Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Breeding habitat selection is firmly connected to fitness as conditions within the breeding territory (e.g., microclimate, food abundance, and predation risk) can strongly affect reproductive success (Cody 1985, Jaenike & Holt 1991, Martin 1998). Therefore, selection should favor strategies that allow birds to assess spatial heterogeneity in resources to bias selection toward higher quality sites to potentially increase reproductive success (Lima 2009, Forsman et al. 2013). Heterogeneity in breeding-site quality can scale up to the population level as demonstrated in sitedependent population regulation models (e.g., Rodenhouse & Holmes 1997, McPeek et al. 2001) and empirical work (e.g., Rodenhouse et al. 2003, Zajac et al. 2008, Kelly et al., in review). Site-dependent models, and habitat selection models in general, invoke several key assumptions including the existence of (1) spatial heterogeneity in primary resources, (2) biased settlement in sites with greater resources, and (3) the ability of organisms to assess spatial heterogeneity in resources, either directly or indirectly. An important resource contributing to breeding-site quality for songbirds likely includes the availability of spatial refugia from predators (or predator-poor space), as the bulk of nest mortality is the result of predation. By several accounts, 80% of nest failures are on average attributed to predation (Ricklefs 1969, Martin 1993, Remes et al. 2012). Furthermore, nest predation has been identified as one of the possible 1 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 causes for declines of some Neotropical migratory birds in North America (Thompson 2007). This suggests birds likely possess behavioral traits to assess spatial variation in predator activity to subsequently make adaptive breeding decisions. Spatial and temporal variation in predator activity can emerge through several behavioral mechanisms such as the underlying distribution of important resources and the predator’s own risk of predation. Regardless of the mechanism, spatial heterogeneity in predator activity can create small-scale spatial refugia for breeding birds particularly when predation is incidental. Incidental predation being undirected and occurring when generalist predators while foraging on relatively common primary prey randomly encounter and consume scarce alternative prey (Vickery et al. 1992, Yanes & Suarez 1996, Schmidt et al. 2001a, Vigallon & Marzluff 2005). Observational (e.g., Spaans et al. 1998, Sergio et al. 2003, Forstmeier and Weiss 2004, Roos & Pärt 2004, Morton 2005, Schmidt et al. 2006) and experimental studies (e.g., Eggers et al. 2006, Fontaine & Martin 2006a, Fontaine & Martin 2006b, Hua et al. 2013) have demonstrated that breeding birds select sites of lower predation risk (further reviewed in Lima 2009). Most studies have investigated nest predator avoidance at scales exceeding the territory level and quantify nest predation risk using coarse measures of predator abundance. Examinations of nest predator avoidance at smaller spatial scales (e.g., nest-site choice within a breeding pair’s territory) combined with finer-grained measures of predator space use are surprisingly scarce (Lima 2009). Yet, small-scale refugia may be an important metric for assessing nest predation risk as it is likely a key component of breeding-site quality. If so, 2 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 determining the underlying factors that create spatial heterogeneity in local predator abundance would be important information for enhancing and managing breeding bird habitat. Furthermore, spatial avoidance of nest predators suggests that birds are able to assess spatial heterogeneity in local predator abundance. Nesting birds should be receptive to direct cues of risk (e.g., predator presence) as well as exploit communication systems (e.g., vocalizations; Peake 2005) that can indirectly reveal areas of concentrated predator activity. But few investigators have experimentally isolated the sources of information used (e.g., Eggers et al. 2006, Forsman & Martin 2009, Mönkkönen et al. 2009, Forsman et al. 2013). Identifying the assessment mechanisms used by birds to evaluate local predator abundance could lend further empirical support for site-dependent regulation of populations (Rodenhouse & Holmes 1997, McPeek et al. 2001). In addition, differences in information use between sympatrically breeding birds may emerge from dissimilar sensory systems, natural history constraints (e.g., ground vs. shrub nests) or ecological tradeoffs (e.g., safety vs. microclimate; Eggers et al. 2006). Such interspecific differences could potentially affect community structure (Fletcher 2008) by causing competitors to increasingly overlapping in space or through dissimilar habitat use. Lastly, identifying the sources of information birds use to avoid nest predators may help predict how particular anthropogenic disturbances may disrupt information acquisition. For example, anthropogenic noise may reduce the ability of birds to detect and use acoustic 3 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 information provided by heterospecifics (e.g., alarm calls) or intercept predator communication for predator avoidance. The study system Ongoing research (1998 – present) at the Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies in southeastern New York, has monitored rodent (white-footed mice, Peromyscus leucopus and eastern chipmunks, Tamias striatus) and songbird populations to primarily understand the relationship between variation in rodent abundance/space use and nest predation. During this time frame, annual variation in summer rodent abundance has spanned over two-orders of magnitude in response to interannual variation in synchronous oak masting events occurring in the prior autumn (Ostfeld et al. 1996, Jones et al. 1998, Schmidt & Ostfeld 2008). These fluctuations are ecologically relevant as rodents are important predators of songbird nests (Reitsma et al. 1990, Jędrzejewska & Jędrzejewski 1998, Williams & Wood 2002, Cain et al. 2003, Forstmeier & Weiss 2004, King & DeGraaf 2006, Clotfelter et al. 2007, Schmidt et al. 2008, Cox et al. 2012, Haché et al. 2014). Studies have focused on two sympatric, territorial songbirds, veeries (Catharus fuscescens) and ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla). Veeries are small (28 g) thrushes building open-cup nests predominately in shrubs (≤ 1.5 m in height) and infrequently directly on the ground. In contrast, ovenbirds are obligate, ground-nesting warblers (20 g) constructing dome-shaped nests possessing a side entrance. Ovenbirds and veeries, like many Holarctic songbirds, are susceptible to nest predation from rodent 4 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 generalist predators that prey upon eggs, nestlings and even fledgling young. Previous research from our study site has verified that rodents are important predators of some songbird nests as annual nest daily-mortality rate in veeries is an increasing function of rodent density (Schmidt 2003, Schmidt & Ostfeld 2008, Kelly et al., in review). However, annual nest daily-mortality rate ovenbirds this is not affected by rodent abundance (Kelly et al., in review). Spatial heterogeneity in predation risk to incidental prey may be generated by the proportion of space used by generalist predators, where space use may largely be determined by their assessment of the profitability of foraging patches (Brown 1988). For example, mice may direct the bulk of their foraging efforts in patches with a greater abundance of primary prey (e.g., arthropods or tree seeds) and/or with low predation risk. Resulting unused space by generalist predators can create spatial refugia for incidental prey to increase the probability of survival (Schmidt et al. 2006, Schauber et al. 2009, Tyler 2011). Furthermore, several experiments suggest that veery nest success is also related to smaller-scale mouse activity near the nest site (Schmidt et al. 2001a, 2001b, 2003b, 2006, Schmidt 2006). General summary of research and hypotheses My dissertation examines the relationship between veery and ovenbird nest success, nest-site selection and spatial heterogeneity in small-scale rodent activity. All research took place in eastern deciduous forest on the property of the Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies in the Hudson Valley of New York. To investigate variation in 5 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 rodent activity I conducted two studies (Chapters II and III). The first study (Chapter II), utilized a long-term dataset of rodent captures collected from six 2.25 ha trapping grids using a spatially-explicit approach to examine spatial patterns in rodent activity. In the second study (Chapter III), I used track plates to measure the spatial activity of nest predators (rodents and mesopredators) on a shorter time scale. The final data chapter describes a playback experiment to determine if songbirds eavesdrop on chipmunk vocalizations to assess their spatial heterogeneity to make nest-site decisions. Based on the earlier work described above, I formulated three general predictions: (1) To lower the probability of nest predation ovenbirds and veeries would place nests in locations of relatively lower rodent activity than other available sites. (2) Veery and ovenbird nest success would be negatively related to small-scale rodent activity surrounding nest sites. (3) Lastly, in response to playbacks simulating “hotspots” of chipmunk activity, ground-nesting songbirds should place nests significantly further from plots broadcasting chipmunk calls compared to control plots that broadcasted frog calls or were silent. 6 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 CHAPTER II NEST-SITE SELECTION AND REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS OF VEERIES AND OVENBIRDS IN RESPONSE TO SPATIAL HETEROGENEITY IN RODENT ACTIVITY Introduction Spatial and temporal variation in predator activity creates enemy-free space for prey to take refuge, increasing the probability of survival for adults or offspring. Spatial heterogeneity in predator space use can be generated through a variety of nonmutually exclusive mechanisms that emerge as a result of underlying heterogeneity in primary resources (Roos & Pärt 2004), the predators’ own risk of predation (Forstmeier & Weiss 2004, Schmidt 2006, Dutra et al. 2011, Malo et al. 2012), competition (Durant 1998), habitat complexity (Bowman 1980, Lecomte et al. 2008, Gorini et al. 2012) and the energetic state of the animal (Brown et al. 1992). The availability of spatial refugia from predation may be especially important for less mobile prey that are restricted to a central location and are thus limited in the types of behavioral responses they can use to avoid predators. The eggs and young of songbirds are an example of vulnerable, sessile prey that are anchored to a central location for a month or more. Indeed, studies have consistently shown that nest failure is primarily the result of predation as, on average, 80% of nest failures are attributed to predation (Ricklefs 1969, Martin 1993, Remeš et 7 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 al. 2012). In addition, nest predation is thought to be principally incidental (Vickery et al. 1992, Yanes & Suarez 1996, Schmidt et al. 2001a, Schmidt et al. 2004a, Vigallon & Marzluff 2005), in that it is largely undirected and occurs when generalist predators, while foraging for primary prey, serendipitously encounter and consume scarce secondary prey (Vickery et al. 1992, Schmidt et al. 2001a, McKinnon et al. 2013). Therefore, selection should favor strategies that allow birds to proactively evaluate spatial heterogeneity in nest-predation risk to select breeding sites that lower the probability of reproductive failure (Lima 2009). Examples of nest predator spatial avoidance include red-backed shrikes (Lanius collurio) that shift their breeding territories in response to annual variation in the spatial distribution of magpie (Pica pica) and hooded crow (Corvus corone) territories, both regular nest predators of shrikes (Roos & Pärt 2003). Similarly, territoriality in eagle owls (Bubo bubo) creates relatively predator-free zones within the interstitial spaces between adjacent owl territories where black kites (Milvus migrans) choose to nest (Sergio et al. 2003). Large-scale predator removals (Fontaine & Martin 2006a) and playback experiments (Hua et al. 2013) have found lower occurrence or abundance of several breeding songbird species compared to control areas. The importance of selecting breeding sites of lower predation risk is highlighted in cases where birds potentially tradeoff food availability for safety. For instance, Morton (2005) found ground-nesting ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla) consistently selected territories along forest edges as chipmunks were nearly absent from these sites. Moreover, he determined that these sites had significantly lower food 8 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 availability for ovenbirds than the forest interior, where chipmunks were in abundance. Likewise, the dusky warbler (Phylloscopus fuscatus), a shrub-nesting songbird, responded to increasing chipmunk abundance by building nests higher and in more isolated shrubs surrounded by open tundra further removed from larger closed shrub stands (Forstmeier & Weiss 2004) despite the extra energy demands expended by flying from isolated patches to forage. Chipmunks avoided open tundra presumably to reduce predation risk associated with their own predators, diurnal short-eared owls (Asio flammeus) and foxes (Forstmeier & Weiss 2004). Within a breeding bird’s territory, small-scale heterogeneity in nest-predation risk can be generated by generalist predators evaluating of the costs and benefits of foraging patches. For generalist predators such as rodents, spatial variation in the profitability of food patches balanced by the costs of predation risk may cause some patches to become periodically unoccupied (Brown 1988). Thus generalist predator activity becomes concentrated in low risk, high reward patches whereas unprofitable space goes unused, potentially creating important refugia for songbirds to place their nests. Schmidt & Ostfeld (2003b) provided evidence for such a mechanism. On six 2.25 ha rodent trapping grids they recorded similar white-footed mouse (Peromyscus leucopus) densities (31 – 49 mice); however, within trapping grids the proportion of space used by mice varied considerably (10 – 70%; Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003b). This suggests the presence of unused space by mice may still persist even when their densities are high. Moreover, they found that the daily mortality rate of artificial songbird nests was an increasing function of the proportion of space used, but not the 9 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 number of individual mice caught within the same area around artificial nest sites (Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003b). Further work by Schmidt et al. (2006) examined if smallscale mouse activity (sub-territory level) affected veery (Catharus fuscescens) nest success and nest-site selection using nests that were found over five years on six trapping grids (see above). Their study revealed (1) significant spatial heterogeneity in mouse activity, some trap stations caught few or no mice (i.e., coldspots) while captures at other stations were > 2 SD above the mean (i.e., hotspots); (2) annual veery nest-predation rates showed both a significant positive relationship to mouse abundance and a negative relationship to mouse-free space. (2) Veery nest success was significantly related to mouse activity within a “nest neighborhood” (i.e., 9 trap stations within a 30 × 30 m area centered on a nest). (4) Lastly, 75% of nests were associated with sites of below-average mouse activity suggesting that veeries are capable of assessing small-scale mouse activity (Schmidt et al. 2006). The crux of this research suggests that predator density may be a misleading indicator of nest-predation risk and that predator space use may poorly correlate with, or lag behind, changes in density (Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003, Schmidt & Schauber 2007). Furthermore, smallscale predator activity surrounding nest sites might provide better estimates of nestpredation risk than larger-scale abundance estimates. This could partly explain why some studies find a lack of congruence between predator abundance and nest success Surprisingly though, few studies have attempted to examine avoidance of nest predators at smaller spatial scales such as nest placement made within a breeding pair’s territory (Lima 2009). As described above, research has primarily focused on 10 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 adjustments that birds make when selecting territories or larger-scale habitat decisions (Roos & Pärt 2003, Sergio et al. 2003, Fontaine & Martin 2006a, Hua et al. 2013). In addition, nest-predation risk is usually quantified by collecting coarse abundance measurements of nest predators (Schmidt & Schauber 2007). However, methods such as transect surveys or point counts (e.g., Forstmeier & Weiss 2004, Morton 2005) are limited in their ability to quantify small-scale space use of nest predators. Quantifying small-scale activity predator activity may be an important metric for assessing nestpredation risk as predator-poor space is likely a key component of breeding-site quality. My objective was to quantify small-scale spatial activity of two common predators of songbird nests, white-footed mice and eastern chipmunks (Tamias striatus). And furthermore, to determine if nest success and nest-site selection of ovenbirds and veeries were related to small-scale spatial heterogeneity in rodent activity. Where “spatial activity”, hereafter, refers to metrics that quantify the aggregate amount a specific location is used by mice or chipmunks. For example, a particular location may record an equal value as a result of a single mouse visiting five times or from five different mice visiting the location once. Hence, spatial activity attempts to quantify space use intensity, rather than the number of individuals, at