* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download What Is Culture? The Conceptual Question
Dual inheritance theory wikipedia , lookup
Ethnicities of the Philippine Cordilleras wikipedia , lookup
Western culture wikipedia , lookup
Cultural relativism wikipedia , lookup
Cultural ecology wikipedia , lookup
Oasisamerica wikipedia , lookup
Cross-cultural differences in decision-making wikipedia , lookup
American anthropology wikipedia , lookup
Cultural anthropology wikipedia , lookup
1 o What Is Culture? The Conceptual Question The anthropologist sees culture as the shared ideas and behaviors of a group of people. These Trobriand Island women are assembling yams they have harvested in preparation for a feast. 24 Overview 25 kkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkk Fishy Culture, Part I On the outskirts of the small town of Main Brook, in northern Newfoundland, surrounded by spruce forest, is “the Pit,” a gravel pit once excavated to build a road but subsequently treated as an all-purpose community resource for storing firewood, drying fishnets, picking berries, and gardening. My wife, Susan, and I drive out to the Pit to watch Aunt Belle, two of her sons, and a granddaughter setting potatoes in the sandy ground. We’re living in Main Brook to learn about Newfoundland culture, and Aunt Belle has become one of our best informants, or consultants, of cultural information. She is a vigorous, vocal advocate of traditional life in rural Newfoundland, which she draws her large extended family into sharing with her. Planting a big vegetable garden with a “crowd” of kin is one way to live that life (Figure 1.1). “I like for all hands to be as one,” she says, pausing to lean on her shovel. “All hands . . .” Aunt Belle’s language is shot through with evidence that her people have been fishermen. The image of the fishing boat and its crew spills over even into the potato patch. Her sons call me “skipper,” a friendly sign of respect less distancing than “sir.” They joke that being a teacher, I have a good “berth.” Berths are locations along the coast for anchoring cod traps, and some berths catch more cod and thus are more profitable than others. When today’s gardening is done, Aunt Belle will air out her garden boots and gloves not on her “stoop,” “stairs,” or “porch,” but on her “bridge,” an allusion to the captain’s observation deck on a ship. When she and her sons drive their pickup truck south to shop they “climb aboard our machine and steam up the coast”— “up . . .” meaning not “north” but upwind into the prevailing sailing winds. Belle’s is a culture thoroughly colored by nautical and fishing lore. So what do anthropologists mean by this term “culture”? kkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkk Overview Social workers, fish biologists, historians, and journalists have all at some time pulled on rubber boots and stood in the mud with Newfoundlanders while they were planting potatoes or cleaning fish. The anthropologist’s distinction doesn’t come from observing something that no one else observes. True, they occasionally study some uncommon subject, such as fish-grease boxes or toilet stall graffiti. And as recently as forty years ago anthropologists often were the first persons to walk into some remote village with the intention of learning the residents’ 26 Chapter One What Is Culture? Figure 1.1 In Newfoundland, Canada, Belle and her neighbors share a culture that calls for entire families to clear their gardens in early June to plant potatoes with fish and seaweed fertilizers. undocumented language and observing their unknown rituals. Those days are gone. The anthropologist’s genuine distinction from the other visitors standing around observing the scene has always been that she or he conceives of what people are doing and thinking in terms of their culture. Over many years we’ve developed the concept of culture and the questions that we ask ourselves, whether standing in muddy gardens or elegant courtyards, about the culture behind what’s happening. This chapter begins with a working definition of culture and goes on to distinguish the terms “ethnic group,” “society,” “race,” and “subculture” from the term “culture.” Seven widely accepted characteristics of culture are described. As societies become less isolated and cultures less geographically separated, anthropologists have adjusted their concept of culture; so three more recently conceived characteristics of culture are introduced. To help sharpen our idea of what culture is, the chapter concludes with seven statements about what culture is not. Upon successful completion of this chapter you should be able to: 1. Define culture. 2. Differentiate culture, ethnic group, and subculture, and distinguish culture from civilization, society, and behavior. 3. Explain why “race” is not a scientific concept. 4. Illustrate how cultural anthropologists study concepts of “race.” 5. Describe the impact of globalization on the anthropological concept of culture. 6. Apply seven characteristics of cultures to describe a culture with which you are familiar. A Definition of Culture 27 A Definition of Culture The questions cultural anthropologists ask about people’s lives derive from the way we define “culture.” I propose this general definition: Culture is the learned, shared understandings among a group of people about how to behave and what everything means. Take that sentence apart and examine the terms. “Learned, shared understandings” contrasts with instinctual or inborn behaviors, of which humans have relatively few except for the powerful drive of little ones to learn to talk, learn to walk, and acquire a culture. “Learned” means that young Newfoundlanders or recent immigrants to the island, such as a new spouse from the mainland, have probably acquired less of the culture than have the old Newfoundlanders and those who have lived in Newfoundland all their lives. Through social interaction Newfoundlanders are always learning from each other. They teach each other, imitate each other, correct each other, and so come to share a culture. From infancy on, a Newfoundlander is always learning, thus acquiring culture, a process we call enculturation. “Learned, shared understandings” means that if Susan and I are going to learn Newfoundland culture, we will have to assemble it from what we find in common among Belle, her neighbors, and many other people. If Belle knows or does something unique, then it isn’t in the culture. If her neighbors and kinfolk begin to know or do that something, too, then by definition it becomes part of the culture. “Learned, shared understandings” means that culture is in people’s minds. I use the word “understandings” to include knowledge, such as Newfoundlanders’ vocabulary for soil types (“pug,” “mud,” “sand,” and so forth) and also their deeper grasp of things, such as a feel for where that thin line is between joking with someone and insulting him. “Learned, shared understandings among a group of people” reinforces the “shared” idea, that culture only exists if a collectivity has it. The group can be small, such as Belle’s family or a fishing crew, which shares some experiences, knowledge, in-jokes, vocabulary, and so on. Or the group can be large, such as all Canadians, which includes French speakers and Inuktitut (formerly “Eskimo”) speakers, among others; but nevertheless all Canadians share some things by attending school, interacting politically, participating in the same economy, dealing with similar northern climates, and watching television. “Learned, shared understandings among a group of people about how to behave” says that culture guides our actions, sometimes as rules and knowledge that you are conscious of (“drive on the right, pass on the left”), and sometimes as habits you picked up unconsciously by imitating those around you, such as the way the tone of your voice rises at the end of a question. My field assistant in Newfoundland, Viva, learned how to sew from her mother Mabel, with additional clues from her Aunt Stella next door and from an older cousin up the road. Viva became a better seamstress than any of them, and now she teaches other women, informally and in workshops. Thus do actions become shared behavior in a group. 