Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
STUDENT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL VOLUME I GENE-ENVIRONMENT INTERACTIONS IN DETERMINING CRIMINALITY Laura Mangan Senior Sophister, Psychology [email protected] ABSTRACT The determination of criminality is multifactorial as it involves the interaction of several different factors including individual, familial and social factors. Correlations between antisocial behaviour and impairment of the prefrontal cortices have been found (Eastman & Campbell, 2006). Socioeconomic deprivation has a strong link with subsequent criminality (Ferguson, Swain-Campbell & Horwood, 2004), as those from low SES backgrounds are three times more likely to engage in antisocial behaviour than individuals from high SES backgrounds. In contrast, both the Social Causation Perspective and Control Theories place the burden of criminal determination firmly on peer relations. Familial factors also have an important role in shaping whether an individual engages in criminal activity as they have the potential to exacerbate or moderate criminality in the developing child (Ferguson et al., 2004). In sum, neither genetic nor environmental factors alone are sufficient in explaining the determination of criminality. They must be combined in order to establish any causal relationship. INTRODUCTION Among the most important factors in predicting criminal convictions are socioeconomic deprivation, poor parenting and an antisocial family (Farrington, 1990). These factors are necessary but not sufficient alone; the emergence of the antisocial behavioural phenotype necessarily must occur as a result of interactions between an individual‟s genetic make-up and their environment (Beaver & Holtfreter, 2009). Genetic and environmental factors work separately and in tandem to create criminal behaviour phenotypes (Moffitt, 2005; Rutter, 2006), for example, if a child 123 REVIEW has both conduct disorder and hyperactivity then he is more likely to engage in antisocial behaviours (Farrington, 1990). Antisocial conduct itself has a moderate to strong association with genetic factors (Rhee & Waldmen, 2002). The importance of delinquent peer association is as such that it is not only a predictor of antisocial conduct but it is also linked with fraudulent behaviours (Beaver & Holtfreter, 2009). Children who suffer abuse or neglect are more likely to have Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) as adults and are more likely to be arrested in adolescence (Wisdom, 1989). From these examples it is clear that criminality should be approached from a multifactorial perspective. The current essay endeavours to discuss the determinants of criminality from an individual, familial and societal level. Firstly, the individual‟s influence over his/her own criminality will be discussed. Personality disorder and genetic factors mediate an individual‟s entry into criminality. Secondly, familial influence is an important determinant of criminality. Factors such as maternal deprivation, parenting and attachment relationship are prominent determinants in the literature. Finally, socioeconomic status (SES) and peer influence are important societal determinants that contribute to the individual‟s level of criminal involvement. ADOLESCENT AND ADULT OFFENDING: BIOSOCIAL MODELS According to Moffitt‟s developmental taxonomy (MDT) there are two types of offenders; life-course persistent (LCP) and adolescent-limited offenders (AL) (Moffitt, 1993). Delinquent involvement is quite common amongst adolescents and it is considered to be age normative (Farrington, 1986). Most adolescents engage in delinquency and only a very small percentage of adolescents abstain from delinquent involvement (Moffitt, 1993). Thus it could be assumed that delinquency in adolescence is not just a form of antisocial behaviour but somewhat of a rite of passage. Even if delinquency is a natural progression, only the minority of delinquents persist with their misconduct throughout the lifespan whereas the majority of delinquent adolescents „grow out of it‟ (Boutwell & Beaver, 2008). LCP offenders account for approximately 6% of the population but they account for a huge proportion of all crimes committed (Moffitt et al., 2002). For example, in America LCP offenders account for 50% of all crimes committed (Eme, 2010). Misconduct in these offenders 124 STUDENT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL VOLUME I emerges in early childhood and it remains stable throughout the lifespan (Moffitt et al., 2006). LCP offending has a causal relationship with two interlocking factors; neuropsychological deficits and adverse home life. In order for LCP offending to develop it is necessary to have neuropsychological deficits but if an individual