spatially-referenced locations. To accomplish these objectives, I expanded on the work of Schmidt et al. (2006). I now examine 15 years of trapping data which captures a greater breadth of rodent population fluctuations and incorporates a more spatially explicit approach using randomization procedures to determine regions of high and low mouse and 11 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 chipmunk activity. I used Spatial Analysis by Distance Indices (SADIE; Perry et al. 1995, 1999). SADIE measures and detects nonrandomness in the overall spatial pattern and calculates local indices for identifying clusters of sample locations as being arranged randomly or consisting of local neighborhoods of similarly sized counts in the form of ‘patches’ or ‘gaps’ (Perry et al. 1999). Such spatially-explicit methods may provide improved estimates regarding the spatial pattern of rodent activity. Furthermore, I include a heterospecific comparison of sympatrically breeding veeries and ovenbirds whose nesting ecology differs markedly (e.g., shrub vs. ground nests), however their nests are both accessible to mice and chipmunks. Based on earlier work I formulated two central hypotheses. First, to lower the probability of nest predation, I predicted ovenbirds and veeries would place nests in areas of lower rodent activity. Second, I predicted that veery and ovenbird nest success would be more strongly predicted by small-scale rodent activity surrounding their nest sites than larger-scale measures of rodent abundance. Lastly, I also expected heterospecific differences in response to the two rodent nest predators. Ovenbirds and veeries both place their nests on or near the ground (veeries ≤ 1.5 m in shrubs; Heckscher 2004, Bevier et al. 2005) so presumably both species’ nests are similar in their accessibility to ground-foraging rodents. However, heterospecific differences in nest-site microhabitats and/or architecture (Martin 1995), how parent birds manage risk (Martin et al. 2000, Eggers et al. 2008) could influence their respective predation risk and consequently which predator they avoid. Based on earlier research from our study site (Emmering & Schmidt 2011; Chapter 4), I predicted that ovenbirds would 12 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 nest in areas of lower chipmunk activity. Emmering & Schmidt (2011) demonstrated that veeries and ovenbirds placed nests significantly further from playbacks broadcasting chipmunk vocalizations than sites broadcasting frog calls or silent controls. Ovenbirds placed nests > 20 m from playbacks of chipmunks whereas the veery response was half the distance of ovenbirds, placing nests only 10 m further from chipmunk playbacks. Playback results suggest that ovenbirds may particularly use chipmunk vocalizations to assess chipmunk activity in space and subsequently are able to avoid concentrations of chipmunks to reduce nest-predation risk. Methods Field research was conducted on the property of the Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies (41º 50´ N, 73º 45´ W) located in Dutchess County, Millbrook, New York, USA from 1998 – 2012. The property consists of ~325 ha of continuous eastern deciduous forest with a oak-dominated canopy (Quercus rubra and Q. prinus). Common understory trees and shrubs include oak and maple (Acer spp.) saplings, witch hazel (Hamamelis virginiana) as well as nonnative honeysuckle (Lonciera spp.) and Japanese barberry (Berberis thunbergii). Trapping protocol and rodent enumeration I used a 15-year dataset (1998 – 2012) of mouse and chipmunk captures collected annually (May – August) from six 2.25 ha permanent trapping grids (GC, GE, TE, TC, HC, and HE). Grids consisted of an 11 × 11 array (12 × 10 for one grid) 13 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 of 121 trap stations equally spaced 15 m apart. Each trap station consisted of two Sherman live traps (7.6 × 8.9 × 30 cm) placed side by side with a cover board on top for protection from sun and precipitation. The trapping schedule rotated between grids such that one pair of grids was trapped every 2 – 3 weeks between trapping sessions. Traps were baited daily with oats between 16:00 – 18:00 hrs. and checked the following two mornings between 07:30 – 11:30 hrs. On cold nights (≤ 10C), sunflower seed and raw cotton were added to traps in addition to the oats to provide a fattier food source and warm bedding material. Captured animals were given a unique numbered ear tag, measured for standard morphometrics and examined for reproductive status and age then released where captured (for more details, see Jones et al. 1998). The above methods were approved by an institutional animal care and use committee. Mouse and chipmunk abundance were estimated using different estimators as the probability of capturing mice is higher than chipmunks. Mouse abundance was estimated for each grid’s trapping week using minimum number known alive (MNA). For chipmunk abundance, a Jolly-Seber (JS) heterogeneous survival model was used (Pledger et al. 2010). Both chipmunk and mouse abundance estimates from, or interpolated for, the approximate midpoint of the songbird breeding season (June 15) were used for all subsequent analyses. To quantify rodent activity total captures (not individuals) per trap station of mice, chipmunks and combined (mice + chipmunks) were enumerated for each grid × year combination. Total captures were summed over the migratory songbird breeding 14 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 season from approximately the first week May – 1 August. I reasoned that total captures, not the number of individuals captured in a trap, likely provides a better proxy for rodent activity. To explain, a single mouse captured several times probably poses a greater threat to a nesting songbird than a single individual caught only once. During this timeframe a minimum of three trapping sessions (or six trapping nights total) for each grid × year combination occurred with a single exception (GE in 2012) that had only 2 trapping sessions. Otherwise, out of 90 total grid × year combinations < 88% had 6 – 8 trapping nights/year; a few grid × year combinations had ≤ 10 (n=9) trapping nights. Nest data From May – July of each year, nest searching took place on all trapping grids but only nests found within 15 m of trapping grids were used in the following analyses. From 2005 – 2012, I recorded nest locations using a GPS unit and determined each nest’s nearest trap station using ArcGIS 9.1; prior to 2005, the nearest trap station was recorded in the field for each nest. Active nests were monitored at minimum every three days until young fledged or the nest was depredated. Only a single nest per breeding pair/territory/year was used for all analyses due to the lack of independence between associated renests. Single nesting attempts regardless of fate (abandoned, unknown, depredated or fledged) were used to investigate nest-site choice. To examine the relationship between rodent activity and reproductive success, nests were classified as either fledged or failed. Nests were considered fledged if they 15 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 successfully fledged at least one nestling. Completed nests that became inactive before the earliest possible fledging date, whether due to abandonment or from clear signs of depredation (e.g., broken eggs, destroyed nest), were considered failed. Spatial Analysis by Distance Indices (SADIE) Grid-level (large scale) and small-scale rodent activity (i.e., rodent captures) within nest neighborhoods was analyzed using Spatial Analysis by Distance Indices (SADIE). SADIE is a set of nonparametric methods developed by Perry et al. (1995, 1999) specifically designed to examine count data collected at spatially-referenced sample locations. SADIE allows an investigator to (1) test spatially-referenced count data for nonrandom spatial patterns, (2) detect clusters of “patches” and “gaps” composed of neighboring sample locations with relatively high or low counts, respectively, and (3) assess the amount to which each sample location contributes to the overall spatial pattern (Perry et al. 1995, Perry et al. 1999). Furthermore, output can be illustrated in corresponding mapped plots depicting local clustering (e.g., Figure 2.1; Perry et al. 1995, Perry et al. 1999). SADIE techniques are advantageous in that they deliberately distinguish between density and clustering, discounting isolated large (or small) values that can heavily influence geostatistical models (Perry et al. 1999). SADIE cluster analysis uses a transportation algorithm to find the optimal way to minimize the distance that sample locations with above-average counts (donor units) would need to distribute and partition their “surplus” to those locations with 16 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 below-average counts (receiver units), thereby equalizing counts across a sample grid into a regular arrangement. This total distance is known as the “distance to regularity”, D. Generally, larger values of D indicate greater spatial aggregation of counts. Permutations of observed counts (≈6000) among the sample locations are used to create a sampling distribution of corresponding values of D. An aggregation index, Ia, is estimated by dividing the observed value of D by the mean of the sampling distribution. This procedure was carried out for mouse and chipmunk captures to assess the overall pattern of rodent activity on trapping grids. Secondly, we used SADIE to quantify the individual contribution of each trap station to the overall spatial pattern by estimating local cluster indices () for captures of mice, chipmunks and combined (mice + chipmunks; Perry et al. 1999). Cluster indices, in part, are calculated using the same transportation algorithm as D to minimize the distance to regularity and partition outflows to below-average sample locations. By convention, donor units (i) are positive and receiver units (j) are negative. The core of the cluster index value is Ƴ , the average outflow distance from donor units weighted by the strength of the individual flows to receiver units. Thus Ƴi is formulated by: Ƴi = ∑j dij vij/∑j vij where i is a donor unit (at position xi, yi), j is a receiver unit (at position xj, yj); dij and vij are the distance and surplus amount from unit i to unit j, respectively. Additionally, is standardized by the terms oƳ, iƳ and cƳ. In brief, cƳ is the mean value of Ƴ when the count at sample unit i is followed around to all the locations it landed for each 17 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 iteration of the randomization, iƳ is similar to cƳ except that it is the mean value of Ƴ with respect to location x, y. Lastly, oƳ is the average value of cƳ over all sample units, thus it is the same value for all sample units and counts. Finally, the cluster index value for donor units (above-average counts), denoted by i, is calculated by: i = Ƴi (oƳ/iƳ cƳ) Likewise, receiver units (below-average counts), j, are similarly formulated. Values of i > 1.0 constitute members of patches and values of j < –1.0 constitute members of gaps (hereafter “hotspots” and “coldspots” of rodent activity, respectively). By convention, values of j ≤ –1.5 and i ≥ 1.5 are considered to represent significant clustering of low and high counts, respectively, where a unit was half as much again than the value expected by chance. A trap station assigned a value of j = –2.0 would indicate that captures at that location were twice that expected by chance. While somewhat arbitrary, the values of j ≤ –1.5 and i ≥ 1.5 when plotted with their corresponding randomized distributions approximate or exceed their 5th and 95th centiles, respectively (Perry et al. 1999, Winder et al. 2001). Cluster index values enclosed between contours ±1 are regions where counts are approximately the sample mean ( ≈ 0.0) or indicate random placement relative to neighboring units (Perry et al. 1999). As with D described earlier, formal randomization tests allow the estimation of p-values associated with j and i to evaluate whether observed regions of coldspots and hotspots are significant. The unique properties of the SADIE approach remove two potential biases associated with the trapping data. First, the number of trapping nights varied annually 18 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 and between grids (Schmidt et al. 2006 resolved this by calculating captures per night instead of using raw counts). The terms described above that standardize Ƴ correct for such differences (Perry et al. 1995), making grids and years directly comparable. For example, if two grids had the same relative spatial arrangement of counts but one had double the number of captures, the index values would still be equal. Also, trapping grids could experience an edge effect whereby traps located on grid boundaries compete less for rodent captures than interior traps (Schmidt et al. 2006) potentially causing edge traps to have higher captures. However, in SADIE, index values of relatively large, isolated counts are discounted so that its cluster index value would be substantially lower than an equivalent value found amid a neighborhood of high counts (Perry et al. 1999). Likewise, the same holds true for isolated low counts. Therefore, discounting relatively large values reduces a potential edge effect. Analyses of nest-site choice and nest success Cluster indices associated with trap stations provide a local measure of the magnitude of rodent activity relative to other locations on a trapping grid. Hence, the index acted as a proxy for nest-predation risk with the assumption that risk is an increasing function of local rodent activity near a nest site. Cluster indices of mice, chipmunks, and combined rodents were examined at three spatial scales around nest sites – the focal trap nearest the nest and two “nest neighborhoods”. Five-trap nest neighborhoods consisted of the focal trap plus its four nearest neighboring traps 15 m away, approximating a 225 m2 area (Figure 2.2). 19 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Nine-trap nest neighborhoods extended the five-trap neighborhood to include four additional traps at 45˚ and ≈ 21 m from the focal trap, approximating a 900 m2 area (Figure 2.2). Nests whose nearest focal trap was along a grid boundary had only three traps and six traps associated with its two neighborhood scales. For each nest, mean cluster index values of nest neighborhoods were used for all subsequent analyses. Randomization procedures (R version 3.0.0, The R Foundation for Statistical Computing) were used to determine if veeries and ovenbirds disproportionately placed nests in areas of lower rodent activity relative to the grid-specific environment in which they nested than would be predicted by chance. Nest locations were bootstrapped among trap stations, but restricted to the respective grid × year combinations in which they were observed, to create a sampling distribution of mean neighborhood cluster indices under the null hypothesis that nests were placed at random with respect to the spatial activity of mice and chipmunks. This procedure was carried out for each pairwise combination of rodent metric (mice, chipmunks and combined) and nest-site scale (focal, five- and nine-trap neighborhoods) for a total of nine tests per songbird species. Bonferroni corrections were used to adjust alpha levels for multiple comparisons to a value of 0.005 (0.05/9 tests per songbird species). All randomization tests consisted of 1000 iterations. I used all veery and ovenbird nests found within 15 m of trapping grids regardless of nest fate. Logistic regression (JMP® Pro ver. 10.0.2, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) was used to examine the influence of (1) grid-level rodent abundance, (2) rodent activity at three scales around the nest site and (3) predator species (mice, chipmunks or 20 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 combined) on ovenbird and veery nest failure. Akaike’s Information Criterion corrected for small sample size (AICc) was used to evaluate candidate models (Anderson 2008). Separate model sets were run separately for veeries and ovenbirds, each consisting of 21 candidate models. No interaction terms were included in the models. Mouse and chipmunk abundance (at grid-level) and activity metrics were also not included together in models as the combined metrics (mice + chipmunk) allowed investigation of the combined effect of the two rodent species. Likewise, models did not include more than one nest neighborhood scale (e.g., focal trap and 9-trap neighborhoods). Model strength was assessed based on multiple selection criteria including values of AICc, ∆AICc, Akaike weights () and evidence ratios. Individual variables were evaluated using 95% confidence intervals of estimates not overlapping with zero and by calculating importance values (Burnham & Anderson 2002) for single variables by summing across all models which included the respective variable. All nests categorized as fledged (veery, n=41; ovenbird, n=23) or failed (veery, n=47; ovenbird, n=28) were used to examine nest failure. Results From 1998 – 2012, we found a total of 848 veery and 554 ovenbird nests across the study site; out of this total 88 veery and 60 ovenbird nests were within ≤ 15 m of the six trapping grids (Table 2.1). Over the 15-year study, there were a total of 90 grid × year combinations in which nests could have been found of which 52 and 40 21 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 grid × year combinations had veery and ovenbird nests, respectively (68 grid × year combinations having either species’ nests). A maximum of four nests for each species were found in any given grid × year combination. Large-scale patterns of rodent activity Indices of aggregation (Ia) of mouse and chipmunk captures (counts) varied among years and grids (Figure 2.3). Mean Ia values (of the six trapping grids) indicated counts were frequently aggregated into clusters as values were consistently greater than 1.0 over the 15-year study period (Figure 2.3). Mean Ia values ranged from 1.06 – 1.36 for mice and 1.15 – 1.47 for chipmunks (Figure 2.3). Nest choice, nest success and small-scale rodent activity Randomization tests showed that for all nest neighborhoods and rodent metrics examined, veery and ovenbird nests were not placed in areas with significantly lower (all p ≥ 0.027, α = 0.005) mean cluster indices than would be predicted by chance (Table 2.2). The top ranked model investigating veery nest predation included rodent activity (mice + chipmunks) within the 9-trap nest neighborhood and rodent abundance (Table 2.3). The second highest model (∆AICc = 0.32) included only rodent abundance, having a similar Akaike weight as the highest ranked model (wi = 0.34 and 0.29, respectively). Two other models also had strong support (∆AICc ≤ 2.0) which also included metrics of combined rodent activity and abundance (Table 2.3). 