28 Chapter One What Is Culture? Figure 1.2 During the twelve days of Christmas in Newfoundland, groups of rowdy “mummers” (disguised tricksters) visit the houses demanding a drink and a dance, playing on their neighbors’ fear of strangers. “Learned, shared understanding among a group of people about how to behave and what everything means” says that not only does culture provide guidance on what to do, how to do it, and when, but culture also predicts and interprets what others will do and say. In Newfoundland, for example, Susan and I fairly quickly grasped that one doesn’t go to the front door of a house—the one that opens into the living room or “parlour”—unless one is the Mountie or the undertaker. We learned that to be interpreted correctly we were expected to enter a house through the kitchen door. Entering through the kitchen door “means” that we are neighbors or kin—that is, familiar, like family. Entering through the front door “means” death and trouble. After we grasped that, it still took us a while to learn to stop knocking on the kitchen door and waiting for the women of the house to admit us. Knocking on a door instead of just walking in and sitting down by the stove “means” that you’re a stranger (“you don’t belong to this place”), perhaps even a ghost, and thus again, probably trouble. At Christmas, the masked troublemakers, the “mummers,” knock on doors to frighten the inhabitants before entering to drink their booze, insult them, and pinch them (Figure 1.2). By the end of the first season of fieldwork, in and out of peoples’ houses all day, Susan and I had learned enough Newfoundland culture to behave correctly in approaching a house and thus be interpreted as friendly and familiar, so we could get on with the business of talking and observing without alarming people. You can imagine how much longer it took us to grasp the meaning of subtler, more complex events, such as when Aunt Sophie sent pies to the Anglican Church Women’s bake sale but didn’t show up herself to make purchases. Sophie’s action may be translated, “I’m going to do my duty as a responsible community member, not like some people I could mention, but I’m upset about the way some things are being done and if you don’t know what they are, you had A Definition of Culture 29 better approach me in a concerned and respectful manner and find out!” Obviously, our learning the Newfoundlanders’ culture was going to require some sophisticated interpretations of what everything means. In the foregoing I have sketched a working definition of culture. Before distinguishing the term from related concepts, note that anthropologists speak both of “culture” and “a culture.” Used in the first sense, and sometimes capitalized, “culture” or “Culture” refers to something that all humans have by virtue of being members of social groups. We mean the word that way when we say, “Culture distinguishes our species from that of the banana slug.” Used in the second sense, “a culture” refers to the particular shared understandings of a certain social group. We mean the word that way when we say, “The culture changed when the community abandoned wood fires for coal stoves.” Thinking about particular cultures associated with certain social groups leads to the concepts of subcultures and ethnic groups. kkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkk Fishy Culture, Part II Belle’s son empties a tub of caplin, a smeltlike fish, onto the newly planted potato bed, and Belle spreads the little fish around with her shovel. Newfoundland is an island in the north Atlantic, the farthest eastern province of Canada. Like Belle, most of the northern peninsula’s residents trace their ancestry back to southeastern Ireland and southwestern England. Their families have been settled in tiny coastal communities, called “outports,” engaged in fishing and logging since the early nineteenth century. In 1911, the geographer J. D. Rogers called Newfoundland’s entire social framework “fishy,” and in many respects it still is, even if Belle’s sons now spend most of their time logging or building highways. The way of life in Main Brook, and much of Newfoundland, is “fishy,” a culture shaped by 200 years of coastal settlement to supply European and Caribbean markets with salt cod, and now to provide fresh snow crab, shrimp, and flounder (Figure 1.3). As a landlubber, I notice immediately the turns of phrase, the clothing, the detailed attention to the weather, and the conversation starters by Belle and her crowd that evoke a life by the sea. A fishy culture, compared to the farming tradition that underlies most of North American culture, has strikingly different ideas about ownership, about risk, and about accumulation of resources. Fishermen’s religion is different, and they treat the land differently than farmers do. This maritime culture is not just a legacy celebrated by proud old women like Belle, however; the young in school have lessons and fieldtrips to perpetuate some of the lore. More important, the teachers are often locals who grew up with the legacy and teach it as much by example as by lessons. One of the teachers is Belle’s daughter-in-law. 30 Chapter One What Is Culture? Figure 1.3 The shared understandings and behaviors that make up Newfoundland culture are permeated by centuries of coastal settlement in pursuit of cod and salmon. This fishy maritime culture has never been static, although to visitors the communities may seem like museums of old-time ways of doing and talking. In fact, Newfoundland culture had been undergoing changes for decades before I stood in Belle’s potato patch. The traditional culture as well as the cultural changes became my research career for the next twenty years, summarized in a book and numerous articles that Susan and I wrote for the scholarly and popular press. How we have thought about that “fishy” culture will often serve as examples in this book. kkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkk Culture, Subculture, and Ethnic Group Many people in European American nations like the United States, Canada, and England use the terms “culture,” “society,” “ethnic group,” and “race,” often interchanging these terms. Cultural anthropologists, on the other hand, have attempted to distinguish and clarify these terms. To review, “culture” has been defined here as shared understandings, a guide to behavior and thought that people learn as members of some group. Culture is not the group, but it is a property of the group. The group could be a Boy Scout troop, a Newfoundland village, a population of Iraqi shepherds traveling over a dry grassland, an entire nation, or perhaps a group of nations. A society is a Culture, Subculture, and Ethnic Group 31 group of people organized into social relationships to perform certain tasks such as feeding and defending themselves and raising children. A society usually has its own territory, perpetuates itself over generations, and is at least partly selfgoverning. The society’s culture specifies what the social relationships should be, how to perform the tasks, and why to go to the trouble in the first place. A subculture is that particular mix of shared understandings held by groups within a larger society. What distinguishes the subculture from other subcultures in the society might be language, dress, religion, habits of work, food preferences, and child-raising practices, to mention just a few topics. Mexican farm workers in Michigan and Francophones (French speakers) in Canada participate in such subcultures. While maintaining some cultural distinctions from those around them, the farm workers and Francophones are also sharing some features of the wider culture. Other subcultures derive from common residence, work, or avocation. Silicon Valley computer programmers and Civil War reenactors participate in subcultures. A hundred years ago, “campus culture” was a fairly distinctive subculture of the United States, in which a relatively small number of students, mostly wealthy and mostly male, participated wholeheartedly for a few years, then remembered fondly and a little sheepishly for the rest of their lives (Moffatt 1989). An ethnic group is a group within a society that maintains a subculture based on religion, language, common origin, or ancestral traditions. The ethnic group invests effort to distinguish itself from others in the wider society. Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia, for example, is a multiethnic city in Southeast Asia. Residing there are Christians and Jews from Europe, Hindus from India, Moslem Malaysians, Buddhist Chinese from South China, and many smaller ethnic groups whose members immigrated to Kuala Lumpur from the rural areas and still practice the languages, religions, and historical traditions of their home area. As for Newfoundlanders like Belle and her family, now that their onceindependent country has become a Canadian province, they are closely tied into the rest of the nation, but they are still somewhat culturally distinct in their dialect and ancestral traditions; so they too may be considered an ethnic group. Ethnic groups usually share some culture with the other ethnic groups in order to participate in the political and economic life of the society of which they are parts. Newfoundlanders, for example, vote in Canadian national elections and watch the same television programs as other Canadians do. Ethnic group members are also aware of their group identity and distinctiveness, and they usually invest effort to foster such awareness in themselves and their fellow citizens. Newfoundlanders are definitely aware of their distinctiveness, as you’ll see in this book. To distinguish themselves from other groups, ethnic group members may intentionally display some cultural differences, which we call ethnic flags (Barth 1998). An ethnic flag might be as obvious as the chador, or “head scarf,” with which Muslim Malaysian women cover their heads in public, or as subtle as the little figurine of the god of commerce mounted next to the cash register in a Chinese Malaysian shop. For Newfoundlanders, one ethnic flag is their Anglo-Irish accent; another is their use of archaic and maritime words. Ethnic groups are easy to label (“Mennonite” or “Japanese Peruvian”) but harder to specify culturally. There is always the question of what is the essential 32 Chapter One What Is Culture? core and where is the boundary. Is it a defining characteristic of Mennonites that they drive black cars with no chrome? What distinguishes the Japanese Peruvians, since few of them speak