not be a sufficient solution. Therefore, we decided to accept a degree of learning and performed identical tests repetitively. The learning effects between different field strength exposures were minimized by randomizing the order of the examinations and by a long time interval between the MR examinations. Additionally, a prestudy training session was held in which the complete test battery was explained and practiced once before the actual measurements were obtained. Another drawback of this study is that neuropsychological testing was only performed before and after the MRI examination and not during MR scanning. Possible transient effects with very fast recovery might therefore be missed. In conclusion, despite the mentioned drawbacks, MRI examinations, even in ultrahigh-field scanners, do not seem to have any persisting influence on the attention networks of human cognition immediately after exposure. REFERENCES 1. Moller HE, von Cramon DY. Bestandsaufnahme zu Risiken durch statische Magnetfelder im Zusammenhang mit der UltrahochfeldMRT. Rofo 2008;180:293–301. 2. Schenck JF. Safety of strong, static magnetic fields. J Magn Reson Imaging 2000;12:2–19. 3. Shellock FG, Crues JV. MR procedures: biologic effects, safety, and patient care. Radiology 2004;232:635–652. 4. Chakeres DW, Bornstein R, Kangarlu A. Randomized comparison of cognitive function in humans at 0 and 8 Tesla. J Magn Reson Imaging 2003;18:342–345. 5. Chakeres DW, de Vocht F. Static magnetic field effects on human subjects related to magnetic resonance imaging systems. Prog Biophys Mol Biol 2005;87:255–265. 6. Chakeres DW, Kangarlu A, Boudoulas H, Young DC. Effect of static magnetic field exposure of up to 8 Tesla on sequential human vital sign measurements. J Magn Reson Imaging 2003; 18:346–352. 7. Kangarlu A, Burgess RE, Zhu H, et al. Cognitive, cardiac, and physiological safety studies in ultra high field magnetic resonance imaging. Magn Reson Imaging 1999;17:1407–1416. 8. Filley CM. The neuroanatomy of attention. Semin Speech Lang 2002;23:89–98. 9. McDowd JM. An overview of attention: behavior and brain. J Neurol Phys Ther 2007;31:98–103. 10. Raz A. Anatomy of attentional networks. Anat Rec B New Anat 2004;281:21–36. 11. Sturm W, Willmes K. On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage 2001;14(1 Pt 2):S76–84. 12. de Vocht F, van-Wendel-de-Joode B, Engels H, Kromhout H. Neurobehavioral effects among subjects exposed to high static and gradient magnetic fields from a 1.5 Tesla magnetic resonance imaging system—a case-crossover pilot study. Magn Reson Med 2003;50:670–674. 13. de Vocht F, van Drooge H, Engels H, Kromhout H. Exposure, health complaints and cognitive performance among employees of an MRI scanners manufacturing department. J Magn Reson Imaging 2006;23:197–204. 14. De Vocht F. Gezondheitsklachten en cognitieve effecten door blootstelling aan magnetische strooivelden van mri-scanners. Tijdschr Diergeneesk 2007;132:46–47. 15. Atkinson IC, Renteria L, Burd H, Pliskin NH, Thulborn KR. Safety of human MRI at static fields above the FDA 8 T guideline: sodium imaging at 9.4 T does not affect vital signs or cognitive ability. J Magn Reson Imaging 2007;26:1222–1227. 16. Atkinson RC, Shiffrin RM. The control of short-term memory. Sci Am 1971;225:82–90. 1066 17. van Zomeren AH, van den Burg W. Residual complaints of patients two years after severe head injury. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1985;48:21–28. 18. Valle-Inclan F, de Labra C, Redondo M. Psychophysiological studies of unattended information processing. Span J Psychol 2000;3:76–85. 19. Theysohn JM, Maderwald S, Kraff O, Moenninghoff C, Ladd ME, Ladd SC. Subjective acceptance of 7 Tesla MRI for human imaging. Magma 2008;21:63–72. 20. Schaefer DJ, Bourland JD, Nyenhuis JA. Review of patient safety in time-varying gradient fields. J Magn Reson Imaging 2000;12:20–29. 21. Bourland JD, Nyenhuis JA, Schaefer DJ. Physiologic effects of intense MR imaging gradient fields. Neuroimaging Clin N Am 1999;9:363–377. 22. Ham CL, Engels JM, van de Wiel GT, Machielsen A. Peripheral nerve stimulation during MRI: effects of high gradient amplitudes and switching rates. J Magn Reson Imaging 1997;7:933–937. 23. Shellock FG, Schaefer DJ, Kanal E. Physiologic responses to an MR imaging procedure performed at a specific absorption rate of 6.0 W/kg. Radiology 1994;192:865–868. Schlamann et al. 24. von Klitzing L. Do static magnetic fields of NMR influence biological signals? Clin Phys Physiol Meas 1986;7:157–160. 25. Hong CZ, Shellock FG. Short-term exposure to a 1.5 Tesla static magnetic field does not affect somato-sensory-evoked potentials in man. Magn Reson Imaging 1990;8:65–69. 26. Muller S, Hotz M. Human brainstem auditory evoked potentials (BAEP) before and after MR examinations. Magn Reson Med 1990;16:476–480. 27. Pacini S, Vannelli GB, Barni T, et al. Effect of 0.2 T static magnetic field on human neurons: remodeling and inhibition of signal transduction without genome instability. Neurosci Lett 1999;267: 185–188. 28. Feychting M. Health effects of static magnetic fields—a review of the epidemiological evidence. Prog Biophys Mol Biol 2005;87: 241–246. 29. de Vocht F, Stevens T, van Wendel-de-Joode B, Engels H, Kromhout H. Acute neurobehavioral effects of exposure to static magnetic fields: analyses of exposure-response relations. J Magn Reson Imaging 2006;23:291–297.