Download Scientists Show Bacteria Can `Learn` And Plan Ahead

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Nature versus nurture wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Published online 8 May 2008 | Nature | doi:10.1038/news.2007.360
News
Bacteria 'can learn'
Colonies evolve to anticipate changes in their surroundings.
Michael Hopkin
Ready to be eaten: bacteria can be
taught to anticipate conditions in the stomach.Punchstock
The simple life of bacteria is a little less simple than you might have thought. New research shows that
colonies of Escherichia coli can demonstrate a form of learning.
Such bacterial colonies can evolve the ability to anticipate changes in their immediate environment, say
researchers led by Saeed Tavazoie at Princeton University in New Jersey. Other types of bacteria could
too. This skill could give them the edge over other bacteria that merely adapt themselves to current
conditions.
E. coli colonies, for example, can develop the ability to associate higher temperatures (as found in a
human mouth, for example) with a lack of oxygen (as found inside the human gut). When exposed to
higher temperatures, they alter their metabolism in anticipation of lowering oxygen levels. The results
are published online in Science1.
The response is similar to Pavlovian conditioning, where after dogs learned to associate the sound of a
bell with food, they would salivate on hearing the bell.
Learning by association
This result is unexpected, Tavazoie explains. "For as long as people have been studying the behaviour
of bacteria, they have assumed that responses to environmental stimuli occur in an action–reaction
fashion," he says. This concept, often known as homeostasis, has dominated the field for a century, he
says.
"What we have found is that homeostasis is not the whole story," Tavazoie says. Bacteria can learn to
respond in a way that is more complex than a simple reaction to current conditions; it anticipates future
conditions.
It's the first evidence that bacteria have an ability for 'associative learning', Tavazoie adds. That's not to
say, of course, that single-celled organisms learn in the same way as dogs, or people.
"Associative learning in dogs and humans happens over the course of the organism's lifetime, and
involves modifications to the strength of connections between neurons in the brain," Tavazoie says.
"The learning that we have discovered in bacteria occurs over a long evolutionary time-scale and
involves changes in the connections between networks of genes."
Model behaviour
Tavazoie's team made the discovery by first using computer simulations to model the evolution of
bacteria and see whether it might be possible for them to develop anticipatory behaviour.
ADVERTISEMENT
After discovering that this indeed seemed possible, they turned to live E. coli to see whether they could
really show learning skills in the lab. They trained lab dishes of E. coli by shifting them from 25 °C to
37 °C and then from about 20% oxygen content to zero. They monitored responses over hundreds of
generations. After a few weeks, the bacteria had 'learned' to anticipate the drop in oxygen by altering
their metabolism just after the temperature change.
The new research could have implications for dealing with microbial infections and drug resistance,
Tavazoie suggests. Anticipating the behaviour of microbes could also help to guide industrial processes
that rely on them, such as brewing.

References
1.
Tagkopoulos, I., Liu, Y.-C. & Tavazoie, S. Science advanced online publication,
doi:10.1126/science.1154456 (2008).
Featured Research
from universities, journals, and other organizations
Scientists Show Bacteria Can 'Learn'
And Plan Ahead
June 18, 2009
Weizmann Institute of Science
Date:
Source:
Summary:
Scientists have shown that microorganisms can "learn" through evolution to
anticipate upcoming events and prepare for them.
E. coli bacteria. New findings show that these microorganisms' genetic networks
are hard-wired to 'foresee' what comes next in the sequence of events and begin
responding to the new state of affairs before its onset.
Credit: Image courtesy of Weizmann Institute of Science
Bacteria can anticipate a future event and prepare for it, according to new research at the
Weizmann Institute of Science. In a paper that appeared June 17 in Nature, Prof. Yitzhak
Pilpel, doctoral student Amir Mitchell and research associate Dr. Orna Dahan of the
Institute's Molecular Genetics Department, together with Prof. Martin Kupiec and Gal
Romano of Tel Aviv University, examined microorganisms living in environments that
change in predictable ways.
Their findings show that these microorganisms' genetic networks are hard-wired to 'foresee' what
comes next in the sequence of events and begin responding to the new state of affairs before its onset.
E. coli bacteria, for instance, which normally cruise harmlessly down the digestive tract, encounter a
number of different environments on their way. In particular, they find that one type of sugar – lactose
– is invariably followed by a second sugar – maltose – soon afterward. Pilpel and his team of the
Molecular Genetics Department, checked the bacterium's genetic response to lactose, and found that, in
addition to the genes that enable it to digest lactose, the gene network for utilizing maltose was
partially activated. When they switched the order of the sugars, giving the bacteria maltose first, there
was no corresponding activation of lactose genes, implying that bacteria have naturally 'learned' to get
ready for a serving of maltose after a lactose appetizer.
Another microorganism that experiences consistent changes is wine yeast. As fermentation progresses,
sugar and acidity levels change, alcohol levels rise, and the yeast's environment heats up. Although the
system was somewhat more complicated that that of E. coli, the scientists found that when the wine
yeast feel the heat, they begin activating genes for dealing with the stresses of the next stage. Further
analysis showed that this anticipation and early response is an evolutionary adaptation that increases
the organism's chances of survival.
Ivan Pavlov first demonstrated this type of adaptive anticipation, known as a conditioned response, in
dogs in the 1890s. He trained the dogs to salivate in response to a stimulus by repeatedly ringing a bell
before giving them food. In the microorganisms, says Pilpel, 'evolution over many generations replaces
conditioned learning, but the end result is similar.' 'In both evolution and learning,' says Mitchell, 'the
organism adapts its responses to environmental cues, improving its ability to survive.' Romano: 'This is
not a generalized stress response, but one that is precisely geared to an anticipated event.'
To see whether the microorganisms were truly exhibiting a conditioned response, Pilpel and Mitchell
devised a further test for the E. coli based on another of Pavlov's experiments. When Pavlov stopped
giving the dogs food after ringing the bell, the conditioned response faded until they eventually ceased
salivating at its sound. The scientists did something similar, using bacteria grown by Dr. Erez Dekel, in
the lab of Prof. Uri Alon of the Molecular Cell Biology Department, in an environment containing the
first sugar, lactose, but not following it up with maltose. After several months, the bacteria had evolved
to stop activating their maltose genes at the taste of lactose, only turning them on when maltose was
actually available.
'This showed us that there is a cost to advanced preparation, but that the benefits to the organism
outweigh the costs in the right circumstances,' says Pilpel. What are those circumstances? Based on the
experimental evidence, the research team created a sort of cost/benefit model to predict the types of
situations in which an organism could increase its chances of survival by evolving to anticipate future
events. They are already planning a number of new tests for their model, as well as different avenues of
experimentation based on the insights they have gained.
Pilpel and his team believe that genetic conditioned response may be a widespread means of
evolutionary adaptation that enhances survival in many organisms – one that may also take place in the
cells of higher organisms, including humans. These findings could have practical implications, as well.
Genetically engineered microorganisms for fermenting plant materials to produce biofuels, for
example, might work more efficiently if they gained the genetic ability to prepare themselves for the
next step in the process.
Prof. Yitzhak Pilpel's research is supported by the Ben May Charitable Trust and Madame Huguette
Nazez, Paris, France.
Story Source:
The above story is based on materials provided by Weizmann Institute of Science. Note: Materials may be edited for content
and length.
Weizmann Institute of Science. "Scientists Show Bacteria Can 'Learn' And Plan Ahead." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 18 June
2009. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2009/06/090617131400.htm>.