Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Clinical Scholarship An International Perspective on the Time to Treatment for Acute Myocardial Infarction Kathleen Dracup, Debra K. Moser, Sharon McKinley, Carol Ball, Keiko Yamasaki, Cho-Ja Kim, Lynn V. Doering, Mary A. Caldwell Purpose: To compare delay and circumstances of decisions to seek care in patients with acute myocardial infarction (AMI) in the United States (US), England, Australia, South Korea, and Japan. Design: Comparative prospective design. Methods: Patients diagnosed with AMI (N=913) were interviewed within 72 hours of hospital admission for confirmed AMI using the Response to Symptoms Questionnaire. Delay times were calculated from review of emergency room records and patients’ interviews. Analysis of variance was used to test differences in delay time among countries. Findings: Median delay ranged from 2.5 hours in England to 6.4 hours in Australia, with the three Pacific Rim countries reporting median delay times > 4 hours. The majority of patients experienced initial symptoms at home (range: 56% in Japan to 73% in the US) with the most common witness being a family member (32% in South Korea to 48% in England). Ambulance use was widely divergent with the highest use in England (85%) and the lowest use in the US (42%). Conclusions: In all countries, median delay was too long to obtain maximum benefit from AMI therapies, particularly thrombolysis. Education and counseling of patients and families to reduce prehospital delay in AMI episodes might be more effective if the various factors influencing patients’ first responses to symptoms are considered, as well as differences in health care systems. JOURNAL OF NURSING SCHOLARSHIP, 2003; 35:4, 317-323. ©2003 SIGMA THETA TAU INTERNATIONAL. [Key words: myocardial infarction, delay time, US, England, Pacific Rim] * P rompt reperfusion therapy for an acute myocardial infarction (AMI) can limit myocardial damage with subsequent positive effect on morbidity and mortality (Berger, Schulman, et al., 1999). However, the efficacy of therapy is directly related to the interval between symptom onset and treatment (Berger, Ellis, et al., 1999; Goldberg et al., 1998). Patients who receive treatment within the 1st hour of the onset of symptoms receive the most benefit, and many patients are excluded from reperfusion therapy because of excessive delay. Mean delay times of 4.6 to 24 hours and median delay times of 2 to 6.4 hours have been reported in studies conducted in North America and Europe in the past 2 decades (Dracup, McKinley, & Moser, 1997; GISSI, 1995; Luepker et al., 2000; Newby et al., 1996). This study was conducted to document and compare delay times and associated factors in five countries. * * Kathleen Dracup, RN, DNSc, Gamma Tau, Dean and Professor, School of Nursing, University of California, San Francisco; Debra K. Moser , RN, DNSc, Delta Psi , Professor, School of Nursing, University of Kentucky, Lexington; Sharon McKinley, RN, PhD, Professor of Critical Care Nursing, University of Technology, Sydney, Australia; Carol Ball , RN, PhD, St. Bartholomew School of Nursing and Midwifery, City University, London, England; Keiko Yamasaki , RN, MSN, Tokyo Women’s Medical College Hospital, Tokyo, Japan; Cho-Ja Kim, RN, PhD, Dean, College of Nursing, Yonsei University, Seoul, Korea; Lynn V. Doering , RN, DNSc, Associate Professor, School of Nursing, University of California, Los Angeles; Mary A. Caldwell, RN, MBA, PhD, Alpha Eta, Assistant Adjunct Professor, School of Nursing, University of California, San Francisco. Supported in part from grants to Dr. Dracup from University of California Pacific Rim Research Program and Fulbright Foundation. Correspondence to Dr. Dracup, School of Nursing, University of California, 2 Koret Ave., Rm N319, Box 0604, San Francisco, CA 94143-0604. E-mail: [email protected] Accepted for publication January 9, 2003. Journal of Nursing Scholarship Fourth Quarter 2003 317 Time to Treatment for AMI Background Recently, the National Institutes of Health launched a campaign to reduce prehospital delay in the US, targeting both health care professionals and the lay public (Dracup et al., 1997; Lenfant, LaRosa, Horan, & Passamani, 1990). Several randomized trials have been conducted to test public education strategies (Luepker et al., 2000; Meischke, Eisenberg, Schaeffer, Larsen, & Henwood, 1994; Moses et al., 1991). All have been conducted in Western industrialized societies and all have been focused on providing information about AMI symptoms and the appropriate steps to take to reduce treatment delay. Few studies to date have included the social factors that contribute to delay and none have indicated the difference in treatment-seeking behavior in AMI between and among various countries. Health care systems