* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Planning for Retirement Terrance K Martin Jr.* and Michael Finke
Investment management wikipedia , lookup
Household debt wikipedia , lookup
Systemic risk wikipedia , lookup
Financial economics wikipedia , lookup
Global saving glut wikipedia , lookup
Public finance wikipedia , lookup
Pensions crisis wikipedia , lookup
Financial Crisis Inquiry Commission wikipedia , lookup
Systemically important financial institution wikipedia , lookup
Planning for Retirement Terrance K Martin Jr.* and Michael Finke Department of Personal Financial Planning, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409 Abstract Most Americans have not estimated how much they need to save for retirement. This lack of awareness may contribute to low levels of retirement savings. We investigate the relation between planning and retirement wealth. Those who have estimated how much they need to save for retirement have significantly higher retirement wealth. Respondents who rely on an advisor to help plan for retirement save more than those who do it themselves and are more likely to own tax advantaged accounts. Advising services that did not involve estimation of retirement needs had no measurable value. Results suggest that planning, particularly with a comprehensive approach, improves retirement outcomes. Key words: financial planner, comprehensive approach, retirement savings, 2008 NLSY 79 Corresponding author. Tel.: 806-742-5050, ext. 239; fax: 806-742-5033. Email address: [email protected] 1 Introduction Consumption and savings decisions made in the earlier stages of the life-cycle can have a disproportionately large impact on retirement preparedness. These saving decisions have had an even greater impact on U.S. household welfare over the last two decades as the responsibility for funding retirement shifted from employers to employees (Ibbotson, Milvesky, Chen & Zhu, 2007). According to the Employee Benefit Research Institute (EBRI), only 38% of all private workers participate in employer-sponsored defined contribution plan and just 14% of Americans are confident in their ability to retire comfortably (Helman, Copeland, and VanDerhei, 2012 ). Greater employee responsibility for funding retirement means that individuals, rather pension professionals, must estimate how much saving is needed to provide an adequate retirement income (Poterba, Rauh, Venti, and Wise., 2007). A lack of financial knowledge and sophistication among many American workers contributes to inefficient retirement savings. Most American households could not maintain a constant level of consumption in retirement with their current retirement savings (Bernheim, 1992, Yuh, Montalto, & Hanna, 1998, Mitchell & Moore, 1998), and one-third of retirees obtain 90% of their income from social security (SSA, 2012). The shift in retirement funding responsibility, coupled with the complexity of the investment process, should increase demand for professional financial advice (Ibbotson, Milevsky, Chen, and Zhu, 2007). Professional financial advisors can help a household accurately estimate the amount of retirement income needed to fund their retirement goals. Additionally, a financial advisor can provide a financial plan that involves establishing the steps needed to meet a retirement goal and regular interaction to evaluate progress. A financial advisor may also increase a worker’s awareness of the consequences of low savings, reduce the psychic 2 costs of making complex choices, and help improve investment performance and tax efficiency within household portfolios. However, there is little evidence in the current literature that the use of financial advisors improves household financial outcomes. We review these articles and emphasize the importance of differentiating comprehensive advisors from professionals who are primarily brokers of financial products. We also question the use of gross investment performance in evaluating the value of professional advice. This paper contributes to the literature on the value of financial advice by evaluating the impact of financial advice on retirement outcomes including retirement wealth accumulation. We use both the 2004 and the 2008 waves of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY79) to estimate the impact of financial advice on retirement savings and the change in accumulated retirement wealth between 2004-2008. We identify respondents who do, and do not, use a comprehensive approach to retirement planning and compare the effectiveness of creating your own retirement plan versus using a professional advisor only. The remainder of the paper is separated into four sections. Section I present a brief review of literature. Section II describes the conceptual framework and presents the hypotheses. Section III describes the data and the method selection. Section IV summarizes the results and Section V concludes and comments on the implications of the research. I Review of literature Household retirement saving is motivated by a desire to smooth consumption beyond the working years. Many are not successful, leading to a welfare loss from reduced consumption in old age (Bernheim, Skinner