Download Early vs. Late Onset Hearing Loss: How Children Differ from Adults

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Memory and aging wikipedia , lookup

Earplug wikipedia , lookup

Telecommunications relay service wikipedia , lookup

Hearing aid wikipedia , lookup

Hearing loss wikipedia , lookup

Noise-induced hearing loss wikipedia , lookup

Sensorineural hearing loss wikipedia , lookup

Audiology and hearing health professionals in developed and developing countries wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Early vs. Late Onset Hearing Loss:
How Children Differ from Adults
Andrea Pittman, PhD
Arizona State University
Heterogeneity of Children with
Hearing Loss
Chronological age
Age at onset
Age at identification
Age at amplification
Age at intervention
Duration of intervention
Quality of intervention
Degree of hearing loss
Configuration of hearing
loss
Etiology of hearing loss
Type of amplification
Consistency of hearing
aid use
Use of supplemental
devices (FM system)
Other handicapping
conditions
Parental involvement
Socioeconomic status
Mono vs. Bilingual
language learner
IQ
The big difference between adults
and children…
Adults use their residual hearing to
continue to communicate…
children use their residual hearing to
learn to communicate.
HI Children vs. HI Adults
Audiologic differences
Configuration of hearing loss
Perceptual differences
Perceptual weighting functions
Perceptual coherence
Perceptual management differences
Multitasking
Hearing Loss in Children and
Adults: Audiometric Configuration,
Asymmetry, and Progression
Pittman & Stelmachowicz (2003)
Ear & Hearing
Methods
Groups
6-year-old children
60-year-old adults
Audiogram Selection Criteria
Right ear thresholds
Pure-tone thresholds at octave frequencies
(250-8000 Hz)
At least one threshold > 30 dB HL
Confirmed sensorineural hearing losses
(air-bone gaps < 10 dB)
Methods
Core Set of Audiograms
227 children
248 adults
Analyses
Configuration
Asymmetry
Progression
All Audiograms
Children (n=227)
Adults (n=248)
Frequency (Hz)
Hearing Level (dB HL)
250
Frequency (Hz)
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
250
-20
-20
0
0
20
20
40
40
60
60
80
80
100
100
120
120
140
140
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
All Audiograms
Children (n=227)
Adults (n=248)
Frequency (Hz)
Hearing Level (dB HL)
250
Frequency (Hz)
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
250
-20
-20
0
0
20
20
40
40
60
60
80
80
100
100
120
120
140
140
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
Audiometric Classification
Frequency (Hz)
Hearing Level (dB)
250
250
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
250
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
0
0
0
20
20
20
40
40
40
60
60
60
80
80
80
Sloping
Rising
250
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
250
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
250
0
0
0
20
20
20
40
40
40
60
60
60
80
80
80
U-Shaped
100
Tent-Shaped
100
Flat
100
100
100
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
Other
100
Results - Distribution
Percent of Audiograms
50
Adults
Children
40
30
20
10
0
Sloping
U-Shaped
TentShaped
Category
Flat
Other
Rising
Results - Configuration
Adults
Children
Frequency (Hz)
Hearing Level (dB)
250
250
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
250
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
0
0
0
20
20
20
40
40
40
60
60
60
80
80
80
Sloping
Rising
250
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
250
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
250
0
0
0
20
20
20
40
40
40
60
60
60
80
80
80
U-Shaped
100
Tent-Shaped
100
Flat
100
100
100
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
Other
100
All Audiograms
Children (n=227)
Adults (n=248)
Frequency (Hz)
Hearing Level (dB HL)
250
Frequency (Hz)
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
250
-20
-20
0
0
20
20
40
40
60
60
80
80
100
100
120
120
140
140
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
Sloping Losses
Adults
73%
Children
% OF AUDIOGRAMS
50
Children
40
30
20
10
0
33%
Adults
Sloping U-Shaped
TentShaped
Flat
Other
Rising
Summary
The children had a wider variety of
audiometric configurations.
The hearing losses in adults were
typically sloping in configuration
accounting for 3-in-4 audiograms.
The same sloping configurations
occurred in only 1-in-3 of the children.
Influence of Hearing loss on the
Perceptual Strategies of Children
and Adults
Pittman, Stelmachowicz, Lewis & Hoover (2002)
Jr of Sp Lang & Hear Res
Pittman & Stelmachowicz (2000)
Jr of Sp Lang & Hear Res
Subjects
Frequency (Hz)
250
-20
Normal Hearing
10 Adults
20 Children
(mean = 6:8 yrs, 5-7)
Hearing Impaired
10 Adults
(mean = 59 yrs, 49-66)
10 Children
(mean = 7:8 yrs, 5-10)
0
Hearing Level (dB HL)
(mean = 28 yrs, 20-44)
20
40
60
80
100
120
500 1000 2000 4000 8000
Stimuli
4 words
Sack
Sock
CVC
2 vowels
2 fricatives
2 conditions
w/ transition
w/o transition
Shack
Shock
Stimuli
4 words
Sack
Sock
CVC
2 vowels
2 fricatives
2 conditions
w/ transition
w/o transition
Shack
Shock
Stimuli
4 words
w/ transition
CVC
2 vowels
2 fricatives
2 conditions