22 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Lastly, the model containing mouse abundance only was modestly supported (∆AICc = 4.45, likelihood = 0.11). Rodent activity in 9-trap neighborhoods was included in two out of the top four models; however, in both cases its confidence intervals straddled zero (Table 2.4a). Rodent abundance was found in the top three models (Table 2.4a) and having much greater importance values (0.910) than the mouse abundance (0.083) variable. All other candidate models (R=16) in the veery model set were weakly supported (evidence ratios ≥ 16.15, model likelihoods ≤ 0.062, ≤ 0.021). For ovenbirds, the model consisting of chipmunk abundance and activity in the 9-trap nest neighborhood obtained the highest rank (Table 2.3), followed closely by two models – rodent activity within the 5-trap nest neighborhood plus rodent abundance (∆AICc = 0.59) and for the second, rodent abundance only (∆AICc = 0.91). Overall, the top six models had ∆AICc values < 2.0 and the top 15 obtained ∆AICc values ≤ 4.0 with evidence ratios ≤ 8.0 although Akaike weights were low for most ( < 0.10). Only two parameter estimates had confidence intervals that did not overlap zero – chipmunk activity in the 9-trap nest neighborhood and mouse abundance (Table 2.4b), with chipmunk activity having a modestly higher importance value (0.233) than mouse abundance (0.138). Discussion Spatial pattern analysis of total summer captures revealed consistent spatial heterogeneity of mouse and chipmunk activity over the 15 year study period (Figure 2.3). Hence, in most years and sites spatial heterogeneity in rodent activity is likely 23 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 available to provide leastwise predator-poor space for nesting songbirds to lower the probability of nest predation. Veeries and ovenbirds did not place nests in areas of relatively lower rodent activity than expected by chance (Table 2.2). This was true for all three spatial scales examined around their nests including the focal trap nearest the nest and the 5- and 9trap nest neighborhoods, areas approximating 225 m2 and 900 m2, respectively, around nest sites (Figure 2.2). However, results indicated that the two species differ in the factors that influence their reproductive success. Ovenbird nest success was best described by chipmunk activity within a 30 × 30 m area (9-trap neighborhood; Figure 2.2) around their nests (Tables 2.3 and 2.4b). Chipmunk abundance was also included in the top model, but importance values and confidence intervals suggest its relative influence was not as substantial as chipmunk activity (Table 2.4b). Mouse abundance also seems to be an important factor in describing ovenbird nest success, as it was the only other variable whose confidence intervals did not overlap with zero and its importance value was only modestly lower than chipmunk activity in 9-trap neighborhoods (Table 2.4b). In contrast, rodent abundance more strongly described veery nest success than spatial activity as four of the top five models included rodent abundance (Table 2.3). Additionally, total rodent abundance and mouse abundance were the only two variables with confidences intervals that did not overlap zero. However, rodent activity at the focal trap, 5-trap and 9-trap neighborhood scales were all included in the top four veery models indicating that space use by rodents at one or more of these scales may also have some influence on veery nest success. 24 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 These results partly corroborate an earlier playback experiment (Emmering & Schmidt 2011; Chapter 4 of dissertation) which provides evidence that ovenbirds eavesdrop on chipmunk calls to avoid nesting near hotspots of chipmunk activity. We had originally predicted that both ovenbirds and veeries would respond strongly to experimental playbacks of chipmunk vocalizations as shrub-nesting veeries are also vulnerable to chipmunk nest predation (Emmering & Schmidt 2011). Interestingly though, the magnitude of the veery response was only half that of ovenbirds. In the current study, while ovenbirds did not choose nest sites with lower chipmunk activity regression models did indicate that ovenbird nest success was related to chipmunk activity near their nests. Veeries also did not avoid areas of greater chipmunk or mouse activity on trapping grids. Instead, combined rodent abundance (mice + chipmunks), rather than local activity, appears to be a more important factor determining their nest success. There are several non-mutually exclusive hypotheses for the lack of a relationship between rodent activity and responses of nesting songbirds. For example, veeries and ovenbirds may be behaviorally constrained to nest within certain microhabitats (e.g., mesic areas) or nest substrates (e.g., shrubs) limiting their ability to select nest sites of lower predator activity. Alternatively, birds may show plasticity in other behavioral traits to reduce predation risk. For example, veeries may nest within denser vegetation for greater concealment (Marzluff 1988; Eggers et al. 2006) or reduce the number of feeding visits to young (Martin et al. 2000). Also, unlike obligate ground-nesting ovenbirds, veeries are not constrained to adjust the height of their nests. 25 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Over the 3-month songbird breeding season trapping grids were not continuously operated but, instead sampled usually 3 – 4 times (6 – 8 nights) per season and total captures were summed for this duration. Summing total summer captures to assess spatial activity, in particular, may be a less efficient method for estimating space use by rodents. Spatial activity of mice can vary considerably over a twelve-week period at our study site (Schauber et al. 2009) suggesting that predation risk is also not temporally homogenous across sites. Quantifying each trapping session individually could provide a more accurate (or current) estimate of rodent activity that is likely more relevant for prospecting birds when making nest-site decisions and for investigating nest success. My study and Schmidt et al. (2006) used total captures (including repeated captures of individuals), not the number of individuals captured in a trap. The rationale behind this was that individual rodents caught several times in the same trap are likely a greater threat to nests than a single individual caught only once. The assumption being that individuals caught less are not as active or exploratory and consequently use less space than individuals caught frequently. This reasoning may require further evaluation as this assumption may be inaccurate. Predator density, spatial activity, or any single proxy of predation risk may poorly predict site occupancy and/or reproductive success of prey (Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003, Schmidt & Schauber 2007) as the behavioral strategies used to reduce risk likely vary among coexisting prey. Prey that use enemy-free space can potentially decrease mortality to themselves and their offspring irrespective of overall predator densities in 26 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 the surrounding environment (Schmidt et al. 2006, Schmidt & Schauber 2007). Nest success of veeries and ovenbirds is at least partly described by measures of both rodent density and spatial activity (Table 2.3). But the predictive strength of the metrics differed for the two sympatric songbirds (Table 2.4). Veery nest success was more strongly described by the combined, grid-level abundance of mice and chipmunks whereas small-scale chipmunk activity was the better predictor of ovenbird nest success. Analysis of either measure alone might cause one to conclude that rodents are not important predators of some songbird nests and therefore fail to uncover important ecological relationships. These results highlight the need for careful selection of metrics to examine prey responses to predation risk. This could partly explain why some studies find a lack of congruence between predator abundance and nest success (Chalfoun & Schmidt 2012, Kelly et al., in review). In conclusion, these findings provide evidence that small-scale spatial variation in chipmunk activity, and not abundance, was the better predictor of ovenbird nest success. This relationship corroborates research demonstrating ovenbirds avoiding perceived concentrations of chipmunks as found in an experimental playback study (Emmering & Schmidt 2011). However, the observed differences between veeries and ovenbirds in their response to spatial variation in rodent activity suggest that they differ in vulnerability to rodent nest predators or adopt different behavioral strategies for managing predation risk. Spatial heterogeneity in rodent activity as recorded at our study site could provide breeding ovenbirds and other ground-nesting songbirds the means to cope with large pulses of rodent abundance (Ostfeld et al. 1996). Future 27 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 research into the underlying mechanisms that produce heterogeneity in rodent space use (e.g., vegetation structure) could lend greater understanding on how changes in forest communities may affect ecological interactions. 28 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Figure 2.1 Example of a trapping grid depicting local cluster indices of mice captured in 2012. SADIE analysis indicated plot counts were clustered overall (Ia = 1.38), and for both hotspots (v𝑖 = 1.30) and coldspots (v𝑗 = -1.39) of mouse activity. 29 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Figure 2.2 Diagram of an 11 × 11 trapping grid illustrating three scales at which rodent activity was examine: focal, five and nine trap neighborhoods. Adjacent traps at 90˚ and 45˚ are spaced 15 and 21 m, respectively. 30 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 (a) (b) Figure 2.3 Mean (± s.e.m.) index of aggregation (Ia) of the six trapping grids for (a) mouse and (b) chipmunk captures. Ia > 1.0 indicates an aggregation of counts; a value of Ia ≤ 1.0 or less indicates a random or regular aggregation of counts, respectively. 31 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 2.1 Sample sizes of veery and ovenbird nests from 1998 – 2012 found on each rodent trapping grid: GC, GE, HC, HE, TC and TE. 32 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 2.2 Results of nest-site selection analysis from restricted bootstrapped tests of cluster index values conducted at three spatial scales for (a) veeries and (b) ovenbirds. Nest sample sizes = n, standard deviation = SD; p-values are estimated from the left tail of bootstrapped sampling distributions. Alpha levels were adjusted using Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons to α = 0.005 33 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 2.3 Summary of logistic regression analyses examining the relationship between nest failure, rodent activity and abundance in veeries and ovenbirds, where PL = mice, TS = chipmunks; R = mice + chipmunks, abund = abundance, focal = nearest trap station, 5-trap and 9-trap = metrics of activity in nest neighborhoods, Log(L) = Log-likelihood function, K = the number of parameters, wi = Akaikie weights and ML = model likelihood. 34 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 2.4 Parameter estimates (β), standard errors (SE) and lower and upper 95% confidence intervals for parameters in the top ranked models for (a) veery and (b) ovenbird from Table 2.3. Asterisks indicate confidence intervals not overlapping zero. 35 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 CHAPTER III EXAMINING SPATIAL ACTIVITY OF MAMMALIAN PREDATORS AT SONGBIRD NEST SITES USING TRACK PLATES Introduction Spatial heterogeneity in predator activity creates refugia for prey to occupy and lower their risk of predation through reduced predator efficiency (Gause 1934, Huffaker 1958, Hilborn 1975, Hastings 1977). Spatial refugia are especially important for sessile, relatively immobile prey and vulnerable life history stages, that are not able to easily flee from predators. Instead, immobile prey may rely more on finding enemy-free space (Blaustein et al. 2004, Resetarits & Wilbur 1989, Walls 1995, Schmidt et al. 2006, Schauber et al. 2009). The eggs and young of songbirds provide a clear example of vulnerable, sessile prey anchored to a central location for a month or more. On average, predation accounts for 80% of nest failures in songbirds (Ricklefs 1969, Martin 1993, Remes et al. 2012), hence nest-site selection is closely linked to fitness and population demography (Martin 1998). Further, nest predation is thought to be principally incidental (Vickery et al. 1992, Yanes & Suarez 1996, Schmidt et al. 2001a, Schmidt et al. 2004a, Vigallon & Marzluff 2005, Schmidt et al. 2006), in that it is largely undirected and occurs when generalist predators, while foraging for primary prey, serendipitously encounter and consume less common secondary prey (Vickery et al. 36 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 1992, Schmidt et al. 2001a, McKinnon et al. 2013). If nest predation is largely incidental, spatial refugia, or perhaps more appropriately “predator-poor” space, may be created through predator behavior (Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003a, Schmidt & Schauber 2007). Examples include the economics of foraging where generalist predator activity is concentrated in low risk, high reward patches. Less profitable patches are mostly ignored, resulting in space that is periodically unoccupied by potential nest predators (Brown 1988, Schmidt et al. 2001a, Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003a). Also, predator territoriality or central-place foraging which spatially restricts or “anchors” predators to core activity centers, such as the nests of raptors (Sergio et al. 2003) or mammalian burrows and larders (Bowers 1995), leaves interstitial spaces between neighboring territories relatively predator-poor for prey to take refuge (Mech 1977, Bowers 1995, Sergio et al. 2003). Localized measures of predator activity focused around nest sites might better predict the probability of nest success instead of coarser measures of predator abundance collected at large scales, yet such studies are uncommon (Lima 2009). After all, incidental, ground-foraging predators are more likely to detect and locate nests as their proximity to the nest increases (Carthey et al. 2011) as the cues associated with nests (e.g., nestling vocalizations and scent) become more evident. Rodents, for instance, primarily search for prey via olfaction (Slotnick 2001) and mice more successfully depredate visually cryptic prey where prey cues are patchy and tightly associated with prey location (Carthey et al. 2011). Taken together, incidental encounters of nests and localization of prey cues by generalist predators 37 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 likely happens, depending upon the species, within close proximity to the nest. Therefore, nest vulnerability should be strongly correlated with local heterogeneity in predator activity (Schmidt et al. 2006) Several studies have demonstrated that birds proactively assess predation risk and subsequently adjust habitat selection and/or reproductive investment (Spaans et al. 1998, Sergio et al. 2003, Forstmeier and Weiss 2004, Roos & Pärt 2004, Morton 2005, Eggers et al. 2006, Fontaine & Martin 2006a, Fontaine & Martin 2006b, Zanette et al. 2011, further reviewed in Lima 2009). Birds may assess risk based on a single species of predator that disproportionately depredates nests due to its numerical dominance (e.g., Schmidt et al. 2001a, Forstmeier & Weiss 2004). Alternatively, prey may simultaneously monitor and evaluate risk based on the combined activity of several potential predators to make optimal breeding decisions (Fontaine & Martin 2006a, Fontaine & Martin 2006b, Zanette et al. 2011). Which predators birds monitor probably depends on a variety of factors including relative predator abundances, previous predation by a specific species (Jackson et al. 1989, Lima 2009), and nest type-location (e.g., open cup-shrub, enclosed dome-ground; Fontaine et al. 2007), to name just a few. Determining which species are significant predators of nests within an ecosystem has clear benefits for wildlife-management strategies and understanding avian population demography (Benson et al. 2010, Ellis-Feleg et al. 2012). Research from our study site has demonstrated a strong relationship between veery (Catharus fuscescens) nest success and mouse activity surrounding nest sites (~15 m radius), and furthermore, that veeries may preferentially select sites of below38 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 average mouse activity (Schmidt et al. 2006). Chipmunks, however, might only weakly affect the reproductive success of open-cup nesters like veeries (Schmidt et al. 2001b). But the opposite may be true for ovenbirds. Emmering and Schmidt (2011; Chapter 4 of this dissertation) recently found that ovenbirds and veeries placed nests significantly further from playbacks broadcasting chipmunk vocalizations than sites broadcasting frog calls or silent controls. However, the magnitude of the ovenbird distance response was twice that of the veery response as ovenbirds placed nests 20 m further from chipmunk playbacks than controls. Although other results have found that combined rodent density (mice + chipmunks) had little effect on ovenbird daily mortality (Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003a, Kelly et al., in review). These results suggest that veeries may be less susceptible to nest predation by chipmunks but for ovenbirds the opposite may be true, though this remains