and Weinberg, 2001). Bernheim (1992) concludes that actual savings rates are 9 to 19% lower annually than optimal levels for young household’s aged 35 to 3 451. Bernheim, Forni, Gokhale, and Kotlikoff (1999) find that the median household would need to increase their savings rate by as much as 20% to replace pre-retirement levels of consumption. Studies of employee participation in employer-sponsored retirement plans shows that many do not participate, those who participate often do so at a default savings rate that is too low, and retirement savings decisions change little over time due to an unwillingness to actively change savings amounts (Choi, Laibson, Madrian and Metrick, 2006). Individuals are bounded by their own rationality due to information asymmetry, time, and their cognitive abilities - which may result in sub-optimal choices (Simon, 1955). As such, financial knowledge is critical to the process of accumulating retirement wealth (Jacobs-Lawson & Hershey, 2005). Complex retirement decisions can be challenging for households that lack financial knowledge (Finke, Huston, & Winchester, 2011) and may also result in a household deciding to not participate in financial markets (Campbell, 2006). Behavioral biases also contribute to inadequate retirement savings and investment decisions (Bailey, Kumar, & Ng, 2011). Investors with strong behavioral biases are more likely to select investments based solely on past performance (Siri and Tufano, 1998; Barber and Odean, 2008), to use funds with higher expenses (Gil-Bazo et al., 2009), and to have higher transaction costs and tax inefficiencies due to high turnover (Barber and Odean, 2001). Further, investors with less financial sophistication select high expense mutual funds, trade frequently, chase returns, avoid index funds, and time the market (Bailey, et al., 2011). Professional Advice Households faced with the responsibility of funding their own retirement can rent the services of a financial expert to make better choices. The decision to delegate assistance involves 1 See Bernheim (1992, 1993, and 1994) for more thorough discussion of his assumptions and calculations used to estimate savings rates. 4 a tradeoff between the time and transaction costs of acquiring financial knowledge and the costs (including direct, indirect and agency costs) of hiring an advisor. A professional financial advisor makes an investment in specific human capital related to individual financial planning. Households should only hire a professional financial advisor if the expected discounted lifetime utility from using an advisor exceeds the expected utility of self-directed planning. A more comprehensive financial professional’s recommendations may incorporate any one or more of the six main planning areas: cash management, risk management, investments, retirement, tax planning, and estate planning (Warschauer, 2008). As an example, a discussion on retirement planning will include an analysis of the assets needed to generate a sufficient income replacement, a recommended pre- and post-retirement portfolio, and a regular savings plan to meet the retirement accumulation goal. Unfortunately, there is little evidence that financial advisors improve welfare outcomes. Using Dutch data, Kramer observes differences in advised and self-directed accounts but finds no evidence of increases in risk-adjusted performance (Kramer, 2012). Dahlquist et al. (2011) provide evidence that advised accounts experience lower net returns. The authors posit that trading costs contribute to outcomes, as advised accounts feature high turnover consistent with commissions being the main source of advisor income. Another study finds evidence of financial advisors being incentivize to promote equity concentrated asset allocation and conclude that there might be a need for enhanced regulative investor protection (Jansen, Fishcher, & Hackethal, 2008). Empirical evidence suggests a positive correlation between moral hazard and mutual fund usage by financial advisors (Kramer & Lensink, 2012). Zhao (2003) concludes that in the presence of moral hazard, financial advisors tend to encourage higher load funds. Further, broker-sold funds show persistent evidence of return chasing and no superior market timing 5 (Bergstresser et al., 2009). Since brokers are not required to place the interests of their clients above those of their employer, their primary objective is to maximize commissions from sales of financial products within the constraints of product suitability. Commission compensation decreases the incentive to hold investments for long periods of time in order to generate greater sales commissions and to recommend products with higher loads - which are often actively managed mutual funds with higher expense ratios and lower historical net returns. This compensation structure encourages some financial intermediaries to recommend underperforming funds (Bergstresser, et al., 2009; Stoughton, et al., 2010). Two shortfalls of the existing empirical literature include the inability to distinguish between the type of financial advisors23 and the use of investment alpha to quantify the value of advice. Professional