Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
On the relation between derivation and inflection in word-formation: evidence from the Griko verb system Nikos Koutsoukos1 & Angela Ralli2 University of Patras1,2 1 Universiteit Leiden , Seeger Research Fellow, Princeton University 2 In grammatical models which predict that derivation and inflection potentially interact, the crucial question to be addressed concerns the demarcation of this interaction in terms of morphological marking. As observed in Stump (2005), in theory the question is a straightforward one. However, in practice, the distinction between the two processes is rather blurred, since the inflectional properties of derived words can be part of the derivational process. This idea has been clearly articulated by Aronoff (1994), who argues on the basis of Hebrew verbal morphology that a derivational process may assign membership in a given inflection class to its output stems. In this paper, we focus on some interesting data drawn from Griko verb inflection. Griko is a dialect of Greek origin, spoken by approximately 20,000 speakers in 9 neighbouring villages which are located at the centre of the Salentine peninsula (Southern Italy). Verb inflection in this dialect is mainly elaborated on two major inflectional paradigms, the distinction between which is based on the presence or absence of a systematic pattern of allomorphy. Verb stems belonging to the first paradigm (P-A) display a pattern Χ(a/e) ~ Χ(i) (where X represents part of the stem and the vowel in parenthesis is the stem-final vowel) whereas the absence of this pattern characterizes verb formations belonging to the second paradigm (P-B) (cf. Ralli, 1988, 2005 for the inflection in Standard Modern Greek). Table (1) provides illustrative examples: Table (1): Griko Inflectional paradigms Paradigm A agap(á)o ‘I love’ BASE-1SG agápi-sa BASE-AORIST.1SG Paradigm B gráf-o ‘I write’ BASE-1SG é-grap-sa AUG-BASE-AORIST.1SG Interestingly, as noticed by Karanastasis (1997) and Katsoyiannou (1995), many verbal bases tend to shift from P-A to P-B, which has been characterized as the ‘strong’ inflection class compared to the ‘weak’ P-A. This shift is realized with the presence of a verbalizer, which, in most cases, takes the form -idz-, a very productively used deverbal suffix in both Standard Modern Greek and its dialects: 1 Table (2): Pairs of verbal formations in Griko Set 1 akkoumb-ó ‘to lean’ BASE-1SG vast-ó ‘to hold’ BASE-1SG Paradigm A A Set 2 akkoumb-ίdz-o ‘to lean’ BASE-VERBALISER-1SG vast-ίdz-o ‘to hold’ BASE-VERBALISER-1SG Paradigm B B On the basis of these data, we would like to claim, first, that the degree of productivity of the two inflectional paradigms triggers the operation of crossparadigmatic levelling in favour of Paradigm B, which can be considered as the default paradigm of the Griko verbal system. Crucially, this levelling process raises an important question with respect to the type of derivational suffix that is used for the restructuring of verb bases. We would like to propose that the choice of this suffix, i.e. that of -idz-, is determined by the base itself, namely by its Xi- allomorph found in the past tense (aorist), which has always played a prominent role in Greek derivation (see also Ralli, 2012). Second, we show that contrary to the general tendency seen in cross-paradigmatic levelling, this change does not result in simplification of the morphological structure involved. As shown by Table (2), the two data sets differ with respect to their structural complexity, while members of the same pair have the same semantic compositionality. In fact, the derived verbs have become more complex, in that they contain a morphologically complex base, as opposed to the original verbs, the base of which is morphologically simple. Finally, assuming that a set of stems bearing a particular mark of derivation can be exactly the set of stems available to a particular inflectional paradigm, we argue that derivational marking changes the inflectional properties of the verbal bases by shifting the inflectional class from the ‘weak’ paradigm A to the ‘strong’ (or default) paradigm B. In fact, the findings of the Griko data have several implications for the discussion on the relation between inflection and derivation, since they show that the two processes are interrelated in word-formation. Selected references Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself. Stems and inflectional classes (Linguistic Ιnquiry monographs). MIT Press. Karanastasis, A. (1997). Grammar of the Greek dialects of Southern Italy (in Greek). Athens: Academy of Athens. Katsoyiannou, M. (1995). The verb in the Greek dialect of Southern Italy (in Greek). Studies in Greek Linguistics. Thessaloniki: University of Thessaloniki, 541-553. Stump, G. (2005). Delineating the boundary between inflection-class marking and derivational marking: The case of Sanskrit -aya. In W. U. Dressler, D. Kastovsky & F. Rainer (Eds.) Morphology and its Demarcations. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 293-309. Ralli, Α. (1988). Eléments de la morphologie du grec moderne: la structure du verbe. Ph.D. Diss. Montreal: Université de Montréal. Ralli, A. (2005). Morfologia [Morphology]. Athens: Patakis. Ralli, A. (2012). Morphology in language contact: verbal loanblend formation in Asia Minor Greek (Aivaliot). In Th. Stolz et al. (Eds.) Morphologies in Contact. Berlin: Akademie Verlag, 185-211. 2