somewhat unclear. Analysis of rodent activity within the nest neighborhood of ovenbirds has yet to be examined as earlier research efforts focused more on veeries, and as a consequence, insufficient samples of nests were found to examine these relationships (Schmidt et al. 2006). Study objectives were to determine (1) which specific ground-foraging species are important nest predators, (2) if predator activity is related to nest fate, (3) do nest sites have lower predator activity than nearby random sites, and lastly (4) if predator species and activity differ between two coexisting, ground-nesting passerines, ovenbirds and veeries. To accomplish this, I present a new approach to quantify predation risk surrounding nest sites using track plates (Connors et al. 2005). Track plates are a highly portable and economical alternative to expensive video systems 39 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 (Ribic et al. 2012). By recording an animal’s tracks, plates can record the activity of multiple individuals and species in space, including larger mesopredators. Further, predator species can be identified based on their species-specific footprints. Two main predictions were formed based on previous research. First, to reduce the risk of nest predation ovenbirds and veeries would construct nests in locations with lower predator activity than randomly chosen sites within their territories (≤ 50 m). And second, nest sites that successfully fledged young would have less nest-predator activity than nest sites that were depredated. Lastly, I conducted a heterospecific comparison to explore if predator activity surrounding ovenbird and veery nest sites differed. Ovenbirds and veeries both place their nests on or near the ground (veeries ≤ 1.5 m in shrubs; Heckscher 2004, Bevier et al. 2005) so presumably both species’ nests are similar in their accessibility to ground-foraging predators. However, heterospecific differences in nest-site microhabitats preferences (Martin 1995), construction and how parent birds manage risk at the nest site (Martin et al. 2000, Eggers et al. 2008) could influence their respective predation risk and where they place their nests. Based on evidence from our study site (described above), I predicted ovenbird nests sites would have fewer chipmunks than nest sites of veeries. Methods The study was conducted on the property of the Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies (41º 50´ N, 73º 45´ W) located in Dutchess County, Millbrook, New York, USA from 2006 – 2008 during the songbird breeding season (May – August). The 40 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 property consists of ~325 ha of continuous eastern deciduous forest with a oakdominated canopy (Quercus rubra and Q. prinus). Common understory trees and shrubs include oak and maple (Acer spp.) saplings, witch hazel (Hamamelis virginiana) as well as nonnative honeysuckle (Lonicera spp.) and Japanese barberry (Berberis thunbergii). Nest-predation risk was assumed to be an increasing function of predator spatial activity. Conceptually, the degree of predator activity recorded in an area may be similar where either an individual mouse exhibited high spatial activity or where multiple mice were only moderately active (Schauber et al. 2009). Either case could result in an equal number of track plates being tracked. In addition, we also assumed nest predation is more commonly the result of undirected, incidental predation (described above; Vickery et al. 1992, Yanes & Suarez 1996, Schmidt et al. 2004a). Incidental predators were thus not expected to direct their foraging activities or shift their territories to areas containing a nest. Track plates were used to quantify nest-predator activity. Several track plates placed in an area can be used to record the presence/absence of specific grounddwelling species and can therefore be used to assess their spatial activity. Track plates are advantageous compared to live traps as traps may be less effective for estimating small-scale activity of rodents. Traps restrict an animal’s movements for extended periods of time and can affect the behavior of individuals by attracting them with bait (i.e., “trap happy”) or making them “trap shy” after initial capture. Furthermore, track plates (1) are highly portable, (2) allow predator identity to species by recording 41 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 species-specific footprints (3) record the activity of multiple individuals and predator species, including mesopredators too large for traps and (4) are economical compared to video-monitoring systems that have been used in other nest predation studies (e.g., Ribic et al. 2012). Track plates have also been successfully used at our study site by other researchers to estimate rodent space use in investigations on the effects of mouse activity on gypsy moth pupa survival (Connors et al. 2005, Schauber et al. 2009) and the effects on space use by immunochallenged mice (Schwanz et al. 2011). Individual track plates consisted of a 14 22 cm acetate sheet (fastened to aluminum sheeting with two paper clips for support), which was coated with a waterresistance solution of graphite, alcohol and mineral oil that provides a dry surface for recording footprints (Connors et al. 2005, Figures 3.1 – 3.3). However, a few nests (< 5) had only 8 track plates due to obstacles within 15 m of the nest such as standing water or roads. Spacing between a track plate’s nearest plate was ~7.6 m and ~11.8 m for 5-m and 15-m plates, respectively. Arrays of track plates were setup around nest sites and at randomly chosen control sites where no nests were found. Track plates were evenly distributed as an array of 12 plates encircling nest and paired control sites with four plates placed at 5 m and eight plates placed at 15 m from the nest (Figure 3.4). Control sites were placed 50 m away from each nest using a random compass bearing chosen a priori and centered on an area or nesting substrate typical of ovenbirds or veeries (e.g., multistemmed shrub). To ensure other conspecific nests were not near focal control sites we intensively searched for nests in the area during track plate set up but also 42 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 throughout the field season. If another conspecific’s nest was found, a new random compass bearing was selected for the control site. Some paired control sites may not have fallen within the same territory as its paired nest site. For veeries, mean territory size based on radio-tracking males at our study site was 1.80 ha (≈ 75 m radius; Belinsky, K.L. unpublished data). Assuming veery territories are roughly circular and nests are centrally located, control sites placed 50 m away may still fall within the territory of the paired nest site. Ovenbird territories, though never measured at our study site, are typically smaller and range from 0.13 – 0.38 ha (Smith & Shugart 1987) which could have a radius of ≈ 20 – 35 m. So for ovenbirds, paired control sites were likely outside of the territory in which the nest was located. Track plates without acetate sheets (aluminum backing only) were set out at least three days prior to data collection to allow animals to habituate to the plates. Acetate sheets were then added to the aluminum backing and inspected every other day for six consecutive days for a total of three checks (or 36 total plate-checks/nest). Individual plates were scored as “tracked” or “not-tracked” for each of the three checks and for each species which included white-footed mouse (Peromyscus leucopus), eastern chipmunk (Tamias striatus), gray squirrel (Sciurus niger), red squirrel (Tamiasciurus hudsonicus), raccoon (Procyon lotor), opossum (Didelphis virginiana) or other less common mesopredators such as foxes (e.g., Vulpes vulpes; Connors et al. 2005). Acetate sheets were replaced when ≥ 25% of the sheet was removed by tracks or rain. Significant rainfall events would usually remove the graphite coating from a large number of acetate sheets in which case track data was 43 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 not recorded on the scheduled check date. Instead acetate sheets were replaced to be checked again two days later. Occasionally, individual track plates were scored as “unreadable” due to rain, plates being flipped or acetate sheets accidently affixed upside down. Over the three years of the study, track plates were placed around 48 ovenbird and 59 veery nests (Table 3.2). I compared nest predator activity between: (1) paired nest and control sites, (2) fledged and depredated nests and (3) ovenbird and veery nest sites (hereafter, heterospecific analysis). Nests were considered fledged if they successfully fledged at least one nestling. Only nests confirmed as depredated or fledged were used in the fate analysis. For the heterospecific analysis, I pooled all nests, regardless of their fate, but excluded any nests that only had eight track plates; however, nests with only eight plates were used in the paired-site and fate analysis. Seven metrics were examined based on different predator assemblages (Table 3.1). The first metric summed the total number of predator species recorded per plate to assess predator richness around a nest site in which all ground-dwelling predators were included (listed above). For instance, if a single plate had tracks from both mice and chipmunks it was scored a ‘2’, if it had tracks from three nest predator species it was scored a ‘3’, and so on. This metric was subdivided into three additional metrics composed of smaller predator guilds (Table 3.1). The final three metrics only estimated presence/absence of mice, chipmunks and all predator species by calculating the proportion of plates that were scored as tracked or not tracked, ‘1’ or ‘0’, 44 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 respectively. All metrics were calculated based on 36 plate checks (12 plates × 3 check days). Statistical analysis Summed track plate scores over the three checks were used to calculate mean metric values for each site (nest, control) and used for all subsequent analysis. Mean scores were used instead of summed scores per site as the number of plates occasionally varied per site (see methods). Control site values were not used in the fate or heterospecific analyses. All statistical analyses were performed using R version 3.0.0 (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing). To test if paired nest and control sites differed significantly in nest predator activity, paired t-tests were conducted for each metric (Table 3.1). The paired t-test was well-suited for this experimental design not only because of the paired association between nest and control sites but additionally it does not assume groups are normally distributed with equal variances (Zar 1999). Rather the paired t-test assumes that the differences between groups are normally distributed (Zar 1999). Visual inspection of normal Q-Q plots and Shapiro-Wilk normality tests confirmed that control and nest site differences for veeries were normally distributed with the exception of the mesopredator metric, which I thereafter excluded from the site analysis. For the ovenbird dataset, site differences for three metrics (mice + chipmunks, mice and presence/absence) were not normally distributed so they were normalized using arcsine-square root transformations. For the six 45 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 comparisons, a Bonferroni correction was used to adjust an alpha level of 0.05 to 0.008. For the fate and heterospecific analyses, permutations were used to generate sampling distributions of the test statistic under the null hypothesis that groups (i.e., dead vs. fledged or ovenbird vs. veery) were no different than would be expected if mean metric values were randomly assigned to different groups. The absolute difference between group means, DIFsim, was used as the test statistic to contrast with the observed absolute difference between groups (DIFobs). P-values were estimated by comparing DIFobs with the tails of the bootstrapped distribution by dividing all values of |DIFsim| ≥ |DIFobs| by the number of bootstrapped iterations (e.g., Figure 3.5). Alpha levels were adjusted for the seven comparisons using a Bonferroni correction to adjust alpha to 0.007. Bootstrapping of metric values was handled differently for the fate and heterospecific analysis. In the fate analysis, values were bootstrapped (1000 iterations) between groups (depredated and fledged) but constrained within years to obtain a mean group difference for each respective year that were then averaged to compute a single value of DIFsim. The heterospecific analysis did not restrict randomizations but instead shuffled metric values between groups and years (100,000 iterations). Lastly, only nests from 2006 and 2007 were used for the heterospecific analysis as track plates were not placed around veery nests in 2008. 46 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Results Nest and control sites Nest-predator activity was consistently higher at ovenbird nests than their paired control sites (Table 3.3), but all metrics were nonsignificant (t48 ≥ -2.230, p ≥ 0.030, α = 0.008; Table 3.3). Likewise, predator activity also did not differ significantly between veery nests and their paired control sites for all of the metrics examined (-1.575 ≤ t58 ≤ -0.346, p ≥ 0.113; Table 3.3). Nest fate Predator activity metrics did not differ significantly between depredated and fledged ovenbird nests (all metrics, p ≥ 0.030, α = 0.007; Table 3.4). Depredated veery nests had, with the exception of the mesopredator metric, consistently higher predator activity than fledged nests, however, these differences did not differ significantly (all metrics, p ≥ 0.252; Table 3.4). Heterospecific comparison The proportion of track plates recording nest predators (i.e., presence/absence metric) and chipmunks was 10% greater around veery nests than ovenbird nests (Table 3.5) and this difference was significant for both comparisons (p < 0.0001, α = 0.007). However, the five remaining metrics were not significantly different between veery and ovenbird nests (p ≥ 0.011, α = 0.007). 47 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Discussion Analysis of track plate results found no relationship between predator activity and nest success, for both songbird species (Table 3.4). Results also suggest that veeries and ovenbirds do not select nest sites with lower nest predator activity compared to unused sites near or within their territories. Though not significant, results showed just the opposite from our predictions – nest predator activity was higher around nest sites. Ovenbird nest sites had higher total predator activity than random sites and this difference was likely driven by rodents as only the all predator and all rodent metrics were higher (Table 3.3). For veeries, track plate results were similar to findings that analyzed mouse and chipmunk captures from permanent trapping grids over the course of the songbird breeding season (Chapter 2). Both studies failed to find a relationship between veery nest-site selection, nest success and small-scale rodent activity (Tables 2.2 – 2.4). Likewise, analyses of trapping data found ovenbirds also did not place nests in areas of lower rodent activity (Table 2.2), but differently ovenbird nest success was related to chipmunk activity within a 30 × 30 m area around their nests (Tables 2.3 – 2.4). Track plates and trapping grids differed markedly in the temporal scale at which sampling occurred (7 days vs. 3 months, respectively) which may partly explain why study results differed. At least at the spatial scale and metrics we examined, our findings suggest that veeries and ovenbirds do not select nest locations based on the activity of several predator species to reduce the risk of nest predation or that there is a relationship with 48 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 local activity and reproductive success. Several non-mutually exclusive hypotheses could explain our results. One alternative is that the species we examined (Table 3.1) are not important predators of veery or ovenbird nests. Certainly, some of the predators investigated may not be as significant as others, but there is good evidence from our study site (including unpublished video evidence) and other systems that mice (Reitsma et al. 1990, Williams and Wood 2002, Cain et al. 2003, Clotfelter et al. 2007, Schmidt and Ostfeld 2008, Cox et al. 2012), chipmunks (Cain et al. 2003, King and DeGraaf 2006, Schmidt et al. 2008, Haché et al. 2014), squirrels (Reitsma et al. 1990, Cain et al. 2003, Haché et al. 2014) and mesopredators (Crooks & Soulé 1999, Schmidt 2003, Rodewald & Kearns 2011, Ellis-Feleg et al. 2012) all regularly depredate nests. Spatial refugia may be less effective at preventing predation from highly mobile avian nest predators like birds of prey which could partly explain the lack of a relationship between mammal activity and nest success. In years of low rodent abundance, birds of prey (e.g., barred owls, Strix varia, broad-winged hawks, Buteo platypterus) may behaviorally respond by switching to alternative prey including songbird nests and fledglings (Jędrzejewska & Jędrzejewski 1998, Selås 2001, Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003, Schmidt et al. 2008). Over the 3-year study duration, rodent densities (mice and chipmunks) fluctuated dramatically which included a 15-year high in 2007 of 83.4 animals ha-1 to 22.5 animals ha-1 in 2008, nearly a four-fold decrease. Crash years, as seen in 2008, could result in higher nest predation by avian predators to compensate for now sparse rodent prey (Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003, Schmidt et al. 49 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 2008). Whether nesting songbirds are able to assess and respond appropriately to such changes in the predator community remains unresolved. Another explanation for observed similarities in predator activity between sites could be that predator activity varied little at the scale of bird territories. If the scale at which predator activity is spatially autocorrelated exceeds that of the territory scale then territory-level heterogeneity would be low, implying that refugia may not exist. Leastwise, prospecting birds might be limited