and cultural attitudes toward health and illness vary from country to country, and these differences might affect the ability of patients with AMI to label and respond to cardiac symptoms in a timely manner, thereby increasing or decreasing prehospital delay times. For example, a health care system in which patients are encouraged to seek emergency care in facilities without cardiodiagnostic capabilities as is the case in many community clinics in Japan (Matsuda, 2000) can lead to significant delay in patients’ diagnosis and treatment. Moreover, fundamental differences exist in how patients from different cultures respond to illness and interact with health care providers. For example, people from Asian cultures who experience cardiac symptoms might turn first to traditional herbal medicines before calling a physician or seeking care at an emergency facility (Chan et al., 2003). Leventhal’s model of self-regulatory conception of illness (Leventhal & Cameron, 1987; Leventhal, Nerenz, & Steele, 1984) behavior is often used to characterize delay in care-seeking behaviors and it has served as a foundation for interventions in Western cultures (Dracup, 1997; Dracup & Moser, 1997; Leventhal, Safer, & Panagis, 1983). According to this model, beliefs about health are hierarchical—based on previous experiences with illness and other information provided in the social environment. Internal and environmental factors such as sociodemographic and clinical characteristics and cultural roles and expectations affect how one responds to a health threat. Environmental stimuli such as family and coworkers can also affect care-seeking behaviors, along with friends, health care providers, and the media. As illustrated in the Figure, a person experiencing a health problem goes through three stages: (a) a mental representation of the health threat (cognitive and emotional), (b) development of an action plan for coping with the perceived threat, and (c) an appraisal that includes an assessment of how well the plan addresses the threat. A feedback mechanism underlies the mental representation and the coping strategies during subsequent illness events. The inclusion of internal and environmental stimuli makes the model particularly appropriate for evaluating the influence of culture on careseeking behavior. With this model, one assumes that at least 318 Fourth Quarter 2003 Journal of Nursing Scholarship some of the variation in human responses is attributable to cultural influences (Dreher & MacNaughton, 2002). World Health Organization (WHO) officials have projected that noncommunicable diseases, particularly heart disease, will increase dramatically in all developing countries over the next few decades (WHO, 1996). By 2020, ischemic heart disease is expected to become the leading cause of disease burden worldwide, overtaking communicable diseases, poor reproductive health, and malnutrition as the leading cause of death (Reddy & Yusuf, 1998). Ischemic heart disease in Asia as a significant cause of mortality is well documented (Thailand Annual Health Statistics, 1994; Toshima, 1994). Therefore, a determination of prehospital delay patterns for treatment of AMI in both Western and Pacific Rim countries is important in preparing future global public health initiatives and in planning health initiatives in countries with ethnically and culturally diverse populations. We conducted this study to document current rates of prehospital delay and to compare delay times in two Western countries (the US and England) and three Pacific Rim countries (Australia, Japan, and South Korea). We also described patients’ and others’ initial responses to symptoms and examined the relationship between patients’ sociodemographic and clinical characteristics in predicting time to treatment of AMI. Internal & environmental stimuli Cognitive representation Coping procedures AMI symptoms Outcome appraisals Emotional representation Seek care Figure.Theoretical framework adapted from Leventhal’s self-regulatory model of illness behaior (Leventhal & Cameron, 1987). Methods Sample and Setting Participants (N=913) were recruited from 10 community hospitals and university medical centers in five countries: Australia, Japan, Korea, England, and the US. Patients were eligible for inclusion if they were diagnosed as having an AMI via cardiac enzyme or standard electrocardiographic criteria, had been hospitalized for 72 hours or less, were pain free and hemodynamically stable, alert, and oriented; living independently (not in a chronic care facility); and free from complicating malignancy or other debilitating illness. Procedures Institutional review boards or their equivalent at the participating sites approved the study protocol. Following approval of the attending Time to Treatment for AMI physician, a