financial advisors may improve retirement savings behavior by increasing a worker’s awareness of the consequences of low savings, by reducing search and psychic costs of making complex choices, and by improving investment performance and tax efficiency of household portfolios. In the United States, advisors can be split between two groups regulated by two different entities with a different set of compensation incentives and legal standard of care toward clients (Macey, 2002). Brokers are self-regulated by the Financial Industry Regulatory Authority according to the 1934 Securities Exchange Act. The primary service provided by registered representatives of brokers is to facilitate the transactions of financial products, and they owe their allegiance to their employer and are not required to make product recommendations that are in the best interest of clients. In fact, brokers are required to only 2 “the fiduciary duties and legal requirements imposed on particular financial intermediaries may differ substantially across jurisdiction and across the profession. For instance, broker –dealers are excluded from regulation aimed at investment advisers if they offer advice that is ‘solely incidental” to their business and receive “no special compensation” for the service.” (Inderst and Ottaviani, 2012) 3 In an SEC sponsored report, investors were asked their beliefs about the services offered by financial services professional, 91% of respondents associated financial planners with retirement planning and 88% felt that financial planners provide more general advice (Hung, et al., 2008). See Hung, et al., 2008 for further discussion. 6 make financial recommendations that are incidental to the sale of products. Registered Investment Advisors are regulated by the Securities and Exchange Commission according to the 1940 Investment Advisers Act. Nearly all RIAs are compensated based on asset fees and are required to provide a fiduciary standard of care toward their client. Many who provide comprehensive financial planning services are regulated by the SEC as investment advisers and hold themselves out as financial planners. These differences in legal standard of care and in compensation may account for predictable differences in household financial outcomes among those served by comprehensive and non-comprehensive advisors. Households that use professional financial advice execute investment strategies more consistent with the economic theory (Bodie & Crane, 1997) and show evidence of improved investment performance among households who actively seek professional advice (Hung & Yoong, 2010). Financial advisors also benefit from financial information economies of scale and are able to increase investor utility by knowing and aligning preferences (Bluethgen et al., 2008). Lusardi & Mitchell (2005) sort their sample into simple planners, serious planners, and successful planners. Forty-nine percent of successful planners (created a retirement plan) consulted a financial planner compared to 39% of simple planners (did some form of retirement calculations). Notably, those that consulted a financial professional are found to have more accurate information relative to those that spoke to family and friends. Further, financial advisors also provide access to direct distribution channels of high performing mutual funds (Stoughton, Wu, & Zechner, 2010). Shapira & Venezia (2001) provide evidence that professional financial advice can curtail some cognitive errors and behavioral biases. The authors notice that the disposition effect is lower among financial professionals. Evidence of psychic cost reduction is found in Engelmann, 7 Capra, Noussair, and Berns (2009) who use MRI scans to conclude less cognitive strain when individuals making financial decision are receiving advice. In a study at Oregon State University, Chalmers and Reuters conclude that individuals who used brokerage services are less likely to remain in the default option in retirement plans and also more likely to allocate their retirement portfolios across multiple asset classes rather than the default fund (Chalmers and Reuters, 2012). Using German bank data, Gerhardt and Hackethal find less risky and speculative trading in household portfolios (Gerhardt & Hackethal, 2009). This result is consistent with another European study which provides evidence of reduced trading activity based on a dataset of Dutch equity investors (Kramer & Lensik, 2012). Some financial advisors actively attempt to send quality signals to their clients and potential clients by increasing their bonding cost by embracing the fiduciary standard (see Spence, 1973) and by initiating contracts that increase their bonding cost and outlines a long-term client planner relationship (Finke, Huston, & Waller, 2009). II Conceptual Framework and hypotheses This paper uses the life cycle hypothesis and draws on human capital theory to evaluate the motivations of households planning for retirement. Less than 50% of US households report estimating their retirement needs. This study examines the value of financial advice by seeking to explain the impact of financial advice on retirement decisions and retirement