Japanese? For individuals, ethnicity, meaning one’s identification with or participation in an ethnic group, can be even more challenging to define. One’s ethnicity may be multiple, or unclear to oneself, or vary depending upon the social situation. As cultural anthropologists have grappled with this complexity and fluidity, they have revised their concept of “culture,” as this chapter later describes. Culture and Race Anthropologists consider the concept of race to be unscientific, yet they study race all the time. How can that be? Defining “race” is difficult, because different cultures and scholars give it different meanings. In practice, most Americans treat ethnic group and race as the same thing, but for anthropologists, race refers to biological differences between populations, while “culture” and “ethnic group” refer to learned behavioral differences between groups (American Anthropological Association 1997). In the nineteenth century anthropologists tried to sort and classify the world’s races by biological criteria, but we now know that it can’t be done. In addition, nothing has been accomplished by trying to explain differences in history, behavior, and abilities of human populations in racial terms, because the differences are better explained by who has the “guns, germs, and steel,” as one scholar puts it (Diamond 1997). Our professional association’s “Statement on Race” points out that race doesn’t explain anything (American Anthropological Association 1998). It concludes that “any attempt to establish lines of division among biological populations [is] both arbitrary and subjective.” Some ethnic groups’ members may indeed appear physically different from others in the society. Among the Chinese in the Philippines, with whom I lived for a couple of years, were individuals who looked different from the Filipino majority, which stands to reason because the Chinese and Filipinos were geographically separate populations until the sixteenth century. But the Amish in western Pennsylvania are not physically different from their non-Amish neighbors, many of whom are, like the Amish, also descended from German immigrants. Even ethnic groups that are usually physically distinguished from other groups include individuals who are physically different from each other. The reverse is also true: individuals who are physically similar may be members of different ethnic groups. In other words, the biological differences within any named group (“Mexicans in Texas,” “Japanese Americans,” “whites,” for example) are as great as the differences between groups (American Anthropological Association 1998). To deny the reality of race in humans is not to deny the reality of biological difference. Obviously, biological variation in our species is enormous, encompassing important and fascinating heritable characteristics such as blood chemistry, body build, skin color, hair form, and physiology (the functioning of cells and organs). However, these traits vary independently of each other; all mixes are Race as a Cultural Construct 33 possible, and many combinations exist today. The traits don’t stick together through time in a cluster as something called a “race.” The branch of anthropology called biological anthropology takes as one of its tasks the study of human physical variety. That study shows that humans constitute just one species—one with much genetic variety, to be sure; but because its members are mobile and interbreeding, clear subdivisions are obliterated. Race as a Cultural Construct Even though there is no scientific basis for classifying people into races, societies never let the facts bother them. They develop ideas about the differences between people; so “race” may be called a cultural construct, meaning a conceptual model of reality shared by a group. A construct may be thoroughly tested, as has been the navigational knowledge about currents and weather possessed by Polynesian outrigger captains (Gladwin 1970). On the other hand, a cultural construct may be just one of many possible but flawed models of reality, as have been most cultures’ systems of racial categories. In creating these cultural constructs, cultures act as Humpty Dumpty does in Through the Looking Glass when he declares to Alice, “When I use a word, it means just what I choose it to mean—neither more nor less.” A scheme of human races is not a fact, it’s an “ideology of inequality” (American Anthropological Association 1998). For example, the North American concept of race and racial distinctions derives from the ideology of inequality developed during the European worldwide expansion that began in the sixteenth century. Explorers groped for reasons to justify conquest in other continents. Deciding that those continents were home to peoples of inferior races who needed help to save their souls and develop a proper work ethic was a convenient justification. In the United States, slavery, the Chinese Exclusion Laws, and the displacement of Native Americans during Western settlement were made more acceptable by defining other peoples as races and then claiming inheritable shortcomings in those races. This is the essence of racism, the belief that actual or alleged differences between racial groups indicate the superiority of one of them (Doob 1999, 7). Some of the nineteenth-century pioneers of cultural anthropology shared this cultural construct, this widespread ideology of differences, and they tried to place race on a scientific footing. It wasn’t until the early twentieth century that anthropologists, particularly Americans led by Franz Boas, the first anthropologist to hold a university position, demonstrated the flaws in racial research. Boas forcefully argued that race and culture should be treated as separate phenomena, and that the behavior that most of his countrymen were attributing to race was in fact cultural (Boas 1948). The question that arises for the cultural anthropologist today is not, therefore, what are the races of humankind, but what races do people of this or that culture imagine there are? What does the group think about human biological diversity, and why does it label differences in that way? Exercise 4 investigates the cultural constructs of English-speaking Americans concerning the races. Besides sharing an understanding of the names for races, most Americans have thought that children inherit a certain race from their parents and then remain that race for life. 34 Chapter One What Is Culture? Figure 1.4 In the United States (top), no matter how different these brothers look from each other, they will all be labeled as the same race: a person is labeled the same as his or her parents and retains that label for life. In Brazil (bottom), these boys will be labeled differently based upon their appearance, regardless of their parents’ appearance; and if their appearance changes, their race label changes. To classify the race of a child of parents of two races, the cultural rule (often codified into law) in the United States has been hypodescent, meaning the child is the same race as the parent of the race of lowest status. Meanwhile, Brazilians, another multiethnic population with much physical variety, define more races of people and permit them to change race during their lifetimes. Each Brazilian’s tipo, or race, is based on appearances, not on her or his parents (Figure 1.4). My professor collected forty terms for tipo used by residents of just one village (Kottak 2004, 122). A morena, for example, is a female with dark, straight hair, dark eyes, brown skin, and thin nose and lips. A sarara is a female with light hair, skin, and eyes, a wide nose, and thick lips. A morena and a sarara could be sisters, with the same mother and father. Brazilians also permit people to change racial labels when their lives change. If they acquire a different social status or cultural lifestyle or become tanned, they will be labeled as a different tipo (Fish 1995). My professor’s Brazilian research assistant referred to himself, variously, as escuro (“dark”), preto (“black”), or moreno escuro (“dark brunet”) (Kottak 2004, 123). Can you see how social mobility could differ between societies depending upon whether they believed the “races” were fixed or variable? Distinguishing the terms culture, a culture, subculture, society, race, and ethnic group allows us to speak somewhat more precisely about human groups and The Characteristics of Culture 35 their shared understandings. Next I’ll continue to sharpen the concept of culture and distinguish it from related concepts. The Characteristics of Culture The concept of culture as anthropologists use it to mean shared understandings and patterns of behavior is only a little more than 160 years old, beginning as European folklorists such as Gustav Klemm developed a way to understand peasants’ traditions and beliefs (Table 1.1). Since then, thousands of anthropologists Table 1.1 Timeline for the Concept of Culture Contemporary Events Year The Concept of Culture Napoleon’s army conquers Italy. 1796 – – – – German philosopher Immanuel Kant uses the term “Kulture,” meaning “civilization.” First telegraph lines are strung across the United States. 1843 – – – – – German ethnographer Gustav Klemm is first to use “Cultur” in an anthropological sense. First Wild West show is staged in Niagara Falls, New York. 1871 – – – – E. B. Tylor is the first English speaker to use “Culture” in an anthropological sense. The “Monkey Trial” in Dayton, Tennessee, convicts John Thomas Scopes for teaching evolution. 