study investigator or research assistant approached patients meeting the study criteria. The study was explained and written consent to participate was obtained. Participants were interviewed as soon as possible following hospitalization to optimize their ability to recount events immediately before hospitalization. Mean time between hospital admission and time _38 hours (median 44 hours). All interviews of interview was 53+ were conducted in patients’ primary language by a nativespeaking researcher. Clinical data were obtained via patients’ interviews and from medical records. The time from symptom onset to hospital admission was established in two ways. Medical records were reviewed to obtain the emergency department physicians’ identification of time of symptom onset and the time of patient admission. Each participant was also interviewed and asked to identify the time that he or she experienced the symptoms that brought them to the hospital. In the case of symptoms that came and went (i.e., intermittent symptoms), participants were thoroughly interviewed by one of the researchers to determine the nature of the symptoms and the exact time that symptoms of a cardiac nature began. Careful interviewing techniques designed to assist patients to remember accurately the time of symptom onset were used (Dempsey, Dracup, & Moser, 1995; Dracup, McKinley et al., 1997; Dracup & Moser, 1997; Reilly, Dracup, & Dattolo, 1994). When a discrepancy occurred between the time to hospitalization recorded in the medical record and the patient’s account, the patient’s cardiologist was consulted along with the electrocardiographic and serum cardiac enzyme data obtained upon admission to determine which of the two values to use (Rawles, Metcalfe, Shirreffs, Jennings, & Kenmure, 1990). Instruments Patients were interviewed using the Response to Symptoms Questionnaire (Burnett, Blumenthal, Mark, Leimberger, & Califf, 1995). This questionnaire was designed to obtain information about the events that occurred before hospital admission, including the patient’s cognitive and psychological responses to symptoms and the responses of people who were with the patient. Both the clinical data form used for medical chart review and the Response to Symptoms Questionnaire were translated from English into Japanese and Korean and then back translated by a second person to ensure equivalency. Previous investigations have shown adequate reliability and validity of this instrument (Dracup & Moser, 1997). Statistical Analyses Proportions and measures of central tendency were used to indicate sample characteristics, delay time, initial symptom experiences, and first response to symptoms. Because delay time was skewed by a number of extreme observations, a logarithmic transformation of delay time was used in all analyses. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to test the overall differences in delay time between countries. Two-factor ANOVAs were used to test for differences in delay time within various subgroups among the countries. The subgroups included sociodemographic variables (sex, age, years of formal education, residence—rural, urban, or suburban—yearly household income converted to US dollars, and marital status) and clinical characteristics (maximum pain level experienced during AMI, admission systolic blood pressure, admission pulse, smoking status, and history of hypertension, diabetes mellitus, or previous AMI). Findings Characteristics of the Sample A total of 913 patients with AMI were enrolled in this study: 192 from the US, 127 from South Korea, 136 from Japan, 141 from England, and 317 from Australia. Sample characteristics by country are shown in Table 1. Mean ages for the five _11 to 63+_12 and years of education countries ranged from 57+ ranged from 10+_4 to 13+_3. Mean systolic blood pressure ranged from a low of 128+_30 in South Korea to a high of Table 1. Sample Characteristics (N=913) Age (M±SD) Education (M±SD) Male (%) Current smoker (%) Residence (%) Rural Urban Suburban Married (%) History of: (%) Hypertension Diabetes AMI Coronary bypass Angioplasty United States (n=192) 61±13 12.8±3.0 50.0 30.9 South Korea (n=127) 57±11 11.3±4.5 77.2 67.7 Japan (n=136) 61±11 12.8±3.1 80.1 68.4 England (n=141) 61±13 10.1±4.3 77.3 44.0 2.9 92.8 4.3 75.7 Australia (n=317) 63±12 9.9±4.4 65.9 na na na na na 69.4 p .