wealth accumulation. Households that lack the financial sophistication to navigate complex financial markets and to make informed financial decision may seek out financial advisors to substitute for their human capital (Collins, 2012). The shift to defined contributions plans has placed retirement planning decisions in the hands of workers who may or may not possess the skill set and financial knowledge to make optimal decisions. Campbell (2006) identifies areas of some 8 groups that may be more prone to make investment mistakes. To avoid discontinuity of consumption at retirement, households must be willing to forego some current consumption in exchange for increased consumption during retirement years4. Being able to quantify the amount that one will need to replace income in retirement can send a strong signal of over or under-preparedness and force individuals in the retirement planning process to act. Mayer, Zick, and Marsden (2011) examine the relationship between retirement income calculations and retirement wealth accumulation. The study shows that individuals that estimated income needs in retirement self-reported increases in retirement savings. As an example, a modest 10% increase in the likelihood of calculating retirement need is related to $16,600 in additional retirement savings for respondents over 40 (Mayer, et.al, 2011). In this study, a logistic regression model is used to estimate the likelihood of calculating retirement need by controlling for the use of financial advice, the respondents own human capital, the respondents economic status, and possible cultural influences, see equation 1. Calculating retirement need = ƒ (financial advice, human capital, economic status, cultural influences.) (1.0) In another logistic regression, calculating retirement need is included as an exogenous variable to determine the relative impact on using a tax deferred saving accounts such as IRA, Keoghs etc. In this model, the financial planner and calculating retirement needed are interacted to obtain the household groups discussed below. Own IRA = ƒ (retirement motivations, human capital, economic status, cultural influences.) (2.0) 4 See Appendix for graphical representation using a simple lifecycle estimation. 9 The sample is separated into four households groups:1) households that report consulting a financial planner for retirement decision and calculating retirement need (comprehensive), 2) households that use only a financial planner for retirement decisions (planner), 3) households that only report calculating retirement need (self-directed), and 4) households that do neither (No Plan). These groups are then included as the main hypothesis variables in the study. By means of this framework, the impact of the retirement motivations is compared to assess the difference that each approach has on accumulation of retirement wealth up to 2008 and the most recent change in retirement wealth between 2004 and 2008 Accumulated retirement wealth = ƒ (retirement motivations, human capital, economic status, cultural influences.) (3.0) ∆ accumulated retirement wealth= ƒ (retirement motivations, human capital, economic status, cultural influences.) (4.0) We also include four logistic regression models estimating the likelihood of a household choosing one approach to retirement over another. III. Data and the variable selection Data This study uses data from the 2004 and 2008 administration of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY79). The survey, which was started in 1979, consists of a random sample of 12,686 people and is representative of U.S. men and women who were born between 1957 and 1965. During the 2008 survey, respondents were between 45 and 52 years old. Annual interviews were conducted by phone or in-person and were continued every second year from 1994 onward. 10 Sample A special module on retirement appeared in the 2008 administration of the NLSY79 which includes the use of professional advice. The module saw a soft introduction in the 2006 wave. To obtain the final sample, the data censored only to households that are asked whether they consulted a financial planner for retirement planning. This condition on the data reduces the sample size to 7616 respondents. Descriptive and univariate analysis focuses on the four household approaches to retirement planning and wealth accumulation:1) comprehensive (836 respondents), 2) planner (478 respondents), 3) self-directed (1006 respondents), and 4) no plan (5296 respondents). Dependent Variables The hypotheses are tested using logistic regression models and quantile regression models. The study incorporates four dependant variables to assess overall retirement preparedness. We use several questions from the NLSY79 to derive the dependant variables. To proxy whether respondents had calculated retirement need and/or had owned other tax advantaged accounts, we used the following to questions: I. “Have you [or] [Spouse/partner's name] ever calculated how much retirement income you would need at retirement?" II. “Other than the accounts you have through your workplace,) Do you [or] [Spouse/partner's name] have any money in IRAs, Keoghs, variable annuities, 