in the number of spatial refugia available for nesting or in their ability to perceive minor variation in activity. Research by Schauber et al. (2009) at our study site suggests this might be the case. They found that the spatial scale of autocorrelation in mouse activity was frequently larger than the scale at which we placed paired track plate arrays or songbird territories (range 44 – 1000 m; Schauber et al. 2009). Whether the activity of other nest predators is spatially autocorrelated at similar scales is unknown. These results also raise the possibility as to whether the distribution of groundforaging birds and generalist predators are actually spatially associated. White-footed mouse and chipmunk diets during the spring and summer months consist largely of arthropod prey (Hamilton 1941, Batzli 1977, Synder 1982, Lackey 1985, Wolff et al. 1985) as are the diets of veeries and ovenbirds especially during the breeding season. Food abundance is considered one of the principle determinants of territory quality for breeding songbirds (Rodenhouse et al. 1997) and is particularly well documented for ovenbirds (e.g., Smith & Shugart 1987, Burke & Nol 1998). Therefore groundforaging rodents and songbirds may have similar site preferences based on shared prey 50 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 causing their densities to be spatially correlated. In such case, settling birds might demand what Brown & Kotler (2004) describe as “hazardous duty pay” where settling birds would require greater food resources to compensate for the increased risk of nest predation. Alternatively, birds may simply prioritize safety ahead of food abundance biasing their selection of territories towards those with fewer predators. Durrant (1998) discovered a similar case where cheetahs were more often found in sites of lower food abundance to avoid their competitors and also intra-guild predators – hyenas and lions. Or parent birds may manage risk using other behavioral strategies including reduced parental activity at the nest (Martin & Ghalambor 1999, Martin et al. 2000), defensive strategies (Caro 2005, Ellison & Ribic 2012, Gloag et al. 2013) or nest-site characteristics (Martin 1998, Marzluff 1988, Eggers et al. 2006, Stevens 2013, Lovell et al. 2013). Predicted heterospecific differences in predator activity were partly supported. Predator activity was greater at veery compared to ovenbird nest sites and this difference appeared to be chiefly caused by chipmunks (Table 3.5), which is consistent with results from an experimental playback study conducted at our study site (Emmering & Schmidt 2011; Chapter 4 of dissertation). In response to playbacks of chipmunk vocalizations that simulated “hotspots” of chipmunk activity, ovenbirds and veeries placed nests significantly further from plots broadcasting chipmunk calls compared to control plots that broadcasted frog calls or were silent. However, ovenbirds nested at twice the distance of veeries (20 m vs. 10 m, respectively) from chipmunk playbacks. This differential aversion to perceived chipmunk activity could 51 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 partly explain the large difference in chipmunk activity recorded around their respective nest sites and together suggests that ovenbird nests may be more vulnerable to predation by chipmunks. In addition, microhabitat nest-site preferences could explain the greater chipmunk activity around veery nest sites. Veeries frequently nest in shrubs such as barberry and honeysuckle both of which can form larger patches which may provide food (i.e., berries) or protective cover from predators for chipmunks (e.g., Forstmeier & Weiss 2004) or other nest predators (Dutra et al. 2011). Ovenbirds differ in that they are obligate ground-nesters, building dome-shaped nests that are frequently placed in locations with sparse to no cover (Van Horn & Donovan 1994). Since veeries experience greater predation from mice (Schmidt et al. 2001a, Schmidt & Ostfeld 2003a), we predicted mouse activity would be greater at their nests. This prediction was not supported as mouse activity was very similar around the two species nest sites (Table 3.5). 52 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 3.1 Description of track plate metrics. For each metric, mean counts were calculated from 36 total plate checks (12 plates × 3 checks) for each nest. Some species, like red fox (Vulpes vulpes), were very uncommon and thus were grouped into a single mesopredator guild; unknown mesopredator species were also included within the mesopredator guild. Metric Description Species included Summation of all nest predator species that tracked a All predators plate, e.g., if a plate was tracked by a mouse, chipmunk and raccoon, the plate was scored a '3' White-footed mouse, eastern chipmunk, gray squirrel, red squirrel, raccoon, opossum and other mesopredators* White-footed mouse, eastern All rodents Summation of all rodent species that tracked a plate chipmunk, gray squirrel and red squirrel Mice and chipmunks Mesopredators Mice Chipmunks Summation of mice and chipmunks Summation of mesopredators Presence/absence of a track left by Peromyscus spp. only Presence/absence of a track left by Tamias striatus only 53 White-footed mouse and eastern chipmunk Raccoon, opossum, fox, coyote and mustelids White-footed mouse only Eastern chipmunk only Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 White-footed mouse, eastern Presence/absence Presence/absence, scored as ‘1’ or ‘0’ respectively, of a chipmunk, gray squirrel, red squirrel, track left by any nest predator under consideration raccoon, opossum and other mesopredators 54 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Figure 3.1 Image illustrating the application of Figure 3.2 Image demonstrating the water resistance of a water-resistant solution to acetate sheet. track plate coated with graphite solution. 55 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 (a) (b) (c) Figure 3.3 Examples of footprints left on track plates by one or more of the following: (a) mice, (b) raccoons and (c) chipmunks. Track plates measured 14 22 cm while the area covered by graphite was slightly less by 1-2 cm. 56 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 TRACK PLATE ARRAY 10m 5m nest 15m unk tracks Figure 3.4 Illustration of track plate array encircling a nest. Control sites were arranged similarly. Plates were placed at 5 m and 15 m from the Arrays –nest Atornest & random control controlsites site center. Plate dimensions were approximately 14 22 cm and hence are not to scale in figure. sites ~50m distance from nest. 57 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 All rodent species Figure 3.5 Histogram displaying the bootstrapped sampling distribution of the absolute difference between depredated and fledged veery nests (DIFsim) from 1000 bootstrapped iterations. The dotted red line represents the observed mean difference, DIFobs. All values of DIFsim ≥ the value of DIFobs constitute the estimated p-value of 0.621 (i.e.,, 621/1000 iterations). 58 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 3.2 Sample sizes of nests by year and reproductive fate for each analysis conducted. Note each nest was also paired with second track plate array (i.e., control site) ~50 m away. 59 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 3.3 Results of nest-site analysis from paired t tests to compare means of nest and control sites. Nests were pooled from all three years of the study to report observed means of each metric. Alpha levels were adjusted using Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons to α = 0.008. Refer to Table 3.1 for detailed descriptions of each 60 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 3.4 Results of fate analysis from bootstrapped tests of the mean difference between dead and fledged nests. Nests were pooled from all three years of the study to calculate observed means of each metric. DIFobs is the observed difference of mean dead and fledged nests. Alpha levels were adjusted using Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons to α = 0.007. 61 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 3.5 Results of heterospecific analysis from bootstrapped tests of the mean difference between veery and ovenbird nests. Means are of all nests for 2006 and 2007 combined. DIFobs is the observed difference of the mean between veery and ovenbird. Alpha levels were adjusted using Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons to α = 0.007. Refer to Table 3.1 for 62 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 CHAPTER IV NESTING SONGBIRDS ASSESS SPATIAL HETEROGENEITY OF PREDATORY CHIPMUNKS BY EAVESDROPPING ON THEIR VOCALIZATIONS Introduction Environmental heterogeneity creates uncertainty for organisms that limits their ability to select behavioral strategies appropriate for the current (or anticipated future) conditions. Information reduces uncertainty (Dall et al. 2004); hence acquiring information is critical for optimal decision-making. Information on predation risk is particularly important for organisms selecting breeding sites, as safety from predators is an important component of site quality (Martin 1995; Blaustein et al. 2004; Forstmeier & Weiss 2004; Rodenhouse et al. 2003). Thus organisms should attend to information which enables them to select sites with a low probability of predation. A rapidly growing body of evidence has documented that birds use a multitude of information sources when selecting breeding habitats and territories (Viitala et al. 1995; Part & Doligez 2003; Safran 2004; Betts et al. 2008; Hromada et al. 2008; Forsman & Martin 2009; Lima 2009). Sources include pre-breeding cues such as the presence of conspecifics (Fletcher 2007) and post-breeding cues from prior years, such as personal reproductive success (Haas 1998) or the presence of fledglings (Betts et al. 2008). Conspecific presence may be useful for finding appropriate habitat patches, 63 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 but less so for estimating small-scale spatial heterogeneity necessary for selecting individual territories or nest-sites (e.g., Morton 2005). Likewise, performance-based cues, such as personal or conspecific reproductive success, can be useful as integrated measures of territory quality; however, they require high temporal correlation between years to accurately forecast future success (Safran 2004). Thus in environments with low annual predictability, for example, in pulsed-resource systems (McShea 2000; Ostfeld & Keesing 2000; Clotfelter et al. 2007; Schmidt & Ostfeld 2008), birds should place a premium on using current information, when possible, to locate refugia from nest predators. However, species differences in life histories, sensory ecology, and/or habitat constraints may result in interspecific differences in information use. Although previous studies have documented that birds make breeding habitat adjustments in response to predation risk (e.g., Marzluff 1988; Sergio, Marchesi & Pedrini 2003; Forstmeier & Weiss 2004; Fontaine & Martin 2006; Schmidt, Ostfeld & Smyth 2006; Peluc et al. 2008; Lima 2009), only a handful have identified the source(s) of information that they use (e.g., Eggers et al. 2006; Forsman & Martin 2009; Mönkkönen et al. 2009). Prey should be sensitive to direct cues of risk (i.e.,, predator activity) given by predators themselves. However, experimental manipulation of individual cues are uncommon, and while predator removals can demonstrate the prey’s behavioral response they do not determine which individual cues are used to assess to risk. We suggest prey may frequently exploit the acoustic signals of predators acquired through ‘eavesdropping’, the process where unintended receivers intercept the signals of others to acquire information (Peake 2005). 64 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Communication is often clearly audible and in the public domain thereby reducing the time and acquisition costs of directly assessing predators to facilitate avoidance. Our work on ground-nesting passerines has previously demonstrated (1) rodent nest predators, including white-footed mice (Peromyscus leucopus) and eastern chipmunks (Tamias striatus), have significant spatial heterogeneity in activity (Schmidt, Ostfeld & Smyth 2006; Goodwin et al. 2005); (2) there is a strong negative correlation between veery (Catharus fuscescens) fledging success and rodent activity surrounding nest sites (Schmidt, Ostfeld & Smyth 2006; Schmidt & Schauber 2007); and (3) veeries build nests at sites with below average rodent activity (Schmidt, Ostfeld & Smyth 2006), suggesting that they acquire and use information associated with rodent activity to select nest sites with a lower risk of nest predation. Because annual fluctuations in rodent abundance can be of two orders of magnitude (Ostfeld, Jones & Wolff 1996; Jones et al. 1998; Schmidt & Ostfeld 2008), the predictability of spatial refugia for nesting passerines based on prior success may be limited. This suggests birds may place a premium on information available during the current breeding period to assess spatial heterogeneity in nest-predation risk. Chipmunks are vociferous throughout the breeding season, and their frequent calling bouts with neighboring conspecifics may provide a proximate cue of predation risk for nesting passerines to assess and avoid “hotspots” of chipmunk activity. As chipmunks will depredate eggs, nestlings and even fledgling birds (Schmidt, Rush & Ostfeld 2008), nest-site and territorial adjustments in response to acoustic cues emitted by chipmunks may substantially lower the risk of nest and juvenile predation. Such behavior may be 65 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 paramount for coping with high densities of nest predators that occur in the wake of masting seed crops (McShea 2000; Schmidt, Ostfeld & Smyth 2006, Schmidt & Ostfeld 2008). To test whether passerines eavesdrop on chipmunks as a means to assess local chipmunk activity and consequently avoid areas of higher perceived nest predation risk, we conducted an experimental playback study to create spatial heterogeneity in perceived risk. We used three types of playbacks at the beginning of the breeding season: (1) chipmunk vocalizations (increased nest predation risk), (2) gray tree frog calls (Hyla versicolor; low-risk control) and (3) no playback (silent control). We examined two potential responses to experimental playbacks: (1) nest-site placement in two ground-nesting passerines, veeries and ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla) and (2) bird activity in two guilds of nesting birds, ground and canopy. We predicted veeries and ovenbirds would nest further away from chipmunk playback plots versus control plots, and the presence or activity of ground-nesters would be reduced near chipmunk playback plots relative to control plots, whereas canopy-nesters would show no change in activity. Methods General procedures Our experiment was conducted in oak-dominated, eastern deciduous forest on the property of the Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies located in Dutchess County, Millbrook, New York, USA. In spring 2006 we established 32 experimental plots: 16 66 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 broadcasting chipmunk vocalizations and 16 silent control plots. Each playback plot consisted of 3 CD/speaker stations (T100-CD Trutech, USA; 40-1441 speakers, Radio Shack, Fort Worth, TX, USA) housed in small plastic containers (13 17 34 cm) to protect the equipment from the elements. Playback stations were equally spaced 30 m apart forming an equilateral triangle and faced inwards. Control plots were similarly arranged; however no playback equipment was present and vertices were marked with a single small flag. All plots (playback and control) were separated by at least 200 m, twice the maximum distance at which we measured nest locations, to avoid pseudoreplication of nests found on experimental plots. Playbacks began May 7 prior to territory establishment (the first nest under construction was found on May 12 and May 17 in 2006 and 2007, respectively) of focal migrant songbirds through peak settlement (after June 15) and ran daily for the approximate six week experiment in each study year. Each speaker intermittently broadcasted chipmunk vocalizations [8085 dB SPL (re. 20µPa) at 5 m] for ~12 min/hr beginning in early morning when fresh batteries were added (~0630-0900) until ~1600 when batteries were depleted. Playbacks were halted on days of heavy rain (2-3 days/year). Chipmunk and frog playbacks were produced from recordings of multiple individuals at the site using a Marantz PMD-670 field recorder (Middlesex, UK) connected to a Sennheiser ME-62 microphone (Buckinghamshire, UK) and Telinga parabolic disc (Tobo, Sweden). Chipmunk vocalizations included ‘chips’, ‘chucks’ and ‘trills’ (Dunford 1970; Elliot 1978) from ~12 wild individuals (plus 8 additional individuals added in 2007). Recordings were compiled and edited using Raven Pro 67 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 1.3 (Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA) to remove background noise and untargeted vocalizations. Final chipmunk and frog playbacks thus contained several call types from multiple individuals. We amended the experimental design in 2007 to include a non-silent, procedural control using gray tree frog calls. Gray tree frogs are common at the site and unlikely to elicit either heterospecific attraction or repulsion, which is a risk if using avian vocalizations (Fletcher 2008). To accommodate the frog treatment in 2007 we added 8 plots increasing the total to 40. In addition, we rotated treatment types between years. The 16 silent control plots used in 2006 were switched to chipmunk playbacks, whereas 12 of the 2006 chipmunk plots played frog calls. The remaining 4 chipmunk plots used in 2006 and the 8 new plots were used as silent controls in 2007. In 2007 we also added a regular rotation (every 3 days) of 3 unique chipmunk exemplars each playing a different assortment of chipmunk calls. Otherwise, all other previously described methods used in 2006 were continued in 2007. Nest-site selection We searched for nests within a 100 m radius of all stations. Search efforts focused only on veeries and ovenbirds whose nests are