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 na na na .000 46.3 26.9 26.9 67.0 13.4 81.5 5.0 92.1 5.9 80.9 12.5 86.0 57.1 27.0 30.7 10.1 21.7 48.8 24.6 7.9 0.8 3.9 54.8 34.8 15.4 2.9 8.1 41.8 26.2 20.6 6.4 5.0 53.4 18.7 28.3 12.1 na .061 .007 .000 .000 .000 Note. na = not available. Journal of Nursing Scholarship Fourth Quarter 2003 319 Time to Treatment for AMI 143+_29 in the US and England. The majority of patients lived in urban or suburban areas with easy access to hospitals and were living with a spouse. On average, 23% of all patients had experienced a previous AMI (range 7.8% to 30.6% in the five countries). Delay Times The mean delay time for the entire group was 17.3±45 hours, and the median was 4 hours. The mean delay times were markedly skewed by the few participants who delayed many hours and even days from the time of symptom onset. Thus, median values reflect delay times more accurately than do mean values. Median delay time in each country is shown in Table 2. Using ANOVA and log-transformed delay times, the overall difference for the countries was significant at p=.001. Posthoc comparisons with Bonferroni adjustment indicated a significant difference between England and Japan (p=.04), and between Australia and England (p=.001). All other comparisons were statistically nonsignificant. In all countries studied, the median delay times were too long for patients to receive maximal advantage of optimal AMI therapies according to current clinical guidelines. Table 2. Comparison Among Countries of Time from Symptom Onset to Hospital Admission (in Hours) Mean±SD) Median 25th percentile 75th percentile United States (n=192) 22.5±69.3 3.3 1.1 15.2 South Korea (n=127) 14.7±29.8 4.4 1.8 13.3 Japan (n=136) 21.7±63.6 4.5 2.0 16.3 England (n=141) 8.0±14.0 2.5 1.5 8.7 Australia (n=317) 17.4±26.4 6.4 1.7 6.4 Note. Overall difference: p=.001. Posthoc Bonferroni: p=.04 for England and Japan; p=.001 for Australia and England, p>.05 for all others. Table 3. Initial Symptom Experience and Response United South States Korea Japan (n=192) (n=127) (n=136) Patient location when symptoms began (%) Home 72.9 59.2 55.7 Work 7.4 10.7 12.9 In vehicle 5.3 3.9 5.7 Others’ home 2.1 1.9 1.4 Public place 9.6 11.7 10.7 None of these 2.7 12.6 13.6 Witness (%) Alone 29.8 17.5 Spouse or partner 43.1 32.0 Other family 14.4 24.3 Friend 3.2 7.8 Coworker 7.4 11.7 Other 2.1 6.8 Mode of transportation to hospital (%) Private car 56.4 32.4 Ambulance 41.5 51.0 Public transportation — 15.7 Other 2.1 1.0 320 Fourth Quarter 2003 England (n=141) Australia (n=317) 70.9 14.2 2.8 1.4 9.9 0.7 68.5 12.3 2.8 3.2 5.4 7.9 32.9 33..6 15.0 8.6 7.1 2.9 29.8 48.2 11.3 2.8 7.8 — 38.8 38.5 9.1 5.4 6.0 2.2 13.6 67.1 10.0 9.3 7.8 85.1 2.8 4.3 35.4 61.4 0.9 2.2 Journal of Nursing Scholarship Initial Symptom Experience and Responses to Symptoms The most common location where patients experienced initial AMI symptoms was the home (56% in Japan to 73% in the US). The most frequent witness at the time of symptom onset was the spouse (32% in South Korea to 48% in England). The majority of patients in each country except the US were transported to the hospital by ambulance. In the US, most participants (58%) used private transportation to get to the hospital. See Table 3 for comparisons. Table 4. Initial Symptom Experience and Response United South States Korea (n=192) (n=127) First response of patient (%) Wished or prayed symptoms would go away 14.9 25.5 Tried to relax 21.3 7.8 Pretended nothing was wrong 7.4 — Tried not to think about it 4.3 — Took medication 17.6 15.7 Called physician 1.6 1.0 Tried self-help remedy 12.2 20.6 Told someone nearby 12.8 7.9 3.7 5.9 Called EMSa Transported to the hospital without EMS 3.2 15.7 Drove to physician’s office or clinic 1.1 — Other action — — First response of person consulted (%) Did nothing 4.4 2.9 Encouraged not to worry 4.9 — Comforted patient 7.7 5.8 Suggested rest or medications 9.3 17.5 Suggested medical help 27.9 12.6 Called EMS 19.7 16.5 Took patient to hospital 17.5 24.3 Became upset 1.5 1.0 No one else knew 7.1 12.6 Other type of response — 6.8 Identification of origin of symptoms (%) Heart 35.1 42.9 Stomach 32.4 38.8 Muscle pain 12.2 5.1 Fatigue 3.2 4.1 Flu 8.0 — Respiratory 2.7 6.1 Other 5.9 3.1 aEmergency Japan (n=136) England (n=141) Australia (n=317) 15.8 16.5 10.7 15.0 9.2 20.6 0.7 3.6 12.4 0.7 17.3 2.2 23.7 10.1 4.3 2.9 30.7 5.0 3.6 13.5 7.9 4.4 16.8 1.6 2.2 22.5 1.6 3.6 6.4 — 2.9 2.2 0.7 — 0.3 8.3 13.6 0.7 14.0 3.6 5.0 5.0 2.1 3.7 5.9 6.4 25.0 12.9 7.1 0.7 23.6 2.1 9.9 18.4 29.8 7.8 — 26.2 — 4.4 25.0 14.0 4.4 0.7 25.7 2.2 50.0 16.7 9.4 12.3 1.4 8.0 2.2 37.9 45.7 7.9 0.7 2.1 5.7 — 40.3 24.3 12.8 7.7 1.0 — 14.1 Medical Services In all countries, most participants initially responded inappropriately to their symptoms. Table 4 shows the first responses in the five countries. For example, in Japan the most common first response was to try a self-help remedy, with only 4.3% calling an emergency medical system (EMS). The highest percentage of patients calling an EMS as a first Time to Treatment for AMI response was in England (8%) and in South Korea (6%). The lowest was in Australia at 1.6%. When participants told someone else about their symptoms, only a minority of witnesses called an