529 plans, or other tax-advantaged accounts?” To derive the third dependent variable, accumulated retirement wealth as of 2008, the value of the respodnent and his/her spouse/partner’s qualified retirement accounts and tax- 11 advantaged accounts for 2008 are totaled. Likewise, the fourth dependent variable in the same manner for 2004 and 2008, with the difference (2008-2004) taken. Independent Variables Hypothesis variables (retirement planning motivators) In a special module on retirement in the 2008 administration of the NLSY79, respondents are asked if they consulted a financial planner for retirement and if they have calculated retirement wealth. These questions are used to create the household groups specified earlier in the paper. The first household group includes respondents that answered ‘yes’ to consulting a financial planner and ‘yes’ to calculating retirement need (comprehensive). The second group consists of those households that only consult a financial planner (planner); third, households that only calculated retirement need (self-directed), and fourth households that answered no to both questions (no plan). The expectation is high values of retirement savings among households that consult and a financial planner for retirement purposes and calculates retirement need. Other predictor variables Marital status was included to account for increased saving ability due to shared household resources. Education and IQ are included as measures of human capital and as a proxy for earnings path. Research suggests the attainment of higher education would result in a better understanding of investments (Coleman, 2003) and increase the chances of holding a risky asset (Haliassos & Bertaut, 1995). Health limitation is included as a control variable due to the potential shock to life cycle consumption and portfolio allocations (Rosen & Wu, 2004). Respondents with poorer health are less likely to save for retirement because of heightened liquidity needs. Empirical evidence suggests minority households show a preference for holding tangible assets and less likely to invest in risky assets (Coleman, 2003). Therefore, we control for 12 race to assess the effect preference of retirement savings. Gender is included to to account for possible differences in preferences on retirement savings. Age is included as control variable to account for differences in attitude toward savings within the cohort. Family size is included since larger families may reduce savings in order to meet current consumption needs relative to households with smaller families. The amount of household income affects life cycle behavior. If households forecast an increase in income, they may postpone saving while households who anticipate income to decrease may save more in order to smooth consumption. Households with higher expected income growth may save less early in life and increase savings later during the accumulation stage of the life cycle (Hanna, et al., 1995). Net worth is included to capture the effect of wealth on retirement savings. Households with a higher level of net worth are able to smooth consumption and are more likely to take financial risks. Home ownership is included due to the impact on households’ life-cycle behavior. Homeownership reduces saving needs if mortgage payments create home equity that can provide a stream of consumption during retirement. Likewise, business ownership may alter life cycle consumption and savings due to the liquidity needs associated with running a business. Business owners may view the business as their retirement plan and may be less likely to save for retirement. Ownership of tax advantage accounts is included to capture the effect of non-employer sponsored retirement accounts on retirement savings. Respondent risk tolerance preference is included to control for the effect on life cycle consumption. Highly risk tolerant households may forego current consumption today for higher future consumption and are generally less concerned about retirement, while less risk tolerant households prefer constant consumption with minimal consumption shocks. Finally, 13 region is included to capture possible regional differences on retirement savings and retirement planning. Hypotheses This study intends to test and answer the following hypotheses: H1: Households using professional financial advice are not likely to have calculated retirement need. Similarly, respondents are probed on their use of tax advantage accounts outside of employer specified accounts. If a private tax advantage account is used respondents are coded as 1 and 0 if they did not use them. The responses are used to test the following hypothesis: H2: Households using professional financial advice are not likely to have additional nonemployer sponsored retirement tax deferred accounts. Finally, the dataset makes it possible to calculate accumulated retirement wealth in 2008 and the change in retirement savings between 2004 and 2008. To arrive at the retirement values, the value of respondent’s and their spouse or their partners value of qualified retirement accounts and the value of other tax-advantaged accounts for both 2004 and 2008 and took