Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
10/14/10 BY2204.Lecture 13 SF Evolution Module Case Study of Co-evolution: Aposematism and Mimicry But some are bright colours, conspicuous shapes, very visible to predators. Why? They are usually toxic and defended by their “warning coloration”. They also have warning smells, movements etc. so it’s better to say “aposematic”. Aposematism = signalling unprofitability to potential predators. Most edible animals are cryptic (= camouflaged). Perfection of crypsis argues that conspicuousness carries a heavy cost Some which can’t hide by crypsis, use masquerade, by appearing to be something inedible e.g. a bird dropping or a leaf For aposematism to work, the predator needs to know the meaning of the signal already. It must also remember which signal meant poison. So the aposematic signaller wants the predator to see and recognise it. It’s to the predator’s advantage to learn too – it doesn’t want to make a mistake and lose its dinner! So aposematic signalling is a co-evolved trait, to both birds’ and insects’ mutual benefit = a mutualism 1 10/14/10 Co-evolution is when changes in one species cause adaptations in another, which further change the selective pressures on the first, and so alter its evolution, and so on. This may be: A mutualism = everybody benefits from a change in either species e.g. signalling by poisonous prey to their predators If aposematism is a mutualism, then we might expect the signal to be designed to help the predator avoid the prey. We might also expect the predator to be adapted to learn this signal easily. Aposematism relies on the predator recognising the signal from its early encounters with it, and learning to avoid touching prey that look the same. So factors which enhance the speed of learning are of benefit to aposematic animals. An Arms Race = adaptation by one species is detrimental to the fitness of the other e.g. increasing crypsis in camouflaged prey and increasing ability to find them by the predator Factors which aid learning - Conspicuousness (also gives more time to remember signal’s meaning) - Novelty of signal - Repeated signal - Truthful about outcome - Consistent effect following signal - Signal quickly followed by the punishment or reward - Confirmed by other signals So aposematic animals should be bright, conspicuous, preferably aggregated and nasty tasting, with other cues e.g. odour, taste, smell to back up their message… and they are. The most common signals are bright, memorable colours, which the predator quickly learns to associate with the toxic effects. Stripes and spots; black with contrasting red or yellow. The cost of aposematism to the prey species is training naïve predators into the meaning of the signal. New baby birds eat the prey because they don’t know it’s dangerous. They learn by this, but they kill the prey insect. So the insect pays the cost that it may be the one who dies training the predator. Aposematism therefore carries an intrinsic risk. This risk is lower, if you share a pattern with lots of others so the chance of it being you who does the teaching is lower. So there is a second reason why aposematic insects should aggregate – dilution of the training cost. 2 10/14/10 In addition, the risk to each individual is reduced if several species share the same signals. Mullerian mimicry was named after Fritz Muller 1878 Mullerian mimicry ring: Ladybirds This reduces the number of signals the predator must learn. Red ring: 7 spot Thus the different prey species co-evolve their colour patterns So there’s a second mutualistic coevolutionary relationship in the system with different prey species sharing the same warning signal. 11 spot Black ring: Pine Kidney spot 5 spot harlequin eyed 24 spot Yellow ring: harlequin 16 spot 14 spot 22 spot Sharing of signals between defended prey species = Mullerian Mimicry Mullerian mimics experience normalising selection pressures Range of pattern found in Different Mullerian mimics experience convergent evolutionary selection pressures Patterns more like the most protected central pattern are at an advantage so normalising selection occurs, reducing variation in the signal. Abundance of pattern Level of protection given by that pattern the species Halo of protection around that pattern, which birds also mistake for the defended pattern Species A and B start off with different patterns Their areas of protection overlap as birds can’t always tell them apart. Prey with patterns in this overlap zone are protected by both patterns, so pay a reduced cost of educating predators, and are therefore at a selective advantage. Thus the two patterns converge until all individuals of both species have the intermediate pattern. 3 10/14/10 Best studied mimics = Heliconius butterflies Mullerian mimicry ring: Viceroy butterfly and models Monarch is very toxic. Viceroy is very nasty tasting, and a very good mimic of the monarch in most of USA, despite being a different genus. In Florida Viceroy resembles the Queen butterfly, a sister species of the Monarch. Similar looking butterflies aren’t closely related. Viceroy Closely related ones have very divergent patterns even if they live in the same place. Monarch In Mexico, it mimics another monarch sister species, the Soldier. The viceroy is therefore in 3 different Mullerian mimicry pairings, copying the local species of Danaus in each place. Suggests convergent evolution occurred several times in the Viceroy Henry Walter Bates 1861 proposed Batesian Mimicry = edible prey copying the signal of aposematic animals. Hoverflies, beetles and stingless wasps all copy the signal of the true wasp, called the model. Each mimic is completely harmless. Batesian Mimics confuse the message, as they are edible, and teach the wrong message to the naïve predators. The models lose out by this because more of them are killed re-educating the predator. The predators lose out, because they end up eating more toxic prey. Batesian mimics are therefore parasites of the aposematic signalling system. Why don’t they all converge to the same pattern? Queen One answer relates to Batesian mimicry… Soldier Batesian mimics e.g. hoverflies, are in two co-evolutionary relationships: One with their models (the aposematic species they copy) e.g. wasps One with the predators e.g birds Both of these are arms races because: an evolutionary step by the Batesian mimic reduces the fitness of the model or predator …and an evolutionary step by the model or predator reduces the fitness of the Batesian mimic. 