EMS (13% in Japan to 30% in England). Table 4 shows the first responses of witnesses. Participants were asked to describe their initial perceptions of the source of their symptoms. Most patients thought their symptoms originated with either the heart or stomach and generally less than 10% in each country indicated other causes. Bivariate Analyses of Delay Times Within Subgroups by Country Delay times were compared within the countries based on sociodemographic, clinical, and risk-factor variables. No variables were consistently associated with delay time among countries. Examination of sociodemographic and clinical factors predictive of delay with multivariate analyses failed to reveal a clinically relevant profile of patients likely to delay or not delay in any of the countries. Discussion The current study is the first to compare delay time in seeking care for symptoms of AMI and to describe the initial symptom experience and first responses to symptoms in an international cohort. Although the five countries were located on four different continents, and therefore have a broad range of traditions and cultures, all five are industrialized and have medical communities that share common treatment philosophies. The standard of care in all five is early revascularization in evolving AMI with thrombolysis or angioplasty with or without stent. Thus, the importance of symptom identification by the patient and prompt presentation to the hospital (ideally 1 hour or less) is shared by all. In all five countries time to treatment was substantially longer than recommended. This finding was surprising given the recent intensive campaigns conducted by clinicians, pharmaceutical companies, and public health experts to reduce delay time both nationally and internationally. It was not substantially different than the times reported over a decade ago (Kereiakes et al., 1990; Wielgosz, & Nolan, 1991). The lack of relationship between knowledge gained in public educational campaigns and behavioral change might be counterintuitive for clinicians, but it is interpretable from public health studies (Luepker et al., 2000; Meischke et al., 1994; Moses et al., 1991). Knowledge does not necessarily lead to behavior change. For example, in the one country where the majority of participants reported knowing about early treatment options (i.e., the US), the median delay time was still over 3 hours (Dracup, McKinley et al., 1997). Our data, combined with those of other investigators (Luepker et al., 2000; Meischke et al., 1994; Moses et al., 1991), indicate that community education programs alone are not sufficient to reduce delay. Additional country or culture-specific issues might require study. For example, Japanese patients might preferentially call or visit their physicians when experiencing symptoms of AMI rather than calling emergency services or going to the hospital. This choice contributes substantially to delay time and would need to be addressed in interventions related to delay time. In the majority of cases, the first response to symptoms was inappropriate because participants did not activate an EMS. Delay time has been shown to be significantly lower when patients activate an EMS and come to the hospital via ambulance compared to patients who do not call an EMS (Kereiakes, Gibler, Martin, Pieper, & Anderson, 1992; Meischke, 1995). Barriers to calling an EMS have been shown to be substantial in the US (Dracup & Moser, 1997) and Australia (Dracup, McKinley, Moser, 1997). Responses might be related to cultural influences. For example, in this study, Korean participants chose first to pray that the symptoms would go away. Koreans often view illness as a result of bad luck or misfortune and they remain stoic and feel unable to change the course of the disease (Lipson, Dibble, & Minarik, 1996). This response could result in delays in seeking care. Japanese participants were most likely to administer a selfhelp remedy or take medications, rather than calling an EMS. Researchers have noted that older Japanese (a majority of the population with AMI) may not respond to illness until it is advanced and they often turn to home remedies when ill (Matsumoto et al., 1995). The structure of the health care system might also influence a person’s decision to call an EMS. For example, all Australian patients have nationalized health care without any monetary fee for care, but the EMS system is private and financial cost might be a barrier to its use. In contrast, in England the EMS is part of nationalized health care. The findings from the current study indicate that first responses might vary by culture or country, but the study should be replicated with attention to how the cultural milieu and health care system affects response time. In all five countries, the majority of