the difference. The hypotheses tested are: H3: Accumulated retirement wealth is not affected by the use of a financial planner or the calculation of retirement need. H4: Change in retirement saving is not affected by the use of a financial planner or the calculation of retirement need. Quantile model justification We use quantile regression (QR) to model accumulated retirement wealth and the change in accumulated retirement wealth between 2004 and 2008. QR allows the analysis to go beyond the estimation of conditional means. A priori knowledge of the groups that tend to use 14 professional financial advice suggests that evaluating the impact of the hypothesis variables using OLS may limit the scope of the analysis. Therefore, conditional quantiles are desired to best explain the outcome variable, i.e. accumulated retirement wealth and the ∆ in accumulated retirement wealth. Also, from the previous discussion, accumulated retirement wealth variable is laden with outliers. To avoid using a variable transformation that can lead to difficult interpretation of the results, a QR can be used. The regression estimates calculated by QR are more robust against outliers in the response to measurement. Relative to an OLS technique, QR provides are more comprehensive view of the relationship between the groups of hypothesis variables, the predictor variables and accumulated retirement wealth. Last, QR allows to compares outcomes across quantiles. For example, the impact of a households comprehensive approach to retirement planning (comprehensive) at the median can be compared to higher quantiles groups that maybe more consistent with using financial advice . QR estimates the linear conditional quantile function eq. 5.0, By solving eq. 6.0, For any quantile τ ϵ (0,1). IV. Empirical Results Univariate Analysis All univariate analyses are weighted using the specified sample weightings provided by the NLSY79 for 2008. Among all households, figure 1 illustrates sample classification by households. In this study, 13% of the sample uses a comprehensive approach to retirement 15 planning, 7% use a planner only approach, while the self-directed group accounted for 14% of the sample. The vast majority of the sample, 66%, had no plan in saving for retirement. Table 1 (also see figure 2), compares mean retirement wealth (ARW) of all households. The mean retirement wealth for all households is $103,978.66. Comprehensive approach households’ ARW is higher than all other households groups. Specifically, comprehensive households report ARW of $246,797.33, while planner households report less than half, $112,668.24. The self-directed households record ARW nearly 50% higher relative to planner households, $163, 659. The no plan households have substantially less ARW, $62,087. Similar patterns are observed in the ∆ in retirement wealth between 2004 and 2008, comprehensive households record a mean changes 100% higher than all households, ∆ARW = $83,500, while ∆ARW among planner households over the four year period of $14,851, which is 50% less than the households using no plan, $28,448. Table 1 also provides results for median retirement wealth and the change in retirement wealth. Median retirement wealth for all households was a mere $1200 in 2008 with a median change of $0. Comprehensive households record higher mean retirement wealth changes compared to fin planner households, $77,568 to $30,000 respectively. At the median, planner households realized higher retirement wealth than self-directed households ($30000 to $20000, respectively). Only comprehensive households report retirement wealth changes at the median, $12,000 relative to all other households that report zero change at the median. Noteworthy, if average retirement wealth is examined closer by survey year 1994-2008, households with a comprehensive approach to retirement planning consistently record higher mean values of accumulated retirement wealth. During 1994-2004, households using the planner 16 approach are a modest second; however, between 2004 and 2008 self-directed households show a mean change of $70,000 accumulated retirement wealth (see figure 3). A more complete list of frequency data is outlined in table 2. Minority groups, black and Hispanics, only accounted for approximately 20% of the entire sample. Of the 14.11%, of black households, 8.84% took a comprehensive approach to planning for retirement, 4.87% turned only to a financial professional, and 11.75% employ a self-directed strategy, while 74.07% chose neither option. Results are similar among Hispanic households. However, Hispanics show a higher propensity to adapt a self-directed approach to retirement planning. Non black and non Hispanic households were less likely to do neither activity. Additionally, married households appear more likely to do both activities or simply calculate retirement needs on their own. Eighty-five percent of separated households in the sample did not consult a professional or took the time to calculate retirement need. As it relates to education, there are observed monotonic increases in partaking in the three retirement planning activities. Specifically, respondents that earned at least a bachelors degree are more likely to consult a financial planner and estimate their retirement need. Notably, about 62% of the sample had only a high school or some high school education and within this group in action the vast majority chose to do neither consult a financial planner nor calculate retirement need. Respondents in the top 20% of cognitive ability chose to do one or both of the retirement planning activities, 22.46% opted to consult a professional and calculate need. Among households in the second highest IQ quintile, estimate retirement need only seem a more likely option. 