4 10/14/10 Using our curves of protection we can look at the coReal evolutionary trajectory: patterns Halo of protection A is the model and B is the Batesian mimic, with a similar but not identical pattern. 2) Batesian mimics are in an arms race with the predators. As Batesian mimics confuse the signalling system, the predators end up unsure whether the signal means danger. They end up taking longer to learn avoidance of it, so end up eating more toxin. model mimic Birds mistake mimics in the overlapping zone for the model, so these individuals get good protection from the birds. So there is directional selection on mimic towards patterns most like the model, and the curve will gradually shift left. But patterns of the model which are in the overlapping zone will suffer most from the confusion caused by the mimic. Patterns on the extreme other side will do best, so species A will gradually move left too. B moves faster, because as A gets more unlike the original pattern, it loses some of its protection from predators who already know that pattern. B loses nothing by changing pattern. Frequency Dependence Mullerian Mimics are positively frequency dependent = they do better the more of them there are because it allows them to share the cost of educating predators. Batesian mimics are negatively frequency dependent = they do better the fewer of them there are… ..because they ruin the signal they are parasitising. Problem for their evolutionary success! Any mutation of the Batesian mimic to look more like the model is still favoured But any mutation of the bird which makes it better able to distinguish the Batesian mimic from the model is favoured. This makes the halo of protection around the original pattern narrower, as the bird’s error around the signal reduces. Overlap much reduced Halo of protection around the signal Less overlap produces fewer errors. Batesian mimics are often polymorphic to get out of this problem. Being polymorphic allows them to spread the load between more than one signal, so increase in total number. 2 spot ladybirds have both red and black morphs, so they parasitise both the red and black Mullerian mimicry rings. 10 spot ladybirds have 3 morphs, one in the red ring, two in the black. NB 2 spot and 10 spot ladybirds are perfectly edible. Their models, 7spot (red model) and pine ladybird (black model), are toxic Red ring model Black ring model 5 10/14/10 Disruptive selection So 2spot and 10spot ladybirds experience disruptive selection, i.e. working to tear the population apart into two distinct morphs. Note: The black model is less common than the red model, so gives less effective protection. Consequently black 2spot ladybirds are less common than red ones. The coral snake is deadly poisonous. The milk snake is harmless, but copies the signal of the coral snake. “red touches yellow will kill a fellow” … but the signal is accurate enough to fool a predator in a hurry! Even less good mimics are ok. But most models are not deadly. A trained predator is a safe one – killing it means having to teach another naïve one (at a cost to the models and the Batesian mimics). It has been argued that actually the coral snake is mimicking the less poisonous milk snake, who is training predators, not killing them. (= Mertensian mimicry) So why don’t Mullerian mimics all converge on the same pattern? Polymorphic model H. melpomene Polymorphic Batesian mimic H. erato The models may end up polymorphic, so as to avoid a pattern which already carries a huge Batesian load. The cost of teaching the predators a new pattern may be less than the cost of working with a signal which the predators are confused by. Automimic = member of an aposematic species which lacks the defensive chemical so are effectively Batesian mimics of their own species. e.g. monarch butterfly usually lays its eggs on milkweed, from which its caterpillars sequester toxins. If no milkweed available, they lay on plants without the toxin. These individuals are automimics, reducing the fitness of their own better protected relatives by degrading their own signal! 6 10/14/10 Summary: Co-evolutionary associations may be very complex, multi-species, and driving evolution in different directions at the same time. There are many more; it’s not only predator/ prey associations. Mutualisms exist in which all the species benefit by adaptation in any of them Arms races are also common, where adaptation by one species is detrimental to the others. Mullerian and Batesian mimicry systems provide examples of both mutualistic and arms race coevolutionary associations which combine to drive evolutionary change in a predictable direction. Key words and glossary: Coevolution - when two or more species influence each other's evolution Aposematism - toxic and brightly coloured organisms (= Warning Colouration) Convergent evolution - the development of similar traits in distantly related organisms. Mullerian mimicry - toxic organisms which share a colour pattern with other toxic ones. Batesian mimicry - edible organisms which copy the signal of toxic models (usually Mullerian mimics). Frequency dependent selection - when the selection pressure depends on the relative numbers of two types of individual e.g. Batesian mimicry It is negative when more of one species means less survival for that species. Background Reading: Barnard “Animal Behaviour” Chapter 8.3 “Antipredator behaviour” Alcock “Animal Behavior” 8th Ed. Chapter 6 “Behavioural adaptations for survival” Krebs and Davies “Introduction to Behavioural Ecology” Chapter 4 “Predators versus prey” And look up any of the key words in the web or behaviour text books. If you get into it: Speed MP 2000 Warning signals, receiver psychology and predator memory. Animal Behaviour 60, 269-278 7