vulnerable to chipmunk predation. Ovenbirds nest exclusively on the ground and veeries directly on or close to the ground in shrubs (≤ 1 m). After nests became inactive, we measured the distance of each nest to the nearest playback or silent station (i.e.,, flagged vertex) 68 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 with a measuring tape. For analyses, we included only those nests that had been built ≤ 2 days following termination of the playbacks. Point counts To examine if adult birds had reduced activity in areas perceived as risky (i.e.,, chipmunk playback plots), we conducted 3 replicate, 5-min point counts (Hutto, Pletschet, & Hendricks 1986) from the center of all study plots. We recorded all migrant species that bred on site grouping them into two nesting guilds: a groundshrub-nesting (hereafter, ground-nesting guild) and a canopy nesting guild. While canopy nests may still be vulnerable to predation by chipmunks, they are less likely to be encountered by chipmunks that bias the majority of their activities on the ground; hence canopy-nesters were considered at relatively lower risk than ground-nesters. The ground-nesting guild included ground-shrub-nesting veery and obligate groundnesting ovenbird, worm-eating warbler (Helmitheros vermivorus), and black-andwhite warbler (Mniotilta varia). The canopy-nesting guild consisted of yellow-billed cuckoo (Coccyzus americanus), black-billed cuckoo (Coccyzus erythropthalmus), blue-gray gnatcatcher (Polioptila caerulea), eastern wood-pewee (Contopus virens), great-crested flycatcher (Myiarchus crinitus), red-eyed vireo (Vireo olivaceus), yellow-throated vireo (Vireo flavifrons), black-throated green warbler (Dendroica virens), scarlet tanager (Piranga olivacea), rose-breasted grosbeak (Pheucticus ludovicianus), and Baltimore oriole (Icterus galbula). The first count was conducted 7 days (2006) and 14 days (2007) after starting the experiment; thereafter each replicate 69 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 point count was separated by 7-10 days over the course of the experiment. Counts were performed between 0500-0900 on days without inclement weather (rain, fog, or steady breeze). After arrival to the plot, counts were delayed 5 minutes before initiating to allow birds to resume normal activities. During the count we recorded all birds detected by sight or sound within 50 m from the center of plots. Each individual detected was placed within one of 5 distance categories: 0-10 m, 10-20 m, 20-30 m, 30-40 m, and 40-50 m. If, however, an estimated distance was difficult to assign we divided the sighting (assigned ½) between the two adjacent categories. Playbacks were turned on after the completion of the point count. Assessment of mammalian nest-predator activity using track plates To determine if chipmunks or other mammalian nest predators [gray squirrels (Sciurus niger), white-footed mice and raccoons (Procyon lotor)] responded to our playbacks, we analyzed track plates that recorded predator activity in bird territories from a concurrent study (Chapter 3 of dissertation). Track plates consisted of a 14 22 cm acetate sheet (fastened to aluminum sheeting for support) coated with a waterresistance graphite/alcohol/oil solution to record activity via footprints (Connors et al. 2005). Track plates were distributed as an array of 12 plates (4 at 5m, 8 at 15m) encircling veery and ovenbird nests on chipmunk plots (n=29 nests) and control plots (n=34 nests). Arrays of aluminum backing were first set out at least 3 days prior to data collection to allow animals to habituate to the plates. Acetate sheets were then added to aluminum backing and inspected every other day for 6 consecutive days for a 70 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 total of 3 checks. Plates were scored as “tracked” or “not-tracked” by individual species (see Connors et al. 2005). We calculated the proportion of 36 plates (3 checks 12 plates) tracked by each species over the 6-day period for each array of plates. Track plate data were analyzed using ANOVA with the arcsine-transformed proportion of plates tracked as the dependent variable and year and playback treatment (chipmunk, control) as independent variables. We found no difference in the proportion of track plates by treatment marked by chipmunks (F1,60 = 0.232, p = 0.632), mice (F1,60 = 0.004, p = 0.951), squirrels (F1,60 = 1.425, p = 0.237), or raccoons (F1,60 = 0.416, p = 0.521), suggesting that movements of chipmunks and other potential nest predators were unaffected by playbacks. Detections of other mammals (e.g., opossums, Didelphis virginiana) were too infrequent to analyze. Statistical analysis Nest distance data were analyzed using ANOVA (SYSTAT 12.02.00, Chicago, IL, USA) with distance as the dependent variable; independent variables included playback treatment, year, and species (veery, ovenbird) and all 2- and 3-way interaction terms. For point counts, we estimated the mean number of birds recorded for the 3 replicate counts on each experimental plot. We hypothesized that birds were more likely to reduce their activity nearer than farther from chipmunk playback stations. Shifting activities (or territories) only a short distance away, for example 2030 m, may not seem substantial, however given the scale of chipmunk territories (0.03-0.40 ha; Snyder 1982) this distance would likely be sufficient to lower predation 71 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 risk. For analysis, we used a hierarchical approach first using Akaike’s information criterion (AICc, corrected for small sample sizes) to determine if bird presence/activity based on point count detections changed over distance between treatments, and furthermore, what distance (comparing 10 m increments) was most parsimonious with our data. AICc scores were based on mixed effects general linear models (GLM) with year, distance (near vs. far), treatment and their interactions as fixed effects, and site (i.e.,, plot) as a random effect. For each nesting guild we compared five models. Four models uniquely categorized near and far distances (0-10 vs. 10-50 m, 0-20 vs. 20-50 m, etc.), and a fifth model had no distance term (0-50 m). We identified the most parsimonious model by the lowest AICc score, and used that model to assess statistical significance of the fixed and random effects specified above. Prior to analyses we examined the 2007 data to corroborate that procedural activities did not affect bird responses and pooled the two control (silent and frog) treatments. To do this, we compared nest distance and point count data across the two controls using two-tailed t-tests. Nest distances were nearly identical between the two control treatments (difference in mean distances between frog and silent treatments was 0.4 m and 3.7 m for veery and ovenbirds, respectively; p > 0.80 in both comparisons). Point count data on the two controls were analysed for all distance categories (0-10, 0-20 m, etc.) and the two nesting guilds (ground, canopy); all analyses were non-significant (t22 ≥ -1.967, p ≥ 0.062). Thus, we concluded procedural effects were not present justifying combining frog and silent controls into a single control group in all subsequent analyses. 72 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Results Nest-site selection During the two-year study we found 70 veery (n=27 for chipmunk, n=43 for control) and 32 ovenbird (n=15 for chipmunk, n=17 for control) nests on experimental plots. On average, birds placed nests at significantly greater distances (playback treatment: F1,94 = 7.347, p = 0.008) from chipmunk playbacks than controls (frog + silent; Fig. 1). The species treatment effect was not statistically significant (F1,94 = 2.179, p = 0.143). However, ovenbirds responded more strongly, on average, to chipmunk playbacks than veeries in both absolute mean distances (57.8 ± 4.6 vs. 37.8 ± 6.1 m for ovenbird and veery, respectively) and the increased distance relative to the control (20.0 m vs. 9.4 m, for ovenbird and veery, respectively; Fig. 1). All other effects and interactions were not significant. Point counts For the ground-nesting guild, the top-ranked model included two distance categories at 0-30 m (near) and 30-50 m (far); Table 1. All lower ranked models had very little support (AICc ≥ 9.90, model weight () < 0.01; Table 1). GLM analysis of the top model indicated significant year (F1,100 = 7.6, p = 0.007) and distance (F1,100 = 38.4, p < 0.001) effects. Most relevant, the total number of ground-nesting birds detected did not differ by treatment (main treatment effect: F1,100 = 0.05, p = 0.82), but fewer ground nesters were detected near chipmunk plot centers relative to control 73 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 plots, while more ground nesters were detected far from chipmunk plot centers relative to control plots (treatment × distance interaction: F1,100 = 12.6, p = 0.001; Fig. 2a). For the canopy-nesting guild, the top-ranked model included two distance categories at 0-30 m (near) and 30-50 m (far); Table 1. All lower ranked models had very little support (AICc ≥ 32.1; Table 1). GLM analysis of the top model indicated a distance effect (F1,100 = 21.0, p < 0.001) with fewer birds recorded within 0-20 m versus 20-50 m; all other main effects (F1,100 < 2.6, p > 0.10) and interaction (F1,100 < 3.14, p > 0.08) were not significant (Fig. 2b). Discussion Our results indicate that two ground-nesting species, ovenbirds and veeries, eavesdrop on chipmunk vocalizations and use this cue to adjust nest-site placement, and furthermore, to alter territory settlement and/or reduce conspicuous behaviors (e.g., calling frequency). Relative to controls, nests of ovenbirds were, on average, 20 m farther from chipmunk playback stations than control stations (Fig. 1). Chipmunk home ranges vary in size from 0.03-0.40 ha (i.e.,, radius between 9.8-35.7 m for circular home ranges; Dunford 1970; Snyder 1982) with greater activity usually focused in the middle of territories at “activity centers” around burrow entrances (Elliot 1978; Bowers 1995). Hence, a shift of 20 m from areas with high perceived chipmunk activity is likely to place the nest outside of a chipmunk’s home range, or at very least, farther from risky activity centers. In addition, newly fledged songbirds are weak fliers remaining relatively close to the nest and within the natal territory for 74 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 several days post-fledging (Rush & Stutchbury 2008; Vitz & Rodewald 2010). Therefore, constructing nests away from areas of greater chipmunk activity could additionally function to reduce fledgling mortality as chipmunks are also important predators of fledgling birds at our study site (Schmidt, Rush & Ostfeld 2007). Veeries too nested, on average, farther away from chipmunk playbacks relative to controls, however the strength of the response was less than half of what we observed with ovenbirds, and may be less biologically significant. There are several, non-mutually exclusive hypotheses for the weaker response observed in veeries. First, mice may be more important nest predators than chipmunks based on experimental evidence (e.g., Schmidt et al. 2001). Second, at our study site veeries show a strong preference for mesic habitat surrounding small forested wetlands (Schmidt et al. 2005). Thus habitat preferences may behaviorally constrain veeries (more so than ovenbirds) from making significant nest-site or territory adjustments (unlike Betts et al. 2008). This hypothesis remains untested, and sites containing both mesic and xeric habitats were used for the study. Lastly, veeries may also use alternative sources of information, such as returning to territories and nest sites where they successfully raised a brood in the past (Greenwood & Harvey 1982; Switzer 1993; Schmidt 2001; Hoover 2003). Repeated use of territories and nest sites by veeries is high at our site, for example, individual shrubs have been repeatedly used as nest substrates 3-5 times over a 10 year period (K.A. Schmidt, unpublished data). However, without a colorbanded population we lacked the ability to quantify fidelity of individual birds. While veeries may not respond like ovenbirds by constructing nests further away from 75 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 chipmunk playbacks this does not necessarily mean they do not eavesdrop on chipmunk calls to make nest-site decisions. Veeries may, alternatively, show plasticity in other features at the nest-site. For example, veeries may place nests in denser vegetation for greater concealment (Marzluff 1988; Eggers et al. 2006), or conversely, nest in less dense vegetation that would expose chipmunks to their own predators (as in Forstmeier & Weiss 2004). Also, unlike obligate ground-nesting ovenbirds, veeries are not constrained to adjust the height of their nests. The number of birds detected during point counts corroborated the nest placement data. Ground-nesting species, which included ovenbirds and veeries, were less frequently detected near (0-30 m) chipmunk playback plots relative to controls (Fig. 2a). This pattern could be the result of two non-mutually exclusive responses. One explanation is that there was a relative shift in the distribution of ground-nesting species from plot centers to the periphery, as predicted if birds adjusted territorial margins to avoid areas perceived as risky. Alternatively or concomitantly, adult birds may have displayed less overt behaviors (e.g., singing frequency; further reviewed in Lima 2009) in response to chipmunk playbacks lowering the detectability of individuals. For example, Fontaine and Martin (2006) found when they removed predators from experimental plots that singing activity of several passerine species significantly increased relative to plots with intact predator communities. We found a similar behavioral response to the perceived presence of nest predators, but uniquely we demonstrated a differential response between two nesting guilds based on the foraging niche of the predator as ground-nesting species responded to playbacks of 76 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 chipmunks while canopy-nesting species with relatively safer and higher nest sites did not. We used track plates to monitor the spatial activity of chipmunks and other potential nest predators across treatments, and were able to rule out the alternative hypothesis that birds responded to differences in the activity of rodents and other predator species and not strictly to our experimental playbacks. We did not examine changes in the frequency of natural chipmunk vocalizations in response to experimental playbacks. However, if wild chipmunks increased their vocalizations in chipmunk plots it would only reinforce our experimental protocol, whereas if increased vocalizations occurred off plots it would diminish differences between treatments and impede our ability to demonstrate significant effects. Thus, either scenario strengthens our findings that chipmunk vocalizations are an important influence on avian settlement. Lastly, it is possible chipmunk predators (e.g., raptors) may have been attracted to chipmunk playbacks, and it was these and not chipmunks per se that ovenbirds and veeries responded to. However, raptors were recorded on < 5 point counts during the 2-year study making this an unlikely scenario. Our results show quantitative differences in information use between veeries and ovenbirds, extending the small number of interspecific comparisons made to date (see Nocera, Forbes & Giraldeau 2006 and Parejo et al. 2007 vs. Doligez, Danchin & Clobert 2002; also Coolen et al. 2003 for an example in social foraging). Differences between species may arise due to their sensory abilities, natural history constraints (e.g., nest type; discussed above), or particular ecological tradeoffs (e.g., safety vs. 77 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 optimal microclimate; Eggers et al. 2006). These interspecific differences can potentially affect community structure (Fletcher 2008), especially when the information has potentially high fitness benefits as is the case when assessing spatial heterogeneity in predation risk. For instance, interspecific differences in assessment or use of information may result in dissimilar habitat use (i.e.,, near versus far from predator activity) or overlap of interspecific competitors. Furthermore, temporally variable environments, such as our pulsed-resource system (Schmidt & Ostfeld 2008), may confer a competitive advantage to those species using proximate cues providing current information on predation risk. Hence, ovenbirds may be better equipped to cope with interannual fluctuations of predator abundance and, in turn, have lower predation rates compared to veeries which do not appear to use chipmunk vocalizations to make nest-site decisions. Indeed, at our study site chipmunk activity measured around ovenbird nest sites as well as predation rates are lower than recorded at veery nests (Q.C. Emmering, unpublished data), but whether differences in nest predation rates are due to chipmunks remains untested. Past studies have demonstrated birds assess predator activity and subsequently make adaptive breeding decisions (Jędrzejewska & Jędrzejewski 1998; Spaans et al. 1998; Larsen 2000; Forstmeier & Weiss 2004; Roos & Pärt 2004; Fontaine & Martin 2006; Peluc et al. 2008) but rarely have assessment mechanisms been identified. Our results are consistent with the few studies that have also manipulated predator cues. For instance, Eggers et al. (2006) demonstrated Siberian jays (Perissoreus infaustus) more than doubled the distance between nest sites used in previous years and nested in 78 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 significantly denser vegetation in response to experimental playbacks of corvid nest predators. By confining least weasels (Mustela nivalis) with nest boxes to add olfactory and visual (weasel hair) cues of nest predation risk, Mönkkönen et al. (2009) found that pied flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) avoided risky nest boxes exposed to weasels significantly more so than control boxes lacking weasel cues. Lastly, Forsman & Martin (2009) recently demonstrated that songbird hosts of the parasitic brown-headed cowbird (Molothrus ater) eavesdrop on cowbird vocalizations to find ‘brood parasite-free’ space. Spatial heterogeneity in predation risk creates spatial refugia that can be potentially exploited by prey and consequently lower predator efficiency (Mech 1977; Fontaine & Martin 2006; Schmidt, Ostfeld & Smyth 2006; Schauber et al. 2009). Models of predator-prey dynamics indicate this reduced efficiency has important consequences for prey persistence and the stability of prey and predator populations (Huffaker 1958; Hilborn 1975; Lewis & Murray 1993; Goodwin et al. 2005; Schauber et al. 2007). In addition, our results may have implications for models of sitedependent regulation in heterogeneous landscapes (Rodenhouse, Sherry & Holmes 1997). Site-dependent regulation produces density dependence through a simple mechanism of heterogeneity in site (i.e.,, territory) quality where individuals fill up available sites according to their rank order of quality (Rodenhouse, Sherry & Holmes 1997). Spatial heterogeneity in local predator abundance is one of several mechanisms that can create spatial heterogeneity in territory quality (Rodenhouse et al. 2003; Schmidt, Ostfeld & Smyth 2006). If such differences in local predator 79 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 abundance can be detected via acoustic or other cues, the mechanism that we document here may partly underlie some of the empirical support for site-dependent regulation, especially in avian populations (e.g., Rodenhouse et al. 2003; Sillett, Rodenhouse & Holmes 2004; Zajac, Solarz & Bielanski 2008). Our study, and a growing list of others (e.g., Blaustein et al. 2004; Eggers et al. 2006; Mönkkönen et al. 2009), suggests that information on spatial distribution of predator activity may be readily available through the communication of predators themselves. Constrained in many cases to give vocal, chemical or other social cues to signal territoriality or to attract mates, predator communication is often publicly available to eavesdropping prey. Thus, heterospecific eavesdropping may be a common feature of predator-prey interactions that allows prey to locate spatial refugia and provide greater stability to predator-prey dynamics. In turn, there may be selective pressures on the evolution of predator communication to avoid informing their prey (e.g., Deecke, Ford & Slater 2005) or vice versa (Bernal et al. 2007). Combining the work of theorists and animal behaviorists is thus bringing to bear how communication and networks of eavesdroppers may have larger ecological consequences on predator-prey interactions than previously suspected. 