17 Multivariate Analysis Logistic regression models Table 3 shows results of four logistic regression models that estimate the likelihood of a household choosing one approach to retirement planning over another. Education is the strongest predictor of choosing a comprehensive retirement planning strategy. Among all four household groups, the more educated a respondent the more likely the respondent will actively plan for retirement. As an example, households with at least a Bachelors degree are 196% more likely to report using a comprehensive approach compared to households with less than a high school education, and having a graduate degree increases the likelihood of using a comprehensive approach to 273%. IQ has a stronger impact on the likelihood of receiving non-comprehensive professional financial advice, while education is a positive but weaker predictor. Greater wealth increases the likelihood of using either type of professional advisor, and of estimating retirement needs without an advisor. More educated, higher IQ, and wealthier respondents were more likely to estimate their own retirement needs. Black households are 56% more likely to choose a comprehensive approach to retirement planning relative to non-black and non-Hispanics. Black households are also 30% less likely to approach retirement without a plan. Married households are 36% more likely to choose a comprehensive approach compared to never married households, while separated households are 54% less likely to use a planner only approach compared to the same reference category. Households who report owning a tax advantage account are 165% more likely to choose a comprehensive approach and 52% more likely to implement a planner only strategy compared to households who report not owning such accounts. Investor attitude towards risk is important to self-directed households. Aggressive investors are 25% more likely to have a self-directed approach to retirement planning relative to 18 conservative households. Northern Central households are 28% more likely to use a planner only approach when compared to households in the South. Table 4 provides empirical results for two regression models i.e. the likelihood of calculating retirement need and the likelihood of owning and funding a tax advantage account (Own IRA). Recall, the hypothesis variables i.e. retirement planning motivators are derived from interactions of the raw financial planner and the calculating retirement need variables. Among households that work with a financial professional for advice, there is a 504% increase in the likelihood of calculating retirement need relative to households not using professional financial advice. Blacks are 29%more likely than non-black and non-Hispanics to attempt to calculate their retirement need. Being exposed to higher levels of education also contributes monotonically to the likelihood of estimating retirement income need. Bachelor degree holders are 86% more likely to calculate retirement need compared to households with less than high school education, while graduate degree holders are 101% more likely than the less than high school group to make the same calculations. Considering IQ, observe increases in the likely are recorded above the 40th percentile. Compared to the lowest IQ quintile group, the highest IQ households are 48% more likely to calculate retirement need. The older and wealthier a respondent is makes them more likely to calculate retirement need. Homeowners are 35% more likely than non-homeowners to make the calculation. In addition to this, owners of tax advantage accounts are 60% more likely than those not owning and funding to go through the process of calculating their retirement needs. North eastern residents appear to be less likely compared to southerners to calculate their retirement need. 19 The results of a second logistic model are also presented in table 3. First, households with a comprehensive approach to retirement planning are 232% more likely to use a tax advantage account than households with no plan. Planner households are 124% more likely to use a tax advantage account relative to No Plan households. Hispanics are 35% less likely to own a tax advantage vehicle compared to non-blacks and non-Hispanics. Blacks, on the other hand, are 48% less likely to own a tax deferred vehicle when compared to non-blacks and nonHispanics. College graduates are a 117% more likely to tax advantage of tax deferred account compares to less than high school groups. Likewise, households with highest cognitive ability are 118% more likely to use such