Download Hearing

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Hearing loss wikipedia , lookup

Auditory processing disorder wikipedia , lookup

Speed of sound wikipedia , lookup

Audiology and hearing health professionals in developed and developing countries wikipedia , lookup

Earplug wikipedia , lookup

Noise-induced hearing loss wikipedia , lookup

Evolution of mammalian auditory ossicles wikipedia , lookup

Sound from ultrasound wikipedia , lookup

Sensorineural hearing loss wikipedia , lookup

Sound wikipedia , lookup

Sound localization wikipedia , lookup

Auditory system wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Sensation and Perception
• Why does a note played on a flute sound differ­
ent from the same note on an oboe?
• If you habitually listen to loud music through
headphones, what kind of hearing impairment
are you risking?
• To locate the source of a sound, why does it
sometimes help to turn or tilt your head 7
Hearing
Like vis ion, the sense of hearing, or audition, pro­
vides a vital link with the world around us. Be­
cause social relationships rely so heavily on hearing,
when people lo se their hearing they sometimes
come to feel socially isolated. That is why many
hearing-impaired people feel strongly about teach­
ing deaf children American Sign Language (ASL) or
other gestural languages, which allow them to com­
municate with and forge close relationships with
other signers.
What We Hear
The stimulus for sound is a wave of pressure cre­
ated when an object vibrates (or when com­
pressed air is released, as in a p ipe organ). The
vibration (or release of air) causes molecules in a
transmitting substance to move together and
apart. This movement produces variations in pres­
sure that radiate in all directions. The transmitting
substance is usually air, but sound waves can also
travel through water and solids, as you know if
you have ever put your ear to the wall to hear
voices in the next room .
As with vision, physical characteristics of the
stimulus-in this case, a sound wave-are related
in a predictable way to psychological aspects of our
auditory experience:
Loudness is the dimension of auditory expe­
rience related to the intensity of a wave 's pres­
sure. Intensity corresponds to the amplitude, or
maximum height, of the wave. The more energy
contained in the wave, the higher it is at its peale
Perceived loudness is also affected by how high or
Iowa sound is. If low and high sounds produce
waves with equal amplitudes, the low sound may
seem quieter.
Sound intensity is measured in units called decibels
(dB). A decibel is one-tenth of a bel, a unit named
1
CHAPTER 6
193
for Alexander Graham Bell, the inventor of the tele­
phone. The average absolu te threshold of hearing in
human beings is zero decibels. The sound of a hum­
ming refrigerator is 50, dB; shop tools average 90, dB;
and a jet plane at 50, feet is 140, dB. Decibels are not
equally distant, as inches on a ruler are. A 6o'-decibel
sound (such as that of a sewing machine) is not 50,
percent louder than a 4o'-decibel sound (such as that
of a whisper); it is 10,0, times louder.
Pitch is the dimension of auditory experi ­
ence related to the frequency of the sound
wave and, to some extent, its intensity. Frequency
refers to how rapidly the air (or other medium)
vibrates-that is, the number of times per second
the wave cycles through a peak and a low point.
One cycle per second is known as 1 hertz (Hz). The
healthy ear of a young person normally detects
frequencies in the range of 16 Hz (the lowest note
on a pipe organ) to 20,,0,0,0, Hz (the scraping of a
grasshopper's legs).
2
11mbre is the distinguishing quality of a sound.
It is the dimension of auditory experience re­
lated to the complexity of the sound wave-to the
relative breadth of the range of frequencies that
make up the wave. A pure tone consists of only one
frequency, but in nature, pure tones are extremely
rare. Usually what we hear is a complex wave con­
sistin g of several subwaves with different frequen­
cies. A particular combination of frequencies results
in a particular timbre . Timbre is what makes a note
played on a flute, which produces relatively pure
tones, sound entirely different from the same note
played on an oboe, which produces very complex
sounds.
When many frequencies are present but are
not in harmony, we hear noise. When all the fre­
quencies of the sound spectrum occur, they pro­
duce a hissing sound called white noise. Just as white
light includes all wavelengths of the visible light
spectrum, so white noise includes all frequencies
of the audible sound spectrum. People sometimes
use white-noise machines to mask other sounds
when they are trying to sleep.
3
loudness
The dimension of auditory
experience related to the
intensity of a pressure
wave .
An Ear on the World
pitch
The dimension of auditory
experience related to the
frequency of a pressure
wave; it is related to the
height or depth of a tone.
When we look at an ear, we see oruy its external en­
trance. The ear actually has an outer, a middle, and
an inner section . The soft, funnel-shaped outer ear is
well designed to co llect sound waves, but hearing
would still be quite good without it. The essential
parts of the ear are hidden from view, inside the head.
timbre
The distinguishing quality
of a sound; the dimension
of auditory experience re­
lated to the complexity of
the pressure wave .
194
CHAPTER 6
I
Sensation and Perception
Outer Ear
,Middle,
'E
I
"""'o(,----------'~'~o(
Inner Ear
,
,
",
"Stirrup"
bone
Oval
Auditory
bone
Basilar
membrane
Middle ear cavity
Hair
cells (cilia)
FIGURE 6.8
Major Structures of the Ear
Sound waves collected by the outer ear are channeled down the auditory canal, causing the eardrum to vibrate. These vibra­
tions are then passed along to the tiny bones of the middle ear. Movement of these bones intensifies the force of the vibra­
tions and funnels them to a small membrane separating the middle and inner ear. The receptor cells for hearing (hair cells),
located in the organ of Corti within the snail-shaped cochlea , initiate nerve impulses that travel along the auditory nerve to
the brain.
~'"='-
live!..
psych
6.S
~
organ of Corti
[CORE-tee]
A structure in the
cochlea containing hair