participants experienced symptoms at home and were with their spouses. This finding, which has not been documented before in Pacific Rim countries but has been reported in Western countries (Alonzo, 1986; Dracup, McKinley, et al., 1997), shows the important potential role of spouses in helping patients interpret symptoms. In this sample, participants’ first responses to symptoms were inappropriate strategies that increased delay time. Less than 8% of patients in all countries reported calling the EMS as a first response, the action deemed most appropriate by the National Heart Attack Alert Program (Dracup, Alonzo, et al., 1997; Lenfant et al., 1990). Spouses and other lay people correctly recommended calling the EMS or going to the hospital as the most common initial response, further indicating the important role of others in helping patients choose the best course of action when faced with AMI symptoms among all countries and cultures. However, examining interrelationships between spouses is important. Spouses in Western cultures such as the US, England, and Australia have nearly equal standing in their relationships, but they often do not in other cultures. Korean and Japanese Journal of Nursing Scholarship Fourth Quarter 2003 321 Time to Treatment for AMI cultures tend to be strongly patriarchal (Lipson et al., 1996; Matsumoto, 2000) which could affect decision making about appropriate responses to AMI symptoms. Age also conveys strong authority in the Japanese and Korean cultures, and younger family members might be reluctant to urge older family members to call an EMS (Matsumoto, 2000). Previous researchers have found that delay is reduced when patients immediately recognize their symptoms as cardiac in origin (Ruston, Clayton, & Calnan, 1998). Unfortunately, less than 50% of patients in any country first identified their symptoms as cardiac in origin. Surprisingly, patients in Japan and South Korea most often labeled symptoms as cardiac, despite the fact that coronary artery disease is a relatively new phenomenon in Asian countries and participants in these two countries had the lowest incidence of previous AMI compared to those in the other three countries. The most common misinterpretation in all countries was that the symptoms were gastrointestinal in origin. This finding indicated that education based simply on providing patients with knowledge of typical AMI symptoms can be ineffective if health care providers do not also help patients appropriately interpret symptoms. Much of the previous research on factors that are associated with patients’ treatment-seeking delay has been focused on identification of sociodemographic and clinical variables that could increase delay (Ghali, Cooper, Kowatly, & Liao, 1993; Meischke, Eisenberg, & Larsen, 1993; Newby et al., 1996). Such research has shown that previous experience with AMI symptoms does not result in decreased delay times (Ghali et al., 1993; GISSI, 1995), but findings have been inconsistent. For example, some investigators have reported an inverse relationship between pain intensity and delay time (GISSI, 1995) but others have found no such association (Bleeker et al., 1995; Hofgren et al., 1988). In some studies, women have been found to delay longer than do men (Meischke et al., 1993; Newby et al., 1996), but others showed no sex difference in delay times (Dracup & Moser, 1997). The current study did not indicate consistent factors associated with delay among the countries studied. No differences in patients’ delay times in any country were based on sex, marital status, residence, admission blood pressure, pain intensity, or history of previous AMI. The lack of association indicates the importance of looking beyond sociodemographic and clinical variables to include cognitive, emotional, and cultural factors to explain delay. Leventhal’s model can be useful by indicating sociodemographic, family, social, cognitive, and emotional factors that contribute to mental representations of illness and subsequent responses to that illness. This study showed some differences in delay and patients’ responses to symptoms of AMI, but it had significant limitations. The sample was nonrandom and it included primarily people living in urban areas. Other countries, and different cultures within countries, could show different results. Additionally, the nature of the health care system in each country and its influence on delay time was not investigated. This study was descriptive, and causal relationships between population characteristics, health care system characteristics, 322 Fourth Quarter 2003 Journal of Nursing Scholarship and delay cannot be drawn. The findings cannot be generalized, including to other patients in these five countries. Conclusions This study adds to the growing literature on the