1925 – – – “Culture” in an anthropological sense first appears in English and American dictionaries. Russia launches the first orbiting satellite, Sputnik. 1958 – – – Influenced by cybernetics and physics, Leslie White describes “Culture” as a system of energy capture measurable in kilojoules per capita. “Ethnic cleansing” leads to massacres in Kosovo, of former Yugoslavia. 1995 – – – – American Anthropological Association President Annette Weiner suggests that “Culture” may have become obsolete in the contemporary globalizing world. Centenary celebration of the Olympic Games is held in Athens, Greece. 2004 Interdisciplinary “Culture Studies” programs and courses are offered at many U.S. colleges and universities. 36 Chapter One What Is Culture? have been writing and teaching about culture in that general sense. A few times they have even declared “culture” dead, but it recovers and even proliferates. The current growth of interdisciplinary “culture studies” programs at universities indicates the robustness of the culture concept. Culture studies combine literary and art criticism, communications, history, and anthropology around a humanistic and political definition of culture. Working from many theoretical positions on a wide variety of human topics, anthropologists have proposed and employed many variations on that definition of culture. Such a variety of definitions may alarm you, as it does some professionals, believing so much variety to be a sign of an immature science. But every discipline organizes itself around a few big concepts which, fortunately, escape precise definition and thus retain some power to generate creative work. Anthropology’s “culture” has been compared to the grand concepts in other fields, such as “energy,” “evolution,” and “society” (Kroeber and Kluckhohn 1963, orig. 1953, 375). I would add to this list “the market,” “personality,” “power,” and “art.” It’s not difficult to identify the discipline associated with each of these big and imprecise concepts. Even though there are many definitions, there is widespread consensus in textbooks about the principal characteristics of culture (Kottak 1997b, 18). A consensus of what culture is contributes to a consensus of what questions to ask about it. So here are seven characteristics of culture, each linked to chapters in this book: 1. 2. 3. 4. Cultures are integrated. Cultures are products of history. Cultures can be changed, and they can cause change. Cultures are strengthened by values. 5. Cultures are powerful determinants of behavior. 6. Cultures are largely composed of and transmitted by symbols. 7. Human culture is unique in complexity and variability. Cultures Are Integrated Cultures are integrated, though imperfectly. Integration means, for example, that one part of Newfoundland’s culture is related to another part. The traditional expectation that a young married couple should build a house next door to the husband’s father and brothers is related to the work organization of a cod-fishing crew of brothers. Not only do the men launch a boat together at 4:00 a.m., but the wives and children help split and dry the catch when the boat returns that afternoon. All this work is easier to organize if the workers are also neighbors. Cultural integration is imperfect, however, because cultures include inconsistencies. Newfoundland men can survive hard nights out in the bush in winter, but many are afraid of the dark in their own houses. The holistic question of Chapter 3 examines culture as an integrated system, or set of mutually influential relationships among the parts. The Characteristics of Culture 37 Cultures Are Products of History Our lives today have been shaped by the outcomes of past practices. Belle’s gardening and her language are “fishy” because her people have been going to sea for two centuries or more. American culture would be different today if German had been adopted as an official second language. In the 1850s, Congress considered adopting German, but decided against it. Sixty years later, the attitude toward German culture in the United States had deteriorated. Propaganda against the Germans during World War I was so intense that German words became taboo. Sauerkraut was called “liberty cabbage.” The comparative question of Chapter 4 reveals that most cultures have always been interconnected to, and thus influenced by, others. The temporal question of Chapter 5 focuses on history and culture change. If cultures are contingent, meaning highly dependent upon the particular events of their history, then it is difficult for anthropologists to construct general laws about how they function (“if this happens, that will happen”) because there will be so many exceptions. The naturalistic question of Chapter 2 and the reflexive question of Chapter 9 both consider the question of whether or not anthropology is a science. Cultures Can Be Changed, and They Can Cause Change Cultures can change rapidly. For example, in the 1970s many Newfoundlanders who had only recently acquired electricity swiftly accepted television into their homes. The change that television generated in visiting customs was equally swift. It became acceptable to stay home and watch “the soaps” instead of going next door to play card games. The social-structural question in Chapter 7 explores the relationship of society and culture that these house-visiting customs reveal. A culture also alters and is altered by the biology of the population that participates in it. As an example of culture affecting human biology, in prehistoric European societies, when milking cows became a regular part of the culture, smallpox also became established, because the bacterium that produces cowpox evolved to flourish in the nearby human host and became more virulent. Finally, a culture changes and is changed by the group’s biophysical environment, as when forest dwellers acquire metal axes and can more easily alter or eliminate their forest. The interactions of human biology, environment, and culture are explored in the bio-cultural question in Chapter 6. Cultures Are Strengthened by Values Cultures are strengthened by values, or shared understandings of what is good and right to do and to be, as well as what is bad and wrong. The values adhering to many of our common understandings compel us to do the right thing, culturally speaking. The presence of values helps to explain why people follow their culture so often, even if they know they don’t have to. In rural Newfoundland, most people are practicing Christians, and they observe the Sabbath by not engaging in any “work,” even if they don’t attend church that day. The value attached to the biblical injunction to treat the seventh day as a day of rest has been strong 38 Chapter One What Is Culture? enough that anyone who absolutely needed to do some chores would do them out of sight. Susan and I considered it a nuisance that Main Brook’s few shops were closed on Sunday and that everyone stopped doing their ordinary work, which is what I was there to observe. Over time, however, we began to see the Sabbath from the Newfoundlanders’ perspective. One day while I was watching Dan help his mother Belle to dig potatoes, I asked him whether they would be back the next day, Sunday, to finish the job. Dan replied, “No, boy, we don’t work on the Sabbath. If you want to work on the Sabbath then you didn’t work hard enough the other six days.” I thought his retort was amusing at the time, but writing it in my notes later, I realized that Dan was linking the value of observing the Sabbath with the value of hard work. Newfoundlanders impressed us as very hard workers, putting in long hours, often both on the job and around their house and garden. To call someone a hard worker is high praise. But as it is written in Ecclesiastes, there is a time for work and a time for not working. Viewing other people’s ways as relative to their historical and cultural situation is what is meant by the relativistic question in Chapter 10. When anthropologists look at other cultures from the relativistic perspective, meaning without judging it by our own standards, we begin to examine our own culture afresh, and even to become self-critical about the way we examine culture, which is the point of the reflexive question in Chapter 9. Cultures Are Powerful Determinants of Behavior Cultures are powerful determinants of behavior, but people are not puppets. Culture is powerful because much of what we have learned is beneath our awareness, or has become a comfortable habit, or is surrounded by values that make us feel bad if we don’t do right, or is being done by everybody else so we conform in order to be accepted. But sometimes we step out between the bars of our cage and do something alternative, deviant, unique, or creative. All of us break some of the rules sometimes. None of us even knows all of the rules, as Exercise 4 in Chapter 2 demonstrates. In Newfoundland I was impressed by how powerful were the forces for conformity in a small coastal village of 500 souls. Yet each person had also carved out an individuality, including quirky behaviors and ideas. So cultures can be employed selectively, argued over, analyzed, and changed by their practitioners. That’s one of the reasons we ask the dialogic question in Chapter 