80 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Mean distance to playback station (m) 70 Chipmunk Control 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Veery Ovenbird Figure 4.1 Mean (± SE) distance of nests to nearest chipmunk playback (gray bars) and control (frog + silent; white bars) stations for veeries and ovenbirds. 81 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 2.5 (a) chipmunk 2.0 control 1.5 Mean number of birds counted 1.0 0.5 0.0 0-30 2.5 30-50 (b) 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0 0-30 30-50 Distance from plot centre (m) Figure 4.2 Mean (± SE) number of birds in the ground-nesting (a) and canopynesting (b) guilds detected during point counts at chipmunk playback (gray bars) and control plots (frog + silent; white bars). Counts were separated into near and far from plot centers based on model selection procedures applied separately for each nesting guild (see Results). The treatment × distance category was significant only for the ground-nesting guild. 82 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Table 4.1 Model comparison using the information theoretic index (AICc) to test for differences in the number of (a) ground-nesting and (b) canopy-nesting birds detected near and far from playback stations during point counts. We used a sliding cutoff (in 10 m increments) to produce 5 models: 4 models with a near-far cutoff occurring at 10, 20, 30, and 40 m, and a fifth model (0-50 m) without a near-far categorization. Data were analyzed as a mixed effects general linear model (GLM) with the fixed effects of year (yr), playback treatment (tr), and distance (dist), random effects of playback station (PB), and interaction effects of treatment year and treatment distance. K indicates the number of parameters for individual models. In both analyses, the AIC weight of the top model was > 0.99. Results of the GLM for the top ranked model based on AIC was used to examine statistical significance (see Results). 83 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 CHAPTER V CONCLUSIONS Research summary Heterogeneity in predator activity creates predator-free space for prey to occupy and increase their chance of survival and/or successfully reproducing. For nesting songbirds, predator-free space can act as spatial refugia for safeguarding defenseless eggs and young from incidental nest predators. Since predation is the primary cause of nest failure in songbirds, nest-site selection is closely linked to fitness and population demography; hence, the presence and use of spatial refugia may have far-reaching consequences for songbirds. Information reliably associated with nest failure, such as predator activity, can be used to adjust breeding decisions leading to higher reproductive success. Songbirds likely possess behavioral traits that allow them to assess, either directly or indirectly, cues of predator activity. Communication between predators is often conspicuous and available in the public domain, including eavesdropping prey that can reduce time and acquisition costs associated with directly assessing predator activity. Previous research has demonstrated that birds often make breeding-site adjustments based on predation risk. Less common are investigations of the adjustments birds make in response to small-scale predator activity and experiments identifying the sources of information birds use to assess this heterogeneity. 84 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 The objective of my research was to determine if breeding veeries (Catharus fuscescens) and ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla) utilize “coldspots” of predator activity as nesting refugia to increase reproductive success. I quantified small-scale predator activity surrounding nest sites and estimated predator density (mice, chipmunks) at a larger scale (grid-level). Predator activity was estimated at similar spatial scales but over dissimilar temporal scales using two different techniques. Track plates estimated predator activity (rodents and mesopredators) surrounding nests over the course of a week. Whereas, capture data from permanent trapping grids estimated “hotspots” and “coldspots” of mouse and chipmunk activity from total summer captures (May – July). Additionally, I conducted a playback experiment to explore whether breeding songbirds eavesdrop on the calls of a common nest predator, the eastern chipmunk (Tamias striatus), to assess spatial heterogeneity in chipmunk activity to make breeding site decisions. Control plots consisted of playbacks of a non-predator (frog calls) and plots with no playbacks. In response to playbacks I compared the activity of ground- and canopy-nesting guilds as they likely differ in their susceptibility to chipmunk predation. Lastly, I recorded veery and ovenbird nest locations relative to experimental treatment and control plots. Results summary As earlier research has found (Schmidt et al. 2006, Schauber et al. 2009, Schartel 2011), mouse and chipmunk activity at our study site were spatially heterogeneous across years and trapping grids. The presence of such spatial 85 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 heterogeneity may be sufficient to create unoccupied space that can be used as refugia for songbirds to nest within to successfully raise young. However, analyses of rodent capture and track plate data, found that local predator activity was only weakly, or unrelated, to veery nest success (Tables 2.3, 2.4, 3.4). Instead, total rodent abundance (mice + chipmunks) had substantially greater support for describing veery nest success (Table 2.4). Veeries also did not place nests in sites of lower predator activity (Table 3.3). If indeed predator activity is only weakly related to veery nest success, it’s not unexpected that veeries would show no preference for sites based on variation in nest predator activity. A somewhat different story emerged with ovenbirds. Similar to veeries, analyses of both rodent capture and track plate data showed that ovenbirds did not place nests in areas of lower mouse or chipmunk activity (Tables 2.2 & 3.3). However, information theoretic analyses indicated ovenbird nest predation was best described by chipmunk activity within a 30 × 30 m area surrounding their nests (Table 2.3 – 2.4). Mouse abundance, and to a lesser extent total rodent activity and abundance, also may be important predictors of ovenbird nest predation. Although, model interpretations warrant some caution as model weights were low (Table 2.3). The playback experiment (Emmering & Schmidt 2011) corroborated the observed heterospecific differences as discussed above. In response to experimental playbacks, veeries and ovenbirds placed nests significantly further from plots broadcasting chipmunk vocalizations compared to control plots (frog, no playbacks). However, ovenbirds responded more strongly than veeries, placing nests 20 m further 86 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 from chipmunk playbacks compared to controls, whereas veeries responded at half the magnitude, nesting only 9.4 m further from chipmunk playbacks (Figure 4.1). In comparisons of two nesting guilds, ground-nesters (including veeries and ovenbirds) significantly reduced their activity within plot centers suggesting a shift in breeding territories or reduction of overt behaviors (e.g., singing; Figure 4.2). Canopy-nesting species, less vulnerable to predation to ground-based chipmunks, showed no differential response to playbacks (Figure 4.2). My results contrast with those of Schmidt et al. (2006). Although abundance was quantified at two different scales (grid versus study site), both studies found that rodent abundance was related to veery nest success as did recent work by Kelly et al. (in review). Dissimilarly, Schmidt et al. (2006) found that veery nest predation was significantly related to mouse activity as quantified by two metrics, neighborhood mouse activity and mouse-free space (Schmidt et al. 2006). An important distinction between Schmidt et al.’s (2006) metrics and SADIE techniques, is that cluster indices quantify spatial aggregations of trap station counts relative to one another in that the magnitude of the index at a trap station is independent of the count there (refer to Figure 2.1; Perry et al. 1999, Winder et al. 2001). This spatially-explicit approach should provide a more reliable estimate of spatial patterns of rodent activity for examining relationships with nest predation. 87 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Population implications Choice of breeding sites is closely tied to fitness and population demography as conditions within the breeding territory, particularly predation risk, can strongly affect reproductive success (Cody 1985, Jaenike & Holt 1991, Martin 1998). The availability of predator-free space as potential nesting refugia could be an essential resource for birds to successfully reproduce and hence influence their territory settlement decisions. Observed heterospecific differences in response to spatial variation in rodent activity could result in meaningful consequences for veery and ovenbird populations. For veeries, local rodent activity around nest sites appears weakly or unrelated to nest success. If this is indeed the case, it’s not unexpected that veeries also showed no preference for selecting sites based on variation in rodent activity. A lack of a relationship between local rodent activity and veery nest predation suggests finding and nesting within spatial refugia may be an ineffective strategy for veeries to increase reproductive success. Unlike veeries, predator activity, particular of chipmunks, is a good predictor of ovenbird nest success. Moreover, ovenbirds appear to eavesdrop on chipmunk vocalizations to assess heterogeneity in their activity and may subsequently select safer nest sites. This difference may allow ovenbird populations to be more resistant to the large temporal fluctuations of generalist predators that characterize pulsed-resource systems like our oak-dominated study site (Schmidt & Ostfeld 2008, see also McShea 2000, Clotfelter et al. 2007, Emmering & Schmidt 2011). Recent analysis of ovenbird nest survival from our study site by Kelly et al. (in review) 88 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 supports this hypothesis. Analyzing a 13-year dataset they found no relationship between ovenbird nest daily survival and rodent abundance; veery nest survival on the other hand was strongly related to rodent abundance (Kelly et al., in review). 89 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 LITERATURE CITED Anderson, D.R. 2008. Model based inference in the life sciences: a primer on the evidence. New York: Springer Batzli, G. O. 1977. Population Dynamics of the White-footed Mouse in Floodplain and Upland Forests. American Midland Naturalist 97:18-32. Benson, T. J., J. D. Brown, and J. C. Bednarz. 2010. Identifying predators clarifies predictors of nest success in a temperate passerine. Journal of Animal Ecology 79:225234. Betts, M.G., Hadley, A.S., Rodenhouse, N. & Nocera, J.J. (2008) Social information trumps vegetation structure in breeding-site selection by a migrant songbird. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 275, 2257-2263. Bernal, X.E., Page, R.A., Rand, A.S. & Ryan, M.J. (2007) Natural history miscellany Cues for eavesdroppers: Do frog calls indicate prey density and quality? American Naturalist, 169, 409-415. Bevier, L.R., Poole, A.F. & Moskoff, W. (2005) Veery (Catharus fuscescens), The Birds of North America Online (A. Poole, Ed.). Ithaca: Cornell Lab of Ornithology Blaustein, L., Kiflawi, M., Eitam, A., Mangel, M. & Cohen, J.E. (2004) Oviposition habitat selection in response to risk of predation in temporary pools: mode of detection and consistency across experimental venue. Oecologia, 138, 300-305. Bowers, M.A. (1995) Use of space and habitats by the eastern chipmunk, Tamias striatus. Journal of Mammalogy, 76, 12-21. Bowman, G. B. and L. D. Harris. 1980. Effect of Spatial Heterogeneity on GroundNest Depredation. The Journal of Wildlife Management 44:806-813. Brown, J. S. 1988. Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 22:37-47. Brown, J. S., R. A. Morgan, and B. D. Dow. 1992. Patch Use Under Predation Risk 2. A Test With Fox Squirrels, Sciurus niger. Annales Zoologici Fennici 29:311-318. Brown, J. S. and B. P. Kotler. 2004. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecology Letters 7:999-1014. 90 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Burke, D. M. and E. Nol. 1998. Influence of food abundance, nest-site habitat, and forest fragmentation on breeding ovenbirds. Auk 115:96-104. Burnham KP, Anderson DR (1998) Model selection inference: a practical informationtheoretic approach. New York: Springer Cain, J. W., K. S. Smallwood, M. L. Morrison, and H. L. Loffland. 2006. Influence of Mammal Activity on Nesting Success of Passerines. Journal of Wildlife Management 70:522-531. Carthey, A. J. R., J. P. Bytheway, and P. B. Banks. 2011. Negotiating a noisy, information-rich environment in search of cryptic prey: olfactory predators need patchiness in prey cues. Journal of Animal Ecology 80:742-752. Chalfoun, A. D. and K. A. Schmidt. 2012. Adaptive breeding-habitat selection: is it for the birds? Auk 129:589-599. Clotfelter, E.D., Pedersen, A.B., Cranford, J.A., Ram, N., Snajdr, E.A., Nolan, V. & Ketterson, E.D. (2007) Acorn mast drives long-term dynamics of rodent and songbird populations. Oecologia, 154, 493-503. Cody, M. L., ed. 1985. Habitat Selection in Birds. New York: Academic Press. Connors, M.J., Schauber, E.M., Forbes, A., Jones, C.G., Goodwin, B.J. & Ostfeld, R.S. (2005) Use of track plates to quantify predation risk at small spatial scales. Journal of Mammalogy, 86, 991-996. Coolen, I., van Bergen, Y., Day, R.L. & Laland, K.N. (2003) Species difference in adaptive use of public information in sticklebacks. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 270, 2413-2419. Cox, W. A., R. T. Frank, III, and J. Faaborg. 2012. Species and temporal factors affect predator-specific rates of nest predation for forest songbirds in the Midwest. The Auk 129:147-155. Crooks, K. R. and M. E. Soule. 1999. Mesopredator release and avifaunal extinctions in a fragmented system. Nature 400:563-566. Dall, S.R.X., Giraldeau, L.A., Olsson, O., McNamara, J.M. & Stephens, D.W. (2005) Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 20, 187-193. Deecke, V.B., Ford, J.K.B. & Slater, P.J.B. (2005) The vocal behaviour of mammaleating killer whales: communicating with costly calls. Animal Behaviour, 69, 395-405. 91 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Doligez, B., Danchin, E. & Clobert, J. (2002) Public information and breeding habitat selection in a wild bird population. Science, 297, 1168-1170. Dunford, C. (1970) Behavioral aspects of spatial organization in the chipmunk, Tamias striatus. Behaviour, 36, 215-230. Dutra, H., K. Barnett, J. Reinhardt, R. Marquis, and J. Orrock. 2011. Invasive plant species alters consumer behavior by providing refuge from predation. Oecologia:1-9. Eggers, S., Griesser, M., Nystrand, M. & Ekman, J. (2006) Predation risk induces changes in nest-site selection and clutch size in the Siberian jay. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 273, 701-706. Ellis-Felege, S. N., M. J. Conroy, W. E. Palmer, and J. P. Carroll. 2012. Predator reduction results in compensatory shifts in losses of avian ground nests. Journal of Applied Ecology 49:661-669. Emmering, Q. C. and K. A. Schmidt. 2011. Nesting songbirds assess spatial heterogeneity of predatory chipmunks by eavesdropping on their vocalizations. Journal of Animal Ecology 80:1305-1312. Ellison, K.S. & Ribic, C.A. 2012. Nest Defense. Video Surveillance of Nesting Birds (ed. Ribic, C.A., Thompson, F.R., and Pietz, P.J.). Berkeley: University of California Press. Studies in Avian Biology. Elliott, L. (1978) Social behavior and foraging ecology of the eastern chipmunk (Tamias striatus) in the Adirondack Mountains. Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology, no. 265. Fletcher, R.J. (2007) Species interactions and population density mediate the use of social cues for habitat selection. Journal of Animal Ecology, 76, 598-606. Fletcher, R.J. (2008) Social information and community dynamics: Nontarget effects from simulating social cues for management. Ecological Applications, 18, 1764-1773. Fontaine, J.J. & Martin, T.E. 2006a. Habitat selection responses of parents to offspring predation risk: An experimental test. American Naturalist, 168, 811-818. Fontaine, J. J. and T. E. Martin. 2006b. Parent birds assess nest predation risk and adjust their reproductive strategies. Ecology Letters 9:428-434. Forsman, J.T., Martin, T.E. (2009) Habitat selection for parasite-free space by hosts of parasitic cowbirds. Oikos, 118, 464-470. 92 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Forsman, J. T., M. Mönkkönen, E. Korpimäki, and R. L. Thomson. 2013. Mammalian nest predator feces as a cue in avian habitat selection decisions. Behavioral Ecology 24:262-266. Forstmeier, W. & Weiss, I. (2004) Adaptive plasticity in nest-site selection in response to changing predation risk. Oikos, 104, 487-499. Gause, G. F. 1934. The struggle for existence. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, Maryland, USA. Gloag, R., V. D. Fiorini, J. C. Reboreda, and A. Kacelnik. 2013. The wages of violence: mobbing by mockingbirds as a frontline defence against brood-parasitic cowbirds. Animal Behaviour 86:1023-1029. Goodwin, B.J., Jones, C.G., Schauber, E.M. & Ostfeld, R.S. (2005) Limited dispersal and heterogeneous predation risk synergistically enhance persistence of rare prey. Ecology, 86, 3139-3148. Gorini, L., J. D. C. Linnell, R. May, M. Panzacchi, L. Boitani, M. Odden, and E. B. Nilsen. 2012. Habitat heterogeneity and mammalian predator-prey interactions. Mammal Review 42:55-77. Greenwood, P.J. & Harvey, P.H. (1982) The natal and breeding dispersal of birds. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 13, 1-21. Haas, C.A. (1998) Effects of prior nesting success on site fidelity and breeding dispersal: An experimental approach. Auk, 115, 929-936. Haché, S., E. M. Bayne, and M.-A. Villard. 2014. Postharvest regeneration, sciurid abundance, and postfledging survival and movements in an Ovenbird population. The Condor 116:102-112. Hamilton, W. J., Jr. 1941. The Food of Small Forest Mammals in Eastern United States. Journal of Mammalogy 22:250-263. Hastings, A. 1977. Spatial heterogeneity and the stability of predator-prey systems. Theoretical Population Biology 12:37-48. Heckscher, C. M. 2004. Veery nest sites in Mid-Atlantic Piedmont forest: Vegetative physiognomy and use of alien shrubs. American Midland Naturalist 151:326-337. Hilborn, R. (1975) The effect of spatial heterogeneity on the persistence of predatorprey interactions. Theoretical Population Biology, 8, 346-355. 93 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Hoover, J.P. (2003) Decision rules for site fidelity in a migratory bird, the prothonotary warbler. Ecology, 84, 416-430. Hromada, M., Antczak, M., Valone, T.J. & Tryjanowski, P. (2008) Settling decisions and heterospecific social information use in shrikes. PLoS ONE, 3, e3930. Hua, F., R. J. Fletcher, K. E. Sieving, and R. M. Dorazio. 2013. Too risky to settle: avian community structure changes in response to perceived predation risk on adults and offspring. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 280. Huffaker, C.B. (1958) Experimental studies on predation: dispersion factor and predator–prey oscillations. Hilgardia, 27, 343-383. Hutto, R.L., Pletschet, S.M. & Hendricks, P. (1986) A fixed-radius point count method for nonbreeding and breeding-season use. Auk, 103, 593-602. Jackson, W.M., Rohwer, S. & Nolan, V., Jr. (1989). Within season breeding dispersal in prairie warblers and other passerines. Condor 91, 233–241. Jaenike, J. and R. D. Holt. 1991. Genetic-variation for habitat preference - evidence and explanations. American Naturalist 137:S67-S90. Jędrzejewski, B. & Jędrzejewski, W. (1998) Predation in vertebrate communities: the Bialowieza primeval forest as a case study. Springer-Verlag, New York. Jones, C.G., Ostfeld, R.S., Richard, M.P., Schauber, E.M. & Wolff, J.O. (1998) Chain reactions linking acorns to gypsy moth outbreaks and Lyme disease risk. Science, 279, 1023-1026. Kelly, J.K., Schmidt, K.A., & Ostfeld, R.S. Coexisting ground-nesting passerines are not alike: rodents and conspecific abundance differentially affect nest survival in the ovenbird (Seirus aurocapilla) and the veery (Catharus fuscescens), in review King, D. I. and R. M. DeGraaf. 2006. Predators at bird nests in a northern hardwood forest in New Hampshire. Journal of Field Ornithology 77:239-243. Lackey, J., D. Huckaby, and B. Ormiston. 1985. Peromyscus leucopus. Mammalian Species 247:1-10. Larsen, T. (2000) Influence of rodent density on nesting associations involving the bar-tailed godwit, Limosa lapponica. Ibis, 142, 476-481. 94 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Lecomte, N., V. Careau, G. Gauthier, and J.-F. Giroux. 2008. Predator behaviour and predation risk in the heterogeneous Arctic environment. Journal of Animal Ecology 77:439-447. Lewis, M.A. & Murray, J.D. (1993) Modelling territoriality and wolf deer interactions. Nature, 366, 738-740. Lima, S.L. (2009) Predators and the breeding bird: behavioral and reproductive flexibility under the risk of predation. Biological Reviews, 84, 485-513. Lovell, P. G., G. D. Ruxton, K. V. Langridge, and K. A. Spencer. 2013. Egg-Laying Substrate Selection for Optimal Camouflage by Quail. Current Biology 23:260-264. Martin, T. E. 1993. Nest Predation and Nest Sites - New Perspectives on Old Patterns. Bioscience 43:523-532. Martin, T.E. (1995) Avian life-history evolution in relation to nest sites, nest predation, and food. Ecological Monographs, 65, 101-127. Martin, T.E. 1998. Are microhabitat preferences of coexisting species under selection and adaptive? Ecology 79: 656-670. Martin, T. E., J. Scott, and C. Menge. 2000. Nest predation increases with parental activity: separating nest site and parental activity effects. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 267:2287-2293. Marzluff, J.M. (1988) Do pinyon jays alter nest placement based on prior experience. Animal Behaviour, 36, 1-10. McKinnon, L., D. Berteaux, G. Gauthier, and J. Bêty. 2013. Predator-mediated interactions between preferred, alternative and incidental prey in the arctic tundra. Oikos 122:1042-1048. McPeek, M. A., N. L. Rodenhouse, R. T. Holmes, and T. W. Sherry. 2001. A general model of site-dependent population regulation: population-level regulation without individual-level interactions. Oikos 94:417-424. McShea, W.J. (2000) The influence of acorn crops on annual variation in rodent and bird populations. Ecology, 81, 228-238. Mech, L.D. (1977) Wolf-pack buffer zones as prey reservoirs. Science, 198, 320-321. Monkkonen, M., Forsman, J.T., Kananoja, T. & Ylonen, H. (2009) Indirect cues of nest predation risk and avian reproductive decisions. Biology Letters, 5, 176-178. 95 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Morton, E.S. (2005) Predation and variation in breeding habitat use in the ovenbird, with special reference to breeding habitat selection in northwestern Pennsylvania. Wilson Bulletin, 117, 327-335. Nocera, J.J., Forbes, G.J. & Giraldeau, L.A. (2006) Inadvertent social information in breeding site selection of natal dispersing birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society BBiological Sciences, 273, 349-355. Ostfeld, R.S., Jones, C.G. & Wolff, J.O. (1996) Of mice and mast. Bioscience, 46, 323-330. Ostfeld, R.S. & Keesing, F. (2000) Pulsed resources and community dynamics of consumers in terrestrial ecosystems. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 15, 232-237. Parejo, D., White, J., Clobert, J., Dreiss, A. & Danchin, E. (2007) Blue tits use fledgling quantity and quality as public information in breeding site choice. Ecology, 88, 2373-2382. Part, T. & Doligez, B. (2003) Gathering public information for habitat selection: prospecting birds cue on parental activity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 270, 1809-1813. Peake, T. (2005) Eavesdropping in communication networks. Animal Communication Networks (ed. P. McGregor), pp. 13-37. Cambridge University Press. Peluc, S.I., Sillett, T.S., Rotenberry, J.T. & Ghalambor, C.K. (2008) Adaptive phenotypic plasticity in an island songbird exposed to a novel predation risk. Behavioral Ecology, 19, 830-835. Perry, J. N. 1995. Spatial-analysis by distance indexes. Journal of Animal Ecology 64:303-314. Perry, J. N., L. Winder, J. M. Holland, and R. D. Alston. 1999. Red-blue plots for detecting clusters in count data. Ecology Letters 2:106-113. Pledger, S., K. H. Pollock, and J. L. Norris. 2010. Open Capture–Recapture Models with Heterogeneity: II. Jolly–Seber Model. Biometrics 66:883-890. Porneluzi, Paul, M. A. Van Horn and T.M. Donovan. 2011. Ovenbird (Seiurus aurocapilla), The Birds of North America Online (A. Poole, Ed.). Ithaca: Cornell Lab of Ornithology; Retrieved from the Birds of North America 96 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL http://www.Rproject.org/ Reitsma, L. R., R. T. Holmes, and T. W. Sherry. 1990. Effects of Removal of Red Squirrels, Tamiasciurus hudsonicus, and Eastern Chipmunks, Tamias striatus, on Nest Predation in a Northern Hardwood Forest - an Artificial Nest Experiment. Oikos 57:375-380. Remeš, V., B. Matysioková, and A. Cockburn. 2012. Long-term and large-scale analyses of nest predation patterns in Australian songbirds and a global comparison of nest predation rates. Journal of Avian Biology 43:435-444. Resetarits, W. J. and H. M. Wilbur. 1989. Choice of oviposition site by Hyla chrysoscelis - role of predators and competitors. Ecology 70:220-228. Rodewald, A. D. and L. J. Kearns. 2011. Shifts in dominant nest predators along a rural-to-urban landscape gradient. Condor 113:899-906. Ribic, C.A., Thompson, F.R., and Pietz, P.J., eds. Video Surveillance of Nesting Birds. Berkeley: University of California Press. 2012. Studies in Avian Biology. Ricklefs, Robert E. 1969. An Analysis of Nesting Mortality in Birds. Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology 9:1-48. Rodenhouse, N.L., Sherry, T.W., & Holmes, R.T. (1997) Site-dependent regulation of population size: A new synthesis. Ecology, 78, 2025-2042. Rodenhouse, N.L., Sillett, T.S., Doran, P.J. & Holmes, R.T. (2003) Multiple densitydependence mechanisms regulate a migratory bird population during the breeding season. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 270, 2105-2110. Roos, S. & Pärt, T. (2004) Nest predators affect spatial dynamics of breeding redbacked shrikes (Lanius collurio). Journal of Animal Ecology, 73, 117-127. Rush, S.A. & Stutchbury, B.J.M. (2008) Survival of fledgling Hooded Warblers (Wilsonia citrina) in small and large forest fragments. Auk, 125, 183-191. Safran, R.J. (2004) Adaptive site selection rules and variation in group size of barn swallows: Individual decisions predict population patterns. American Naturalist, 164, 121-131. 97 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Schartel, T. E. 2011. Quantifying and manipulating spatiotemporal trends in rodent space use and consumption rates on incidentally encountered prey. Master's. Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, Illinois. Schauber, E.M., Goodwin, B.J., Jones, C.G. & Ostfeld, R.S. (2007) Spatial selection and inheritance: Applying evolutionary concepts to population dynamics in heterogeneous space. Ecology, 88, 1112-1118. Schauber, E.M., Connors, M.J., Goodwin, B.J., Jones, C.G. & Ostfeld, R.S. (2009) Quantifying a dynamic risk landscape: heterogeneous predator activity and implications for prey persistence. Ecology, 90, 240-251. Schmidt, K.A., Goheen, J.R., Naumann, R., Ostfeld, R.S., Schauber, E.M. & Berkowitz, A. (2001a) Experimental removal of strong and weak predators: Mice and chipmunks preying on songbird nests. Ecology, 82, 2927-2936. Schmidt, K. A., J. R. Goheen, and R. Naumann. (2001b) Incidental nest predation in songbirds: Behavioral indicators detect ecological scales and processes. Ecology 82:2937-2947. Schmidt, K. A. 2003a. Linking frequencies of acorn masting in temperate forests to long-term population growth rates in a songbird: the veery (Catharus fuscescens). Oikos 103:548-558. Schmidt, K. A. 2003b. Nest predation and population declines in Illinois songbirds: a case for mesopredator effects. Conservation Biology 17:1141-1150. Schmidt, K. A. and R. S. Ostfeld. 2003a. Songbird populations in fluctuating environments: Predator responses to pulsed resources. Ecology 84:406-415. Schmidt, K. A. and R. S. Ostfeld. 2003b. Mice in space: Space use predicts the interaction between mice and songbirds. Ecology 84:3276-3283. Schmidt, K.A. 2004a. Site fidelity in temporally correlated environments enhances population persistence. Ecology Letters, 7, 176-184. Schmidt, K. A. 2004b. Incidental predation, enemy-free space and the coexistence of incidental prey. Oikos 106:335-343. Schmidt, K.A., Nelis, L.C., Briggs, N. & Ostfeld, R.S. (2005) Invasive shrubs and songbird nesting success: Effects of climate variability and predator abundance. Ecological Applications, 15, 258-265. 98 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Schmidt, K.A., Ostfeld, R.S. & Smyth, K.N. (2006) Spatial heterogeneity in predator activity, nest survivorship, and nest-site selection in two forest thrushes. Oecologia, 148, 22-29. Schmidt, K.A. & Schauber, E.M. (2007) Behavioral indicators of predator space use: Studying species interactions through the behaviour of predators. Israel Journal of Ecology & Evolution, 53, 389-406. Schmidt, K.A. & Ostfeld, R.S. (2008) Numerical and behavioral effects within a pulse-driven system: Consequences for shared prey. Ecology, 89, 635-646. Schmidt, K.A., Rush, S.A. & Ostfeld, R.S. (2008) Wood thrush nest success and postfledging survival across a temporal pulse of small mammal abundance in an oak forest. Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 830-837. Schwanz, L. E., D. Brisson, M. Gomes-Solecki, and R. S. Ostfeld. 2011. Linking disease and community ecology through behavioural indicators: immunochallenge of white-footed mice and its ecological impacts. Journal of Animal Ecology 80:204-214. Selas, V. 2001. Predation on reptiles and birds by the common buzzard, Buteo buteo, in relation to changes in its main prey, voles. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 79:2086-2093. Sergio, F., Marchesi, L. & Pedrini, P. (2003) Spatial refugia and the coexistence of a diurnal raptor with its intraguild owl predator. Journal of Animal Ecology, 72, 232245. Sillett, T.S., Rodenhouse, N.L & Holmes, R.T. (2004) Experimentally reducing neighbor density affects reproduction and behavior of a migratory songbird. Ecology, 85, 2467-2477. Slotnick, B. (2001) Animal cognition and the rat olfactory system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 216–222. Smith, T. M. and H. H. Shugart. 1987. Territory Size Variation in the Ovenbird: The Role of Habitat Structure. Ecology 68:695-704. Snyder, D.P. (1982) Tamias striatus. Mammalian Species, 168, 1-8. Spaans, B., Blijleven, H.J., Popov, I.U., Rykhlikova, M.E. & Ebbinge, B.S. (1998) Dark-bellied brent geese, Branta bernicla bernicla, forego breeding when Arctic Foxes, Alopex lagopus, are present during nest initiation. Ardea, 86, 11-20. 99 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Stevens, M. 2013. Evolutionary Ecology: Knowing How to Hide Your Eggs. Current Biology 23:R106-R108. Switzer, P.V. (1993) Site fidelity in predictable and unpredictable habitats. Evolutionary Ecology, 7, 533-555. Thompson, F. R. 2007. Factors affecting nest predation on forest songbirds in North America. Ibis 149:98-109. Van Horn, M. A. and T.M. Donovan. 1994. Ovenbird (Seiurus aurocapilla), The Birds of North America Online (A. Poole, Ed.). Ithaca: Cornell Lab of Ornithology Vickery, P. D., M. L. Hunter, and J. V. Wells. 1992. Evidence of incidental nest predation and its effects on nests of threatened grassland birds. Oikos 63:281-288. Vigallon, S. M. and J. M. Marzluff. 2005. Is nest predation by Steller's Jays (Cyanocitta stelleri) incidental or the result of a specialized search strategy? Auk 122:36-49. Viitala, J., Korpimaki, E., Palokangas, P. & Koivula, M. (1995) Attraction of kestrels to vole scent marks visible in ultraviolet-light. Nature, 373, 425-427. Vitz, A.C. & Rodewald, A.D. (2010) Movements of fledgling Ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla) and Worm-eating Warblers (Helmitheros vermivorum) within and beyond the natal home range. Auk, 127, 364-371. Walls, S. C. 1995. Differential vulnerability to predation and refuge use in competing larval salamanders. Oecologia 101:86-93. Williams, G. E. and P. B. Wood. 2002. Are traditional methods of determining nest predators and nest fates reliable? An experiment with wood thrushes (Hylocichla mustelina) using miniature video cameras. Auk 119:1126-1132. Winder, L., C. J. Alexander, J. M. Holland, C. Woolley, and J. N. Perry. 2001. Modelling the dynamic spatio-temporal response of predators to transient prey patches in the field. Ecology Letters 4:568-576. Wolff, J. O., R. D. Dueser, and K. S. Berry. 1985. Food Habits of Sympatric Peromyscus leucopus and Peromyscus maniculatus. Journal of Mammalogy 66:795798. Yanes, M. and F. Suarez. 1996. Incidental nest predation and lark conservation in an Iberian semiarid shrub steppe. Conservation Biology 10:881-887. 100 Texas Tech University, Quinn C. Emmering, May 2014 Zajac T., Solarz W. & Bielanski, W. (2008) Site-dependent population dynamics: the influence of spatial habitat heterogeneity on individual fitness in the sedge warbler, Acrocephalus schoenobaenus. Journal of Avian Biology, 39, 206-214. Zanette, L. Y., A. F. White, M. C. Allen, and M. Clinchy. 2011. Perceived Predation Risk Reduces the Number of Offspring Songbirds Produce per Year. Science 334:1398-1401. Zar, J.H. 1999. Biostatistical Analysis. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall. 663 p. 101