subject of patients’ response to AMI symptoms and is among the first to report data about patients’ responses to symptoms of AMI in Pacific Rim countries. Delayed treatment was a substantial problem in all of the countries studied. The majority of patients in all countries did not accurately identify the symptoms as being cardiac and did not call the EMS. In all countries studied, the majority of first responses were inappropriate; however, family members more often than patients selected an appropriate course of action, indicating the importance of involving family members in all patient education efforts. Future education and counseling programs might be more effective if social and cultural factors influencing patients’ first responses to symptoms are considered. References Alonzo, A.A. (1986). The impact of the family and lay others on careseeking during life-threatening episodes of suspected coronary artery disease. Social Science and Medicine, 22(12), 1297-1311. Berger, A.K., Schulman, K.A., Gersh, B.J., Pirzada, S., Breall, J.A., Johnson, A.E., et al. (1999). Primary coronary angioplasty vs thrombolysis for the management of acute myocardial infarction in elderly patients. JAMA, 282(4), 341-348. Berger, P.B., Ellis, S.B., Holmes, D.R., Granger, C.B., Criger, D.A., Betriu, A., et al. (1999). Relationship between delay in performing direct coronary angioplasty and early clinical outcome in patients with acute myocardial infarction. Circulation, 100, 14-20. Bleeker, J.K., Simoons, M.L., Erdman, R.A., Leenders, C.M., Kruyssen, H.A., Lamers, L.M., et al. (1995). Patient and doctor delay in acute myocardial infarction: a study in Rotterdam, The Netherlands. British Journal of General Practice, 45(393), 181-184. Burnett, R.E., Blumenthal, J.A., Mark, D.B., Leimberger, J.D., & Califf, R.M. (1995). Distinguishing between early and late responders to symptoms of acute myocardial infarction. American Journal of Cardiology, 75(15), 1019-1022. Chan, M.F., Mok, E., Wong, Y.S., Tong, T.F., Day, M.C., Tang, C.K., et al. (2003) Attitudes of Hong Kong Chinese to traditional Chinese medicine and Western medicine: survey and cluster analysis. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 11(2),103-9. Dempsey, S.J., Dracup, K., & Moser, D.K. (1995). Women’s decision to seek care for symptoms of acute myocardial infarction. Heart and Lung, 24(6), 444-456. Dracup, K. (1997). Educational strategies to prevent prehospital delay in patients at high risk for acute myocardial infarction (No. 97-3787). Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health. Dracup, K., Alonzo, A.A., Atkins, J.M., Bennett, N.M., Braslow, A., Clark, L. T., et al. (1997). The physician’s role in minimizing prehospital delay in patients at high risk for acute myocardial infarction: Recommendations from the National Heart Attack Alert Program. Working group on educational strategies to prevent prehospital delay in patients at high risk for acute myocardial infarction. Annals of Internal Medicine, 126(8), 645-651. Dracup, K., McKinley, S.M., & Moser, D.K. (1997). Australian patients’ delay in response to heart attack symptoms [see comments]. Medical Journal of Australia, 166(5), 233-236. Dracup, K., & Moser, D.K. (1997). Beyond sociodemographics: factors influencing the decision to seek treatment for symptoms of acute myocardial infarction. Heart and Lung, 26(4), 253-262. Time to Treatment for AMI Dreher, M., & MacNaughton, N. (2002). Cultural competency in nursing: Foundation or fallacy? Nursing Outlook, 50, 181-186. Ghali, J.K., Cooper, R.S., Kowatly, I., & Liao, Y. (1993). Delay between onset of chest pain and arrival to the coronary care unit among minority and disadvantaged patients. Journal of the National Medical Association, 85(3), 180-184. GISSI, A.D.G. (1995). Epidemiology of avoidable delay in the care of patients with acute myocardial infarction in Italy. A GISSI-generated study. GISSI—Avoidable delay study group. Archives of Internal Medicine, 155(14), 1481-1488. Goldberg, R.J., Mooradd, M., Gurwitz, J.H., Rogers, W.J., French, W.J., Barron, H.V., et al. (1998). Impact of time to treatment with tissue plasminogen activator on morbidity and mortality following acute myocardial infarction (The Second National Registry of Myocardial Infarction). American Journal of Cardiology, 82(3), 259-264. Hofgren, K., Bondestam, E., Johansson, F.G., Jern, S., Herlitz, J., & Holmberg, S. (1988). Initial pain course and delay to hospital admission in relation to myocardial infarct size. Heart and Lung, 17(3), 274-280. Kereiakes, D.J., Gibler, W.B., Martin, L.H., Pieper, K.S., & Anderson, L.C. (1992). Relative importance of emergency medical system transport and the prehospital electrocardiogram on reducing hospital time delay to therapy for acute myocardial infarction: A preliminary report from the Cincinnati Heart Project. American Heart Journal, 123(4 Pt 1), 835-840. Kereiakes, D.J., Weaver, W.D., Anderson, J.L., Feldman, T., Gibler, B., Aufderheide, T., et al. (1990). Time delays in the diagnosis and treatment of acute myocardial infarction: A tale of eight cities. Report from the Pre-hospital Study Group and the Cincinnati Heart Project. American Heart Journal, 120(4), 773-780. Lenfant, C., LaRosa, J.H., Horan, M.J., & Passamani, E.R. (1990). Considerations for a national heart attack alert program. Clinical Cardiology, 13(8 Suppl. 