11. Cultures Are Largely Composed of and Transmitted by Symbols Bundling our shared understandings in symbols is what gives culture such power to cumulate, to be transferred between people, and to endure across generations. A symbol is anything to which its users assign meaning. The meaning is given by the culture and may be quite arbitrary. The action or object may have no obvious relationship to its given meaning. Actions can be symbolic, as when Mother waggles her index finger at you. Objects can be symbolic, such as a red octagon representing the STOP sign. The eight-pointed icon used on the first page of each chapter in this book is an old symbol still being stitched into Amish and plains The Characteristics of Culture 39 Lakota quilts as the “lone star” or “morning star,” rising in the east and representing Christian hope. We may treat it here as hope for the enlightenment or insight about human culture. Human languages are symbolic. The funny little figures covering this page or the squeaks and grunts I utter when I lecture are symbolic, transmitting meaning to readers and listeners who have learned English. Some symbols are freighted with deep layers of significance, such as a wedding ring, and others have simple meanings, such as the word “asparagus.” An individual is largely enculturated through symbolic communication, even when practical experience accompanies it. For example, a Newfoundlander teaching her friend to hook a rug relies on demonstration but also on a good deal of talk. The talk greatly extends the cultural transmission or lesson about how to hook this rug. It will probably include reference to the past (“this is how my mother used to do this”), to the hypothetical (“another way this stitch could be done,” or “be sure not to . . .”), and to the relationship between instructor and student (“just because I’m teaching you this doesn’t mean I think I’m better than you are”). Verbal or visual, most of human behavior is symbolic, in that it carries meaning assigned to it by the participants. If the ads in my New Yorker magazine are to be believed, even the fragrance of a cologne can carry a fairly precise meaning. Discovering meaning is the purpose of the interpretive question in Chapter 8. Human Culture Is Unique in Complexity and Variability Scholars disagree about human uniqueness. But arguing about uniqueness is good intellectual exercise, so here is a taste of the debate. Some anthropologists propose that the abilities of other species indicate a continuum in cultural behavior between humans and other animals, such as the great apes (Savage-Rumbaugh 1992). To claim that we are unique, those anthropologists warn, may not be particularly useful and may in practice justify bad treatment of other animals. No, we’re unique, say others (e.g., Barrett 1991, 55–76; Perry 2003, 58–60). They acknowledge that other social species—wolves, honeybees, and chimpanzees—share complex behaviors, communicate abundantly, and solve problems creatively; but they point out that those shared understandings are not learned and expressed symbolically, as human culture is, which greatly expands our possibilities. We alone, they say, can discuss the past, plan the future, and imagine alternate worlds. Some linguists add that human thought, being syntactical—meaning it is structured into sentences—is also unique to humans (Bickerton 1996; Chomsky 1988). But apes can be taught grammar, others retort; consider Kanzy, the pygmy chimpanzee who constructed sentences in a symbolic language on a computer keyboard (Greenfield and Savage-Rumbaugh 1990). But apes don’t do that kind of communication except in captivity with human trainers, comes the reply, and a lot of teaching is necessary. No human has to be taught symbolic language. “In fact, you can’t prevent the child from learning it” (Chomsky 1994). The debate over uniqueness has led to many interesting discoveries about language, learning, apes, and animals’ abilities. Whichever side of the debate one takes, however, one will still be impressed by the great gulf between humans and 40 Chapter One What Is Culture? other species in the complexity and variability of human culture and the variability of cultures within our species (Geertz 1973b). As for the complexity of human culture, Exhibit A might be the Australian aborigines’ kinship and marriage rules, the workings of which can render a graduate student insensate. For an example, see “Gidjingali Marriage Arrangements” (Hiatt 1968). As for human cultural variability, anthropologists find great differences among cultures in such matters as their political or religious systems. The speed with which a culture can change is also an indicator of its variability. Because shared understandings can be altered and the new forms transmitted from person to person within a lifetime, a group can change culturally almost overnight. Cultures’ ability to change swiftly has resulted in many varieties of human societies, not only over time but at any one time. For example, in just a few centuries between 1600 to 1900, the culture of Great Britain generated variations of itself among its colonists in South Africa, Australia, Newfoundland, the Falklands, and the United States. These colonies adapted Mother England’s culture to the land, climate, and societies encountered in their new place, resulting in differences among them in language, farming methods, religion, and which side of the road to drive on, to name a few. Meanwhile, thousands of other human societies, on those and other continents, whose heritage owed nothing to Great Britain, were adapting, adopting, and inventing their way to distinctiveness. Thus we find Jivaro, Samoan, Zuni, Chinese, and Icelandic cultures exhibiting greater behavioral differences among them than we find between different species of seagull or gazelle. Anthropology arose as a discipline to discover, catalogue, and explain all this human variety; so the comparative question of Chapter 4 has long been central to our inquiry. As Cultures Change, “Culture” Changes Now our culture-made and culture-making human species has reached the twenty-first century. The people of the world are drawn into an increasingly dense web of communication, trade, and travel. This change process, referred to as globalization, is not new, but just the latest phase in 500 years of European expansion. With few exceptions, the current trend is still largely driven by and for the interests of societies derived from those of the early expansionists, namely, those in North America and Europe. This long-term globalization trend has had a pronounced effect upon cultures and on anthropology’s idea of what “culture” is. One effect has been that the idea of “a culture” as equivalent to a single, bounded society in one physical place may have become obsolete. Until the 1960s, the cultures that anthropologists studied were commonly presented in textbooks as discrete ways of life associated with geographically separate societies, such as the Hopi or the Samoans. On a map you could point to a few islands in the South Pacific, just east of New Guinea, and say “Trobriand Island culture is here,” or to a spot in the Amazon and say, “Yanomamo culture is here.” With few exceptions, the old days of distinct, geographically bounded, slowchanging societies and cultures are gone. Samoans are in Los Angeles, and As Cultures Change, “Culture” Changes 41 Figure 1.5 The prominent role of ethnicity in current national and international affairs demands a sophisticated and flexible notion of culture. These Kurds distinguish themselves from other Iraqis in language, history, dress, and religion, and insist upon political representation in the newly forming government. Trobriand Islanders watch Hollywood movies. Many of my friends from Newfoundland have moved to the petroleum fields of Northern Alberta or above the arctic circle, on the tundra in Nunavut, historically the territory of those we called the “Copper Eskimo.” Nevertheless, “culture” is still at the center of human events. The powerful and sometimes alarming role of ethnicity in current events is evidence of that (Figure 1.5). So anthropologists, the students of cultures, still have a subject matter. But anthropology will need to adjust to what is happening to cultures. Some of the language that anthropologists have used to talk of culture will be abandoned, and new language will be adopted. These days we think of cultures as being more fluid, constructed, and mixed than we used to. Let’s look at each of these characteristics. Culture Is Seen as Fluid and Negotiable A culture is not just a tidy inventory of understandings shared by one society in one place. Because people are relocating themselves and passing around many objects and ideas, cultures flow between multiple groups. Two interacting groups may adjust their cultures to each other in a kind of unspoken bargaining process. Cultures may be situational, with different elements practiced or displayed depending upon where the practitioners are and who is nearby. When Newfoundlanders migrated in large numbers to Ontario in the 1960s to work in the factories, they emphasized their cultural tradition of hard work and sense of humor 42 Chapter One What Is Culture? and downplayed their religious and speech traits that Ontarians considered unsophisticated or old-fashioned. Although one’s culture includes much of what one ought to do and think, nevertheless participants can respond to these instructions in various ways, by emphasizing some aspects and downplaying others, or by giving new meanings to some traditional parts. In North America, for example, Christopher Columbus and his arrival in the New World are still commemorated by a bank holiday, but now we view the man and his achievement critically as being in the vanguard of conquest as much as we celebrate them as symbols of our cherished values of valor, ambition, and curiosity. Culture is Treated as a Constructed Phenomenon The shared understandings in a group are not necessarily dictated by reality; this was the point of my earlier description of Americans’ notion of race as a cultural construct. Groups construct or create their understandings by selecting this, ignoring that, and making up another thing. Although there are plenty of reality checks, a culture’s ideas sometimes have a life of their own, and they define reality for its practitioners. For example, in North America, in the seventeenth century the early European colonists’ conception of a pasture as good and a wilderness as bad was such a cultural construct. For practical and religious reasons, wilderness was nothing but trouble for those pioneers. By the nineteenth century, some Americans and Canadians reversed that view, deciding that wilderness was what made their nations great (Nash 1973). The North American wilderness hadn’t changed substantially in three centuries (except to grow smaller), but North Americans’ culture substantially changed its conception of wilderness. By the nineteenth century Americans thought of their landscape and their society as the antithesis of a class-bound Europe packed into a domesticated landscape. The idea of an America that is free and powerful like wild nature is a construct that remains central to our culture. Culture Is Expected to Be Mixed Individuals participate in a combination of subcultures (such as the understandings that you share with your sports team), regional cultures (such as Cajun or Pennsylvania Dutch), national (such as “American”), and even international cultures (sharing English, nuclear physics, air travel, the euro, soft drinks, traffic signs, and other traits). “We are all multicultural,” writes anthropologist John Caughey (2002, 174). He means that each of us has learned several cultures, which overlap and even compete or contradict one another in our lives (Figure 1.6). The idea of someone being multicultural was made clear to me one summer when I was analyzing interviews conducted with families in San Francisco’s Chinatown. The father, the mother, and one teenage child in each of these families were interviewed. My task was to see how these families were becoming acculturated. Acculturation is the culture change in one population that has come into contact with another, usually larger and dominant, population. Which families, and which family members, were changing the most, and what traits were chang- What Isn’t Culture? 43 Figure 1.6 Filipinos, colonized by the United States from 1898 to 1942, are widely literate in English. From it they have developed their own English slang. This tiny shop’s name illustrates the distinctive way that Filipinos compose English slogans. ing among these first-generation Chinatown immigrants? I developed a coding system to rate the interviewees’ participation and identification with Chinese or American culture. I expected a simple replacement process, in which a family member became more “American” over time and less “Chinese,” in language, recreational activities, values, self-identification, and the ethnicity of coworkers. The data refused to conform to my replacement model. How much and how fast an immigrant acculturated to American ways varied markedly from family to family, and even within families. Family members in school or multiethnic workplaces picked up American ways first, as I expected, but they didn’t always abandon Chinese ways in the process. So my analysis indicated that some interviewees were at once more Chinese and American than were others. This struck me as nonsensical at first, but I recalled from my language studies that some bilingual speakers are more articulate in both languages than some monolingual speakers are in either language. So I came to accept that some of these immigrants were richly bicultural. They played mah jong and they played tennis. What Isn’t Culture? To help sharpen your image of what culture is, consider some of what culture is not, at least when anthropologists use the term. 1. Culture is not the same as civilization, although in the early nineteenth century the political philosophers, in their attempts to write histories of mankind, equated 44 Chapter One What Is Culture? the two. Then, the word “culture” carried the notion of improvement or refinement, particularly by careful breeding. Improvement in livestock was an example of this meaning of “culture” and was indeed one of eighteenth-century science’s most momentous accomplishments. By extension, scholars of the time thought that humans too could be cultured and become civilized if they were properly educated. Today, anthropologists claim that every human group has culture. Civilization now refers to a complex society supported by intensive food production and organized around large urban centers providing administrative, commercial, artistic, and religious leadership. Civilization represents only one form of society among several, all of which operate with common shared understandings, or culture. 2. Culture is not the same as being refined or sophisticated, as when we think of someone who appreciates the art forms of European urban civilization such as opera and oil paintings. While it may be true that “you ain’t got no culcha,” you do have what anthropologists call culture. 3. Culture is not the same as society, although we often use those two terms interchangeably in loose talk. We might say, “Pathan culture is invading Besseri culture in Pakistan,” but what we mean is that groups of people who share a set of understandings that includes self-identification as Pathan are moving into the territory occupied by a population who share identity as Besseri. The true relationship between society and culture is that a society, by means of the interactions among its members, creates, shares, and perpetuates a culture. 4. Culture is not the explanation for everything people do. “His culture made him do it” could be an interesting legal defense, but anthropologists know that people can act with, around, or against their culture (Figure 1.7). Also, things happen that aren’t part of the plan. That Newfoundlanders used to play soccer with a seal bladder on the harbor ice can be viewed as cultural, but which team happened to win, or which player broke through the ice and got wet, isn’t cultural. Anthropology doesn’t claim that culture offers a complete explanation of human behavior, just that “there is a cultural element in most human behavior and that certain things in behavior make the most sense when seen through culture” (Kroeber and Kluckhohn 1963, 369). In this way culture joins the other grand concepts such as the market, personality, a lust for power, and lust itself, in being a valuable but partial explanation for what people do. 5. Culture is not behavior, although it guides behavior by informing us actors what to do and why to do it. Culture is shared understandings, which can’t be seen themselves. Anthropologists construct an ethnography, a description of a culture, by observing what people do, listening to what they say, and examining the objects that they make. From the patterns we observe, we then infer the common understandings that generated them—that is, the culture. Chapter 2 will discuss further how ethnographies are created, and Exercise 3 in that chapter provides an opportunity to practice constructing your own ethnography. I can check my recognition of a cultural pattern by predicting to myself what others will do or say the next time, and then watching to see if they do it. Then to What Isn’t Culture? 45 Figure 1.7 Newfoundland culture provides guidelines for who should fish together, how they should cast their net, and how they’ll divide the income. On the other hand, culture cannot explain why they didn’t catch many fish today, or why the second mate got seasick. test my understanding I try to act, say, or make something according to what I’ve learned, to see how it is received. If people smile and pat me on the back, then I’ve successfully tested my cultural discovery. If strong men faint and the elders turn their dogs on me, then it’s clear that I need to revise my conclusions. 6. Culture is not just food customs, musical traditions, and colorful costumes. These aspects of a culture are easily noticed, shared, even marketed. At diversity festivals or international fairs, food, music, and costume are celebrated. These are some of the ethnic flags defined earlier: attractive and distinctive but only small parts of the whole. Culture also includes much that occurs during one’s ordinary day. Beyond distinctive food recipes, for example, culture also influences elementary food routines such as how you hold your fork and knife, or that you eat with some other utensil altogether, or with your fingers, and which hand may be used to eat, and when you may eat, and with whom. Beyond songs and dances, culture also suggests ordinary physical actions such as how you are to move and hold your body the rest of the day when you’re not dancing, and which sounds you are allowed or expected to make when you’re not singing. Beyond the distinctive costume you might wear at a festival, culture also guides your basic clothing choices such as what you wear to bed, which parts of your body may be uncovered in public, what distinctions male and female garb will have, and when a garment ought to be laundered. 7. Culture is not complete agreement or consensus. You and I may share quite a bit of culture, but we could disagree strongly about many things, such as the morality of eating veal, the best way to raise children, and whether an all-powerful God 46 Chapter One What Is Culture? would permit evil to happen to good people. What culture actually does is define the debates and provide a language for our disagreement. Each culture has its issues of contention, some of which may not even occur to people of another culture. The issue of abortion, for example, is currently an intense legal, religious, moral, and political argument in the United States, about which reasonable, moral people will disagree. Calculating the number of angels that can dance on the head of a pin is not an issue for us now, although it once was very important in European scholastic circles. In the abortion debate, our culture defines the positions we take. We argue, for example, about how much authority a woman has over her own body. But doesn’t the state also have a responsibility to protect its future citizens? We also argue about the personhood of a fertilized egg. Is it a human at conception—or not until the second trimester? In U.S. society we do not argue about whether or not abortion is acceptable if the fetus is female rather than male. In other societies, there is cultural agreement that this is a hot debate item. It has been argued and legislated recently in India (Fukuyama 2002). In sum, each society is an “organization of diversity” (Wallace 2003); each society’s culture includes shared understandings of what to disagree about (Varenne 1984). Ideas and practices vary in their “cultural standing”: some are highly controversial, others disputable, many are common opinion, and others are completely taken for granted (Strauss 2004). Summary Culture is defined here as “the learned, shared understandings among a group of people about how to behave and what everything means.” After 200 years of use, the modern term “culture” has been defined in many ways, but there is a surprisingly consistent core of ideas within these definitions. Seven characteristics of culture are widely agreed upon: that cultures are integrated, the outcomes of history, and both the causes and results of events outside of culture. Also, anthropologists agree that cultures are powerful determinants of human behavior, are strengthened by values, and depend upon symbolic communication. Lastly, culture makes human behavior highly complex and variable. To further clarify the anthropological definition of culture, we examine seven common misconceptions about that definition. Culture is not the same as civilization, nor does it refer to refinement or sophistication. Culture is not the same as society, although the two make each other possible. Culture cannot explain everything that people think or do. It does not refer to the observed behavior itself, although it guides behavior. Culture is not limited to observable ethnic flags such as food, music, and costume. Finally, culture does not imply complete agreement or consensus among its practitioners. Cultural isolation is rare today, and culture change accelerates, fueled by what many people call the globalization process. Globalization has consequences both for cultures and for cultural anthropology. Culture, including cultural difference, will probably remain a dominant force in human affairs in the twenty-first Summary 47 century. At the same time, cultural anthropologists have adjusted the culture concept to emphasize three additional characteristics that acknowledge the practitioners’ mobility and motivations: culture is now seen as fluid and negotiable, as a constructed phenomenon, and as mixed for individuals as well as groups. kkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkk Fishy Culture, Part III We’re back from the gardens, sitting in Belle’s kitchen over a cup of tea. Belle’s best friend Flo drops in to join us. I’m quizzing Belle about some of her cousins in town. She remarks in passing, “’Is hant ’ad ’ee.” Flo replies, “Yis, girl,” and takes a sip of tea. I glance at Susan, but she shakes her head and raises her eyebrows. “’ee what?” I try. Patiently, as if we were two of her younger grandchildren, Belle explains. I’ve just cracked my shin, so to speak, on a cultural difference between Belle and me. We both think we speak English. But English has spread widely around the world, diversifying in pronunciation, idiomatic expression, and meanings of words. The journal World English is devoted to exploring this diversification. English speakers from Scotland, the Philippines, and Kenya would be hard pressed to understand one another without some time together to work things out. It took a couple of fieldtrips to Newfoundland before Susan and I no longer needed to repeat everything we said to Belle and her sons, or to ask them to repeat for us. “’Is hant ‘ad ‘ee” is English, but steeped in Newfoundland culture. To recognize Belle’s remark, put the h’s on where they were taken off, and take them off where they were put on. The result is “his aunt had he.” What that means, Belle explained, is that her cousin was born out of wedlock and raised by his mother’s sister, who was married and had other children. Therefore his biological mother became his “aunt” and his aunt became his “mother.” In witty Newfoundland shorthand, we have a statement that is true both ways: his aunt was his mother. It all makes perfect sense—after a while. So, anywhere there are humans there is culture, being practiced, constructed, manipulated, evaded, mixed—and, too often, misunderstood. Most of the time, most people are not aware of culture any more than the fish is aware of the water. But by the time you finish learning about the anthropological questions that make up the following chapters, you will be aware of culture and 48 Chapter One What Is Culture? also prepared to use the questions to think about what you see. Next we answer the question How do anthropologists learn about culture? kkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkkk t EXERCISES Exercise 1: The Embarrassing Incident If culture includes shared understandings for how to behave, then anthropologists can learn a good deal from observing what embarrasses the culture’s participants, and from inferring which understandings about how to behave were broken. In this exercise you will collect a pair of biographical anecdotes from your classmates and analyze them for the cultural rules lying behind them. Break into groups of three, and collect a story from two of you. Each informant gives a short description of an embarrassing incident in which the informant was present, although not necessarily as the “perpetrator.” All of you take notes on the form on page 49, which is a semistructured interview protocol—a fancy name for a common anthropological research tool. Such an interview protocol guides the questioning and organizes the answers but doesn’t constrain their wording. The informant provides the answers to the first four questions, but your group should develop an answer to the fifth question: What was the cultural rule that appears to have been broken? The cultural rule may be expressed as a dictum, such as You may begin eating only after Mother has raised her fork. Compare the two incidents you’ve collected. What do they have in common? For example, is the same rule broken in both? Did the audience respond to the transgression with similar remarks or actions? Take a poll of what rules were broken in all the incidents in the class. Does the gender, age, or ethnic affiliation of the participants influence what causes embarrassment? Exercise 2: The American Family This class shares some culture. Need proof? Remove a blank page from your notebook and orient it in landscape mode (long side is horizontal). Across the top write “The American Family.” Now draw a picture on that page illustrating that title. You hesitate, you chuckle, and you say, “I can’t draw.” That’s not the point I intend to make, but notice how widespread this reaction is. Most adults say, “I can’t draw,” although most drew enthusiastically when they were children. I’ve asked you to do something that is not a comfortable habit for adults and definitely odd for an anthropology class, so everyone is nervous. Exercise 2: The American Family 49 Semistructured Interview Protocol: Embarrassing Incidents Incident 1 Incident 2 1. Describe the scene: who, where, when, what was happening: 2. Identify the embarrassing event: 3. Describe the responses of the “audience”: 4. Describe the responses of the “perpetrator”: 5. Explain the cultural rule that was broken: Just draw, for at least five minutes. When everyone is finished, the pictures can be taped on the wall (in small classes) or circulated within groups (in large classes). This exercise is not a test of your artistic ability, an exercise in embarrassment, or a technique for diagnosing your neuroses. This is “auto-ethnography”—you are interviewing yourselves as informants of American culture, using a technique called “personal documentation.” To analyze these drawings for what they reveal about U.S. culture, identify the patterns of shared understandings among their images. On one level, of course, 50 Chapter One What Is Culture? each drawing is unique, like your fingerprint or your personality. Compared to people in other nations, many Americans like to be “different,” to express their individuality. But at the same time, there are a surprising number of common elements in these drawings, aren’t there? 1. What are the shared elements? 2. How widely are they shared? This is a measure of shared understandings, or how much culture the class shares. 3. Sort the drawings into a few piles that differ on some basic pattern. How many patterns are represented? 4. How would you label each of those patterns? 5. Do the patterns reflect any of the cultural and ethnic diversity of the students in the class? 6. What proportion of all the drawings does each pattern represent? 7. As for the most common pattern: how did we come to share those particular elements? Beyond replying, “It’s in our culture,” you need to suggest how this pattern got into our culture during our society’s history. For guidance, consult The Family in America: Searching for Social Harmony in the Industrial Age (Carlson 2003). 8. How did you learn this pattern that you drew? 9. You are surely aware of all the patterns represented here, so what did you “mean to say” when you chose the pattern you drew? Exercise 3: Being Multicultural As I quoted earlier in this chapter, “Most students, like other contemporary humans, are multicultural. They operate with a diverse and often contradictory set of cultural traditions” (Caughey 2002, 174). You have a diverse set of traditions because of the variety of ethnic, religious, social class, or other communities that you have belonged to. Those traditions may not fit well with each other, so you have to manage or “juggle” them. Furthermore, you have a relationship with each of these acquired traditions. You may be an advocate of the tradition, as I am of my longtime membership in the Adirondack Mountain Club. You may be a resister, as a lapsed Catholic might be. You may be in a negotiated/critical relationship with the culture, accepting it but holding it at arm’s length, picking and choosing from its repertoire of practices and ideas, as some people do with their family traditions. One’s identity is made up substantially of the way one combines these various cultures and subcultures to answer the question, Who am I? Today students tackle this question when they create personal web pages. In this exercise you will analyze the mix of cultures by which a person represents himself or herself on a personal web page. Exercise 3: Being Multicultural 51 Background If you haven’t looked at students’ personal web pages, begin this exercise by browsing among a few at your institution. If you haven’t created your own web page, sketch out what you would want yours to look like and to say. Analysis Analyze the “who am I?” aspects of two other students’ personal web pages as well as your own. The page’s author uses verbal and graphic means to announce (or hint at) characteristics such as the following, all of which suggest how the author is linked to communities with cultures. Those communities may be face-to-face (such as one’s residence group), multisited (one’s far-flung family), imagined (one’s coreligionists in America), or virtual (one’s discussion group on the Internet). 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. age gender sexual preference hometown current living conditions work racial or ethnic affiliations language political and other opinions recreational pursuits participation in organizations, clubs likes and dislikes religious or spiritual background and interests academic focus future plans 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. marital/romantic status other persons or animals central in their lives possessions central in their lives lifestyle (vegetarian, night-owl, etc.) links to other websites Notice how the personal web pages describe their authors by linking them to a specific, perhaps a unique, mix of communities, each with its own set of shared practices and ideas, lifestyles and symbols, ethnic flags, value orientations, rituals, and jargon. 52 Chapter One What Is Culture? Such self-presentations may be incorrect or incomplete, but they are fairly accurate because feedback from our peer groups who see these pages holds us to a certain standard of honesty and contributes to our self-knowledge. (“You . . . a granola? No you’re not.”) The self-presentations in web pages often emphasize the author’s distinctiveness, even uniqueness, but that emphasis on distinctiveness paradoxically reveals our shared American cultural values of individualism, selfexpression, and self-realization: we’re all alike in our efforts to be unalike. Answer these analytical and comparative questions about the multicultural character of three personal web pages, including your own page (or proposed page). • What connections to lifestyles, cultures, or subcultures can be inferred • • • • from the students’ self-descriptions? How do the other two students’ mixes of cultures compare to those that can be inferred from your web page (or proposed page)? Do you think the degree of multiculturalism evidenced in the three web pages is a recent phenomenon—that is, that young men and women in America in 1906 would not have been as multicultural? In the personal pages, are there clues to any perceived or potential contradiction among the various communities the student participates in? Perhaps her religion and her science major are potentially in disagreement about Creation and evolution. Are there clues to how the student manages this contradiction? In your web page (or proposed page), what is your relationship (advocate, resister, or negotiated/critical) to each of the cultures that can be inferred from your self-description? The product of this exercise is your personal web page or proposed web page, a page from each of the other two web pages, and answers to these questions. Exercise 4: Race Classification This exercise investigates notions of race in American culture, relying on readers of this book and the U.S. government as informants. 1. On a card or a slip of paper labeled “Humankind,” list the races of the world today. Put an asterisk (*) beside the term or terms that best describes your race. (You may describe yourself as multiracial or declare that none of these terms applies.) The lists will be collected, counted, and discussed in class. 2. On another card or slip of paper labeled “U. S.,” list the races in the United States today. For each of those groups, estimate its percentage of the U. S. population (currently about 300 million total). These slips will be collected, analyzed, and discussed in class. 3. Your instructor will provide you with the questions about race and ethnic identification asked on the current U. S. Census standard form. Answer Recommended Reading 53 these questions on a slip of paper or card labeled “Census.” These will also be collected, analyzed, and discussed in class. Recommended Reading 1. Alland’s and Shanklin’s volumes state anthropology’s view of race in useful and thoughtful ways. Race is a collection of case studies of American notions of race concerning blacks, whites, Puerto Ricans, Jews, Asian Americans, and others. Alland, Alexander. 2004. Race in Mind: Race, IQ, and Other Racisms. New York: Berg Publishers. Gregory, Steven, and Roger Sanjek, eds. 1994. Race. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Shanklin, Eugenia. 1994. Anthropology and Race: The Explanation of Differences. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. 2. De Waal criticizes humans’ “anthropocentrism,” or claim to human superiority over other primates such as apes and monkeys. On the other hand, Kemp and Smith suggest that language abilities lie along a continuum from bee to human being, with apes and monkeys in the middle. De Waal, Frans. 2001. The Ape and the Sushi Master: Cultural Reflections of a Primatologist. New York: Basic Books. Kemp, William, and Roy Smith. 1998. “Signals, Signs, and Words: From Animal Communication to Language.” In Language: Readings in Language and Culture, 6th ed., eds. Virginia Clark, Paul Eschholz, and Alfred Rosa, 658–80. Boston: Bedford/St. Martin’s. 3. A favorable treatment of the culture concept in anthropology in today’s world, including an historical summary of what the concept has meant in its two centuries of use. Kuper, Adam. 2000. Culture: The Anthropologist’s Account. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 4. Globalization hasn’t eliminated the need to think in terms of culture. Edited by anthropologists at the World Bank, this book brings together case studies by writers of international renown—including but not limited to cultural anthropologists—to demonstrate that culture is still central to economic development in many countries. Text, videos, and discussion of the book are at www.cultureandpublicaction.org. Rao, Viyayendra, and Michael Walton, eds. 2004. Culture and Public Action. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.