accounts as an IRA compared to less than high school. Business owners are 72% more likely than those not owning a business to own a tax advantage account. Quantile regression models Tables 5 and 6 provide quantile regression results that estimates the relationship of predictor variables on accumulated retirement wealth (table 5) and ∆ in retirement wealth (table 6). First, across all reported quantiles, households that adapt a comprehensive approach to planning retirement show evidence of significantly higher retirement wealth accumulation across all quantiles relative to households with no plan. As an example, within the 90th quantile of accumulated retirement wealth, households using a comprehensive approach report $189,558 more in retirement wealth than households reporting not having a plan. At the median, doing both activities result in $17,162 more in retirement wealth accumulation relatively to not doing either. By implementing a self-directed approach, households appear to be strongly motivated to save for retirement. Among self-directed households at the 80th quantile, accumulated retirement wealth is $34,998 more than when compared to households with no plan. Surprisingly, statistical 20 significance only emerges at the 70Th quantile for households only using financial advice; however, in all other quantile regressions presented there is no statistical significance. Moreover, at an alpha level of 10% results suggests that Hispanics have less retirement wealth than nonHispanics and non-blacks. For example, at the 70th quantile Hispanics have $2,910 less than the reference groups. Married couples show statistical significance at the investigated quantiles. At the median, married households report $2,557 more in accumulated retirement wealth than never married households. Health limitations negatively affect reported retirement wealth at the median, 60th, and the 70th quantile. Level of education, cognitive ability, and home ownership are also important contributors to accumulated retirement wealth. Table 6, shows a similar pattern of results as described in the above paragraph. At the 90th quantile, comprehensive households realized an increase in retirement wealth of $142,553 relative to households doing neither activity. In the same way, at the 80th quantile ($70,525), at the 60th quantile ($17,718.70) increases in retirement wealth than between 2004 and 2008 compared to households actively not to seek financial advice or calculate retirement need. Like the results in table 5, simply adapting a planner only strategy proves to have no measurable impact on retirement wealth. Case-control study sample: Robustness check for endogeneity We use a case and control study as a further test for the potential endogenity in the study. We classify households that are using a comprehensive advisor as cases and derive controls from the no planning group. To do this, we use a SAS program (see Mounib and Satchi, 2000). The program developed by Mounib and Satchi (2000) allows us to matches multiple variables. We match the controls to the cases according to net worth and IQ. The program randomizes the selection of the controls. We match 21 one control per case. The initial number of cases n=836. From a sample of 5029 (no plan) we randomly select controls based on a net worth range of $25000 and a difference in IQ quintile of 1. After adjusting for some missing values the number of cases was reduced to 785. From the analysis we want to determine whether the mean of retirement wealth is different between the plan approaches, comprehensive vs. noplan. We test the mean difference using the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test because of the non-normality of retirement wealth. The mean retirement wealth for respondents with comprehensive advisor is approximately $$61,655.00 higher than those with no plan who had similar net worth ±25000 and relative similar IQ. V. Conclusions Prior research has shown that the lack of sufficient retirement savings can negatively affect consumption in retirement (Hamermesh, 1984, Bernheim et a., 2001, Skinner, 2007). The transition to defined contribution plans has transferred the financial responsibility of retirement funding and investment management to households. However, not all households possess the financial sophistication needed to efficiently plan for retirement. Financially unsophisticated households and households with other behavioral biases can benefit from the use of a financial intermediary such as a comprehensive financial planner. Although there is little evidence in the prior literature that financial advisors provide value, most of these studies focus on investment performance within accounts of clients who use non-comprehensive advisors. Our results show important outcome differences between households who are more likely to use a comprehensive financial advisor to create a retirement savings plan and clients who use 22 an advisor but do not calculate retirement needs. Even though both groups include wealthier, more educated, and higher IQ households, there are consistent and economically significant differences in retirement savings. Using a quantile regression