8), VIII9-VIII11. Leventhal, H., & Cameron, L. (1987). Behavioral theories and the problem of compliance. Patient Education and Counseling, 10, 117-138. Leventhal, H., Nerenz, D.R., & Steele, D.J. (1984). Illness representations and coping with health threats. In A. Baum, S.E. Taylor, & J.E. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of psychology and health (Vol. IV, pp. 219-252). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Leventhal, H., Safer, M.A., & Panagis, D.M. (1983). The impact of communications on the self-regulation of health beliefs, decisions, and behavior. Health Education Quarterly, 10(1), 3-29. Lipson, J., Dibble, S., & Minarik, P. (Eds.). (1996). Culture and nursing care. San Francisco: UCSF Nursing Press. Luepker, R.V., Raczynski, J.M., Osganian, S., Goldberg, R.J., Finnegan, J.R., Jr., Hedges, J.R., et al. (2000). Effect of a community intervention on patient delay and emergency medical service use in acute coronary heart disease: The Rapid Early Action for Coronary Treatment (REACT) trial. JAMA, 284(1), 60-67. Matsuda, S. (2000). The health and social system for the aged in Japan. Aging Clinical Experimental Research, 14(4), 265-270. Matsumoto, D., Pun, K.K., Nakatani, M., Kadowaki, D., Weissman, M., McCarter, L., et al. (1995). Cultural differences in attitudes, values, and beliefs about osteoporosis in first and second generation JapaneseAmerican women. Women and Health, 23, 39-56. Matsumoto, D. (2000). Culture and psychology: People around the world. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Meischke, H., Eisenberg, M.S., & Larsen, M.P. (1993). Prehospital delay interval for patients who use emergency medical services: The effect of heart-related medical conditions and demographic variables. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 22(10), 1597-1601. Meischke, H., Eisenberg, M.S., Schaeffer, S.M., Damon, S.K., Larsen, M.P., & Henwood, D.K. (1995). Utilization of emergency medical services for symptoms of acute myocardial infarction. Heart and Lung, 24(1), 11-18. Meischke, H., Eisenberg, M.S., Schaeffer, S.M., Larsen, M.P., & Henwood, D.K. (1994). Impact of direct mail intervention on knowledge, attitudes, and behavioral intentions regarding use of emergency medical services for symptoms of acute myocardial infarction. Evaluation of Health Professions, 17(4), 402-417. Moses, H.W., Bartolozzi, J.J., Jr., Koester, D.L., Colliver, J.A., Taylor, G.J., Mikell, F.L., et al. (1991). Reducing delay in the emergency room in administration of thrombolytic therapy for myocardial infarction associated with ST elevation. American Journal of Cardiology, 68(2), 251-253. Newby, L.K., Rutsch, W.R., Califf, R.M., Simoons, M.L., Aylward, P.E., Armstrong, P.W., et al. (1996). Time from symptom onset to treatment and outcomes after thrombolytic therapy. GUSTO-1 Investigators. Journal of the American College of Cardiology, 27(7), 1646-1655. Rawles, J.M., Metcalfe, M.J., Shirreffs, C., Jennings, K., & Kenmure, A.C. (1990). Association of patient delay with symptoms, cardiac enzymes, and outcome in acute myocardial infarction. European Heart Journal, 11(7), 643-648. Reddy, K.S., & Yusuf, S. (1998). Emerging epidemic of cardiovascular disease in developing countries. Circulation, 97(6), 596-601. Reilly, A., Dracup, K., & Dattolo, J. (1994). Factors influencing prehospital delay in patients experiencing chest pain. American Journal of Critical Care, 3(4), 300-306. Ruston, A., Clayton, J., & Calnan, M. (1998). Patients’ action during their cardiac event: qualitative study exploring differences and modifiable factors [see comments]. British Medical Journal, 316(7137), 1060-1064. Thailand Annual Health Statistics. (1994). Bangkok, Thailand: Department of Public Health Statistics; Ministry of Public Health. Toshima, H. (1994). Coronary artery disease trends in Japan. Japanese Circulation Journal, 58(3), 166-172. World Health Organization. (1996). Investing in health research and development. Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Wielgosz, A.T., & Nolan, R.P. (1991). Understanding delay in response to symptoms of acute myocardial infarction. A compelling agenda. Circulation, 84(5), 2193-2195. Journal of Nursing Scholarship Fourth Quarter 2003 323