has neuropsychological deficits this does not mean he must be an LCP offender (Boutwell & Beaver, 2008). Secondly, LCP offending is causally linked with an adverse home life. A criminogenic family and neuropsychological deficits work in tandem with each other and are conducive for chronic antisocial conduct. Thus the cause of LCP offending is biosocial. Biogenic risk factors (e.g. neuropsychological deficits) coupled with environmental risk factors (e.g. criminogenic family) creates LCP offenders (Boutwell & Beaver, 2008). AL offenders constitute the vast majority of delinquent offenders. They confine their criminal involvement to adolescence; they tend not to display antisocial behaviour in childhood and they do not offend as adults (Boutwell & Beaver, 2008). According to Moffitt (1993, 2006) the „maturity gap‟ is the main cause of AL offending. It occurs when the adolescent is biologically mature (i.e. physically, they resemble adults), but they are not permitted to engage in adult behaviours. Adolescents aim to reduce the disjuncture of the „maturity gap‟ by mimicking adult behaviours. These behaviours include consuming alcohol, smoking, having sex and they are expressed in order to portray social maturity. With age comes the privileges associated with adulthood and thus the maturity gap diminishes as does delinquent behaviour along with it (Moffitt, 2006). The cause of AL offending is biosocial because there is an interaction between biological (i.e. puberty onset) and social factors (i.e. restraints imposed by society) (Boutwell & Beaver, 2008). NEURAL AND PERSONALITY FACTORS According to Eastman and Campbell (2006) there is a correlation between antisocial behaviour and impairment of the prefrontal cortices (PFC). Deception, for example, is linked with activation of the PFC and the anterior cingulate cortices. The PFC has a pivotal role in the regulation of behaviour. Frontal lobe damage in infancy results in impairments in decision-making and social and emotional regulation (Anderson, Damasio, Tranel & Damasio, 2001). PFC deficits are also greatly involved in the development of disruptive behavioural disorders such as hyperactivity, 125 REVIEW executive functioning and response inhibition deficits (Schachar & Logan, 1990). Specifically, lesions to the ventromedial PFC may result in impaired ability to adhere to social norms and attitudes (Moll et al., 2005). The social norms and attitudes affected by PFC impairment are directly linked to the laws that criminals tend to violate (Knabb, Welsh, Zieball & Reimer, 2009). Impairment in the limbic or paralimbic regions will affect moral behaviour. For example, rage, a lack of empathy or abnormal sexual behaviours will occur as a result of this type of impairment (Moll et al, 2005). Hence limbic dysfunction may contribute directly to antisocial behaviour (Knabb et al., 2009). According to Weber and colleagues (2008, p. 23) the “existing body of evidence tentatively supports the idea that psychopathy is associated with brain abnormalities in the prefrontal-tempero-limbic circuit”. Hesse (2010) has stated that Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) and psychopathy are two related concepts that are used to describe the personality problems that may result in antisocial behaviour. In support of this statement, Coid and Ullrich (2010, p. 432) have found that psychopathy and ASPD are not distinct clinical syndromes but rather are both on a continuum of ASPD. Specifically, psychopathic ASPD is “a more severe form than ASPD alone” with a greater risk of violence (Coid & Ullrich, 2010, p. 432). In relation to brain abnormalities and ASPD, Vollm and colleagues (2004) have stated that ASPD suggests dysfunction of the frontal lobes. Research has found that the frontal lobe volume of ASPD patients was 11% smaller than that of the control group (Raine et al., 2000). Damasio and colleagues (1990) have used acquired brain injury as evidence of the association between PFC damage and antisocial behaviour. PFC damage is associated with higher rates of impulsivity and poor self-control which are the characteristics of ASPD. Individuals with Cluster B personality disorders (especially ASPD) display many different PFC and memory dysfunctions. Of all the personality disorders Cluster B is particularly associated with criminal activity (Moran, 1999). Additionally, there is a strong link between Cluster B and the risk of violence and recidivism (Moran, 2002). ASPD has a prevalence rate of between 2-3% in most Western countries (Coid, 2003) and at least 50% of UK prisoners meet DSM criteria for ASPD (Singleton et al., 1998). There is a strong relationship between ASPD and crime, including violent crime 126 STUDENT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL VOLUME I (Eronen, Hakola & Tiihonen, 1996). Thus there is a greater chance of committing a felony if one has ASPD (Thompson & Bland, 1995). There is little evidence to suggest a definite cause of ASPD but familial factors including early adverse events (e.g. childhood abuse) are involved in the development of personality disorders later in life (Farrington, 1993). The possible causes of personality disorders include genetic and neurobiological liability and structural brain abnormalities. A criticism would be that in the DSM-IV the definition of ASPD emphasises antisocial behaviour and thus there is an overlap between ASPD and criminality, while according to Robins (1998) the most common symptoms of ASPD include violence and marital problems but not criminality. SOCIOECONOMIC FACTORS There is little doubt that individuals from socio-economically deprived backgrounds exhibit a greater propensity towards criminal involvement (Rutter, Giller & Hagell, 1998). According to Ferguson, Swain-Campbell and Horwood (2004) socioeconomic deprivation has a strong association with later criminal involvement. For example, they suggested that socioeconomic deprivation experienced as a child is related to higher rates of crime later in life. Also individuals with a low socioeconomic status (SES) are three times more likely to engage in criminal activity than those with a high SES (Ferguson et al., 2004). There are several theories relating the causes of crime to SES. Merton‟s (1938) strain theory aims to explain the relationship between social structures and deviance. Strain theory states that there is a dichotomy between societal expectations of desirable achievements and the possibility to actually achieve these within the society. The link between criminality and socioeconomic disadvantage is caused by these „strains‟ or imbalances which result in predisposed individuals engaging in antisocial behaviour in order to balance these stains. Hence the experience of low SES encourages criminal involvement (Kelly, 2000). According to the differential association theory of crime the higher rates of crime amongst individuals from socioeconomically deprived backgrounds are due to greater exposure to antisocial peers and environment (Ferguson et al., 2004). Ferguson and colleagues (2004) suggest that the differential association theory contrasts with strain theory via an emphasis on peer influence and motivation towards criminality. On the other hand, the social learning perspective theory 127 REVIEW claims that early exposure to maladaptive parenting and a lack of supervision encourage the development of crime (Rutter et al., 1998). Furthermore, Rutter and colleagues posit that differences in crime rates across differing social strata are directly linked with differing child rearing practices for each of the social stratum. Finally the social control theory states that the inclination towards deviance is a characteristic of all individuals but social bonds mediate these impulses (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). These social bonds may include attachments to family and school however, deterioration of these bonds occurs as a result of socioeconomic disadvantage, thus leading to criminal activity (Kelly, 2000). Ultimately, individuals from low socioeconomic environments are at an increased risk of later criminal participation because of the accumulative effects of individual, familial and peer factors (Ferguson et al., 2004). Other intervening factors between socioeconomic deprivation and crime include marital conflict, parental hostility and poor parenting (Conger, Patterson & Ge, 1995). FAMILIAL FACTORS Family influences are the greatest contributors to criminal involvement but conversely also have the greatest propensity to moderate criminal behaviours (Ferguson et al., 2004). Externalising problem behaviour is related to poor parenting (Shaw et al., 1998). Farrington (1990) found that there was a high risk of hyperactivity and conduct disorder as a result of parenting risk factors discussed below. Morrell and Murray (2003) postulated that conduct disorder requires environmental risk factors for the expression of the genetic factors that are associated with conduct disorder. Patterson‟s coercive family process model states that cycles of negative reinforcement are a risk factor for criminality. For example, this occurs when a child does not comply with the parent‟s demands but is still rewarded by the parent giving in to them (Morrell & Murray, 2003). Poor quality parenting is a better predictor of poor attention at three to four years and hyperactivity at five years than biological or temperamental factors (Carlson, Jacobvitz & Scroufe, 1995). However, a positive relationship between parent and child is an effective method of prevention