w/ transition
w/o transition
w/o transition
Presentation
5 levels
80
Level (dB SPL)
vowel
60
40
20
0
-20
500
2000
Frequency (Hz)
8000
Performance
80
100
60
80
% Correct
Level (dB SPL)
Short-Term Audibility
40
20
60
40
0
20
-20
0
500
2000
Frequency (Hz)
8000
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
Short-Term Audibility
1
Performance
100
80
60
40
20
0
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
Short-Term Audibility
1
Results
Normal-Hearing Adults
Performance
100
80
60
40
w/ transition
w/o transition
20
0
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
Short-Term Audibility
1
Normal-Hearing Adults
Normal-Hearing Children
80
80
Performance
100
Performance
100
60
40
60
40
20
20
0
0
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
1
0
Short-Term Audibility
80
Performance
80
Performance
100
60
40
0
0
0.6
0.8
1
40
20
0.4
0.8
60
20
Short-Term Audibility
0.6
Hearing-Impaired Children
100
0.2
0.4
Short-Term Audibility
Hearing-Impaired Adults
0
0.2
1
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
Short-Term Audibility
1
HIA
100
NHC
HIC
80
Performance
NHA
HIC
60
40
20
0
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
Short-Term Audibility
1
Conclusions
Relative to the normal-hearing adults…
The normal-hearing children demonstrated
typical development of speech perception.
The hearing-impaired adults demonstrated
the typical effects of hearing loss for the
perception of soft speech.
The hearing-impaired children
demonstrated a combination of speech
perception development and hearing loss
effects.
The whole was greater than the
sum of the parts.
Perceptual coherence in listeners
with childhood hearing losses, adultonset hearing losses, and normal
hearing
Pittman (January, 2008)
Jr Acoust Society Amer
Perceptual Coherence
Subjects
10 Normal hearing (mean age 25 years)
10 Acquired hearing losses (mean age 64 years)
10 Congenital hearing losses (mean age 35 years)
Frequency (Hz
Hearing Level (dB SPL)
0.25 0.5
1
2
4
0.25 0.5
8
-10
10
10
10
30
30
50
50
70
70
90
90
110
10
130
Acquired
30
1
Congenital
2
4
8
Stimuli
Speech
9 naturally
produced words
(sonorants)
Produced by a
male, female and
child.
Paradigm
Yes/No
Yes trial
F2 in the word
No trial
F2 not in the word
Results
Conclusions
Perceptual coherence was not
affected by hearing loss.
Perceptual coherence was affected by
the presence of hearing loss early in
life.
Practical consequences of poor
perceptual coherence are largely
unknown.
Results suggest long-term effects of
hearing loss.
Effect of minimal hearing loss
on children’s ability to multitask
in quiet and in noise
McFadden & Pittman (submitted)
J Lang Sp Hear Ser Schls
How do children allocate their
cognitive resources?
How do children allocate their
cognitive resources?
Downs & Crum (1978)
Normal-Hearing Adults
Paired Association Task & Probe Reaction Time
35 dB SL
Quite and +6dB SNR
SECONDARY TASK
PRIMARY TASK
400
12
11
350
300
Reaction Time (ms)
# of Learning Trials
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
Worse Better
Better Worse
10
250
200
150
100
2
50
1
0
0
Quiet
Noise
Quiet
Noise
Downs (1982)
Hearing-Impaired Adults
Word Recognition Task & Probe Reaction Time
58 dB SPL
0 dB SNR
SECONDARY TASK
PRIMARY TASK
125
80
Reaction Time (ms)
Word Recognition (% correct)
150
60
40
20
Worse Better
Worse Better
100
100
75
50
25
0
0
Aided
Unaided
Aided
Unaided
Hicks & Tharpe (2002)
Normal-Hearing & Hearing-Impaired Children
Word Recognition Task & Probe Reaction Time
70 dB SPL
Quiet, +20, +15 & +10 dB SNR
NHC
HIC
SECONDARY TASK
250
90
200
Reaction Time (ms)
Word Categorization (% correct)
100
80
70
Worse Better
Worse Better
PRIMARY TASK
150
100
50
60
0
50
Quiet
+10 dB SNR
Quiet
+10 dB SNR
Method
NH children
N=11
Age = 8-12 years
HI children
N=10
Age = 8-12 years
Minimal Hearing Losses
Unilateral
Mild
High-frequency
Method
Primary Task: Word categorization
Animal, Person, Food
Example: Dog, Mother, Pizza
Presented binaurally via earphones at
65 dB SPL
Listening conditions: Quiet, +6 dB SNR,
and 0 dB SNR
Secondary Task: Dot to dot puzzles
18 puzzles
Dot rate (dots/minute)
Method
Word Categorization
PERSON
FOOD
ANIMAL
Dot-to-dot Games
Results
NHC
HIC
PRIMARY TASK
SECONDARY TASK
100
100
90
Dot Rate (dots/min)
Word Categorization
80
80
70
Better Worse
Worse Better
90
70
60
50
40
30
20
60
10
0
50
Quiet
0dB SNR
Quiet
0dB SNR
Conclusions
Hearing loss did not alter the way these
children approached multiple tasks.
Children did not appear to redirect their
cognitive resources from one task to
accommodate the listening task.
May be unwilling to redirect
May be unable to redirect
May be unaware of the need to redirect
How does the difficulty of the competing
task affect their ability to attend to
speech?
Summary
Children’s hearing loss configurations
differ from those of adults
The presence of hearing loss early in
life has immediate and long-term effects
on auditory perception
Children do not (or are not able to)
approach listening tasks in the same
way as adults.
In the future…
Configuration specific amplification
strategies
Life-long effects of hearing loss
Management of perceptual resources