cells that serve as the re­
ceptors for hearing.
cochlea [KOCK-Iee-uh]
A snail-shaped, flUid-filled
organ in the Inner ear,
containing the organ of
Corti, where the receptors
for hearing are located.
A sound wave passes into the outer ear and
through an inch-long canal to strike an oval-shaped
membrane called the eardrum (see Figu·re 6.8). The
eardrum is so sensitive that it can respond to the
movement of a single molecule! A sound wave
causes it to vibrate with the same frequency and
amplitude as the wave itself. This vibration is passed
along to three tiny bones in the middle ear, the
smallest bones in the human body. These bones,
known informally as the "hammer," the "anvil,"
and the "stirrup," move one after the other, which
has the effect of intensifying the force of the vi­
bration. The innermost bone, the stirrup, pushes
on a membrane that opens into the inner ear.
The actual organ of hearing is the organ of
Corti, a chamber inside the cochlea, a snail-shaped
structure within the inner ear. This organ plays the
same role in hearing that the retina plays in vision.
It contains the all-important receptor celis, which
in this case look like bristles and are called hair cells,
or cilia. Brief exposure to extremely loud noises, or
sustained exposure to more moderate ones (heavy
city traffic, factory noise, a noisy restaurant), can in­
jure these fragile cells. They flop over, like broken
blades of grass, and if the damage reaches a critical
point, hearing loss occurs. In modern societies, with
their rock concerts (120 dB in front of the speak­
ers), deafening bars, and millions of automobiles,
snowmobiles, power saws, leaf blowers, jackham­
mers, and sound systems (often played at full blast
and listened to through headphones), such dam­
age is common. Many college students already have
impaired hearing because of harm to the cilia.
The hair cells of the cochlea are embedded in
the rubbery basilar membrane, which stretches across
the interior of the cochlea. When pressure reach­
es the cochlea, it causes wavelike motions in fluid
within the cochlea's interior. These waves of fluid
push on the basilar membrane, causing it to move
in a wavelike fashion, too. Just above the hair cells
Sensation and Perception
is yet another membrane. As the hair cells rise and
fall, their tips brush against it, and they bend. This
causes the hair cells to initiate a signal that is passed
along to the auditory nerve, which then carries the
message to the brain. The particular pattern of hair­
cell movement is affected by the manner in which
the basilar membrane moves. This pattern deter­
mines which neurons fire and how rapidly they
fire, and the resulting code in turn determines the
sort of sound we hear. For example, we ctisCiirninate
high-pitched sounds largely on the basis of where
activity occurs along the basilar membrane; activ­
ity at different sites leads to different neural codes.
We discriminate low-pitched sounds largely on the
basis of the frequency of the basilar membrane's
vibration; again, different frequencies lead to dif­
ferent neural codes.
Could anyone ever imagine such a complex
and odd arrangement of bristles, fluids, and snail
shells if it did not already exist?
Constructi ng
the Auditory World
Just as we do not see a retinal image, so we do not
hear a chorus of brushlike tufts bending and sway­
ing in the dark recesses of the cochlea. Just as we
do not see a jumbled collection of lines and colors,
so we do not hear a chaotic collection of discon­
nected pitches and timbres. Instead, we use our
perceptual powers to organize patterns of sound
and to construct a meaningful auditory world.
For example, in class, your psychology instruc­
tor hopes you will perceive his or her voice as fig­
How
w~1I
CHAPTER 6
195
ure and the hum of a passing airplane, cheers from
the athletic field, or distant sounds of a construc­
tion crew as ground. Whether these hopes are re­
alized will depend, of course, on where you choose
to direct your attention. Other Gestalt principles also
seem to apply to hearing. The proximity of notes in
a melody tells you which notes go together to form
phrases; continuity helps you follow a melody on one
violin when another violin is playing a different
melody; similarity in timbre and pitch helps you pick
out the soprano voices in a chorus and hear them
as a unit; closure helps you understand a celi-phone
caller's words even when interference makes some
of the individual sounds unintelligible.
Besides needing to organize sounds, we also
need to know where they are coming from. We
can estimate the distance of a sound 's source by
using loudness as a cue. For example, we know
that a train sounds louder when it is 20 yards away
than when it is a mile off. To locate the direction a
sound is coming from, we depend in part on the
fact that we have two ears. A sound arriving from
the right reaches the right ear a fraction of a sec­
ond sooner than it reaches the left ear, and vice
versa. The sound may also provide a bit more en­
ergy to the right ear (depending on its frequency)
because it has to get around the head to reach the
left ear. It is hard to localize sounds that are com­
ing from directly in back of you or from directly
above your head because such sounds reach both
ears at the same time. When you turn or cock your
head, you are actively trying to overcome this prob­
lem. Many animals do not have to do this because
the lucky things can move their ears independently
of their heads.
can you localize the answers to these questions?
1. Which psychological dimensions of hearing correspond to the intensity, frequency, and complexity of
the sound wave?
2. Fred has a nasal voice and Ted has a gravelly voice. Which psychological dimension of hearing de­
scribes the difference?
3. An extremely loud or sustained noise can permanently damage the
of the ear.
4. During a lecture, a classmate draws your attention to a buzzing fluorescent light that you have not
previously noticed. What will happen to your perception of figure and ground?
Answers:
<I:)!OIl ~,J<lmp<ll <141 pue <lmDIJ <lwo:)<lq II!M puno~ 5ul22nq <141'17
'!\liJejU<lWOw l~e<llle 'pUnOJD <lWO:)<lq II!M
(eIIP) ~II<I:) Jle4 'C; <lJqwI1 ' 2 <lJqW!l '4:)1!d '~~<lupnol ' 1