of conduct disorder (Pettit, Bates & Dodge, 1997). Maternal depression is another significant risk factor in whether the individual develops a disruptive behavioural disorder. Research has found that five years after 128 STUDENT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL VOLUME I postnatal depression was experienced there was a higher risk of behavioural problems in children of postnatally depressed mothers (Murray et al., 1999). Poor quality of attachment relationships is also another risk factor for criminality in later life. According to Lyons-Ruth (1989) the attachment relationship at 18 months is a predictor for an individual‟s later functioning. Externalizing problem behaviour is associated with low quality of attachment. Golder, Gillmore, Spieker and Morrison (2005) suggested that poor attachment security may foster poor psychosocial development and behavioural problems. Conversely, Stein, Milburn, Zane and Rotheram-Borus (2009) stated that high quality attachment, particularly to fathers, was protective against delinquent behaviour. In relation to the mother, “Primary attachment relationships provide a protective foundation for the child (and later adult) to explore his or her environment and to meet developmental changes” (Stein et al., 2009, p. 40). Stein and colleagues (2009) also found that homeless youths who engaged in antisocial behaviours lacked a „secure base‟ in the form of an attachment figure and therefore these problem behaviours developed as a consequence. In contrast, Robins (1998) criticised poor parenting as a determinant of criminality because a child‟s own behaviour is a better predictor of adult antisocial behaviour than rearing, family background or SES. PEER FACTORS Finally, criminal behaviour is correlated with having criminal friends. Having criminal friends is positively linked with antisocial conduct and drug-taking (Warr, 2002). There are two opposing theories regarding peer influence over criminality. The Social Causation Perspective states that delinquent peers imprint delinquent values onto their friends because their behaviours‟ and values are mimicked and adopted by the other members of the social network (Akers, 1998). However, Control Theories state that delinquents produce traits that may instruct them to join a delinquent peer network (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Individuals self-select peers with similar traits and interests. According to Walsh (2002, p. 173) “We all seek environments that are compatible with our genetic dispositions” Interestingly, Beaver et al. (2008) posit that choosing to associate with a delinquent peer network is influenced by an individual‟s genotype. Hence the genotype chooses the environment that allows for optimum gene 129 REVIEW expression (Scarr, 1992). The phenomenon gene x environment correlation (rGE) posits that genotype and environment are inextricably intertwined. Thus genes that influence particular characteristics also influence the environment in order to achieve optimal expression (Beaver et al., 2008). For example, male adolescents with the 10R allele for DAT1 are significantly more inclined to be associated with delinquent peers than adolescent males without the allele. In families in high and low risk backgrounds the 10R allele increases the likelihood that male adolescents will associate with delinquent peers. However, low risk parents may be able to control for the effect of the gene via parenting strategies (Beaver et al., 2008). CONCLUSIONS The issue surrounding the determination of criminality is essentially a nature-nurture debate. Separately, neither genetic nor environmental factors are sufficient in explaining the determination of criminality and thus a heuristic approach must be considered. According to Beaver and Holtfreter (2009) it is the interplay between environmental and genetic factors which determines expression of an antisocial behavioural phenotype. In essence, the independent influence of one or the other tends not to yield a strong causal effect however their combined influence has a stronger causal link for individual criminal involvement. Specifically, in the case of LCP offenders a causal relationship exists between neuropsychological deficits and adverse home life. It is necessary that these individuals possess neuropsychological deficits and combined with a criminogenic family, results in chronic antisocial behaviour (Boutwell & Beaver, 2008). In relation to ASPD, causal factors include familial influences (Farrington, 1993) and dysfunction of the frontal lobes (Vollm et al., 2004). These factors work in tandem and are conducive for the expression of the antisocial phenotype. Similarly, conduct disorders require environmental risk factors for the expression of the genetic factors that are associated with the disorder (Morrell & Murray, 2003). Presently, it is suggested that a positive parent-child relationship is the most effective preventative measure against conduct disorder (Pettit, Bates & Dodge, 1997) as high quality attachment relationships have been found to be protective against delinquent behaviour (Stein et al., 2009). Beaver and colleagues (2008) found that in families in high and low risk 130 STUDENT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL VOLUME I backgrounds that although the 10R allele increased the likelihood that male adolescents associated with antisocial peers, low risk parents may be able to control for the effect of the gene through appropriate parenting strategies. Further research is required to understand how to prevent conduct disorder in those at high risk. In sum, each determining factor – such as familial, peer and genetic influences – when assessed independently is insufficient in explaining the determination of criminality. Analyses of the interactions between genetic and environmental factors are therefore necessary for understanding who is at risk for criminal behaviour and how such behaviour arises, and also for developing and implementing preventative strategies. 131 REVIEW REFERENCES Akers, R. L. (1998). Social Learning and social Structure: A General Theory of Crime and Deviance. Boston: Northeastern University Press. Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R., (2001). Long-term sequelae of prefrontal cortex damage acquired in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 281-296. Beaver, K. M., & Holtfreter, K., (2009). Biosocial influences on fraudulent behaviours. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 170(2), 101-114. Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., & DeLisi, M., (2008). Delinquent peer group formation: evidence of a gene x environment correlation. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169(3), 227244. Boutwell, B. B., & Beaver, K. M. (2008). A biosocial explanation of delinquency abstention. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health,18, 59-74. 132 Carlson, E. A., Jacobvits, D., & Sroufe, L. A., (1995). A developmental investigation of inattentiveness and hyperactivity. Child Development, 66, 37-54. Coid, J. W. (2003). Epidemiology, public health and the problem of personality disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182(44), 3–10. Coid, J., & Ullrich, S. (2010) Antisocial personality disorder is on a continuum with psychopathty. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 51, 426–433. Conger, R. D., Patterson, G. R., & Ge, X. (1995). It takes two to replicate: A meditational model for the impact of parents‟ stress on adolescent adjustment. Child Development, 66, 80-97. Damasio, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., (1990). Individuals with psychopathic behaviour caused by frontal damage fail to respond automatically to social stimuli. Brain and Behaviour Research, 41, 81-94. STUDENT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL VOLUME I Eastman, N., & Campbell, C., (2006). Neuroscience and legal determination of criminal responsibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 311-318. Eronen, M., Hakola, P., & Tiihonen, J., (1996). Mental disorder and homicidal behaviour in Finland. Arch General Psychiatry, 53, 497-501. Eme, R. (2010) Male LifeCourse-Persistent Antisocial Behavior. The Most Important Pediatric Mental Health Problem. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 164(5), 486-487. Farrington, D. P. (1986). Age and crime. In T. M. Morris (eds) Crime and Justice: An Annual Review of Research (pp. 189-250) Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Farrington, D. P., (1990). Implications of criminal career research for the prevention of offending. Journal of Adolescence, 13, 93-113. Ferguson, D., Swain-Campbell, N., & Horwood, J., (2004). How does childhood economic disadvantage lead to crime? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(5), 956-966. Golder, S., Gillmore, M. R., Spieker, S., & Momson, D. (2005). Substance use, related problem behaviors and adult attachment in a sample of high risk older adolescent women. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 14, 181-193. Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T., (1990) A General Theory of Crime. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Hesse, M. (2010) What should be done with antisocial personality disorder in the new edition of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-V)? BMC Medicine, 66(8), 1-4. Kelly, M. (2000). Inequality and crime. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 82, 530-539. Knabb, J. J., Welsh, R. K., Ziebell, J. G., & Reimer, K. S., (2009). Neuroscience, moral reasoning, and the law. Behavioural Sciences and the Law, 27, 219-236. Luntz, B. K., & Wisdom, C. S., (1994). Antisocial personality 133 REVIEW disorder in abused and neglected children grown up. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 670674. Lyons-Ruth, K. (1991). Rapprochement or approchement: Mahler's theory reconsidered from the vantage point of recent research on early attachment relationships. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 8(1), 1-23. Merton, R. K. (1938). Social structure and anomie. American Sociological Review, 3, 672-682. Moffitt, T. E. (1993) Adolescents-limited and lifecourse-persistent antisocial behaviour: A developmental taxonomy, Psychological Review,100, 674-701. Moffitt, T. E. (2006). A review of research on the taxonomy of lifecourse-persistent versus adolescence limited antisocial behaviour. In F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright, & K. P. Blevins (eds) Taking Stock: The Starus of Criminological Theory, 15,(pp. 277-311) New Brunswick: Transaction Publications. 134 Moll, J., Zahn, R., OliveriraSouza, R., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J., (2005). The neural basis of human moral cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 799-809. Moran, P. (1999) The epidemiology of antisocial personality disorder. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34, 231-242. Moran, P., (2002). Editorial: Dangerous severe personality disorder – bad things from the UK. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 48(1), 6-10. Morrell, J. & Murray, L. (2003). Parenting and the development of conduct disorder and hyperactive symptoms in childhood: a prospective longitudinal study from 2 months to 8 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 489-508. Murray, L., Sinclair, D., Cooper, P., Ducournau, P. & Turner, P., (1999). The socioemotional development of 5-year-old children of postnatally depressed mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 1259-1271. STUDENT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL VOLUME I Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A., (1997). Supportive parenting, ecological context, and children‟s adjustment: A seven-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 68, 908-923. Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., Colletti, P., (2000). Reduced prefrontal fray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 199-127. Rhee, S. H. & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behaviour: a metaanalysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490-529. Robins, L. N., (1998). The intimate connection between antisocial personality and substance abuse. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, 393-399. Rutter, M,. (2006). Genes and behaviour: Nature-Nurture Interplay Explained. Maiden, MA: Blackwell. Rutter, M. Giller, H., & Hagell, (1998). Antisocial Behaviour by Young People. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences, Child Development, 63, 1-19. Schachar, R., & Logan, G., (1990). Are hyperactive children deficient in attentional capacity? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 493-513. Shaw, D.S., Winslow, E.B., Owens, E.B., Vondra, J.I., Cohn, J.F., & Bell, R.Q. (1998). The development of early externalizing problems among children from low-income families: A transformational perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 95–107. Singleton, N., Meltzer, H., Gatward, R., Coid, J., & Deasy, D. (1998). Psychiatric Morbidity Among Prisoners in England and Wales. London: HMSO. Stein, J. A., Milburn, N. G., Zane, J. I., Rotheram-Borus, M. J. (2009) Paternal and Maternal 135 REVIEW Influences on Problem Behaviors Among Homeless and Runaway Youth. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 79(1), 39-50. Thompson, A. H., & Bland, B. C., (1995). Social dysfunction and mental illness in a community sample. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 5-20. Vollm, B., Richardson, P., Stirling, J., Elliott, R., Dolan, M., Chaudhry, I., Del Ben, C., McKie, S., Anderson, I., & Deakin, B., (2004). Neurobiological substrates of antisocial and borderline personality disorder: preliminary results of a functional fMRI study. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 14, 39-54. 136 Walsh, A. (2002). Essay review: Companions in crime: a biosocial perspective. Human Nature Review, 2, 169-178. Warr, M. (2002). Companions in Crime: The Social Aspects of Criminal Conduct. New York: Cambridge University Press. Weber, S., Habel, U., Amunts, K., & Schneider, F., (2008). Structural brain abnormalities in psychopaths- a review. Behavioural Sciences and the Law, 26, 7-28. Wisdom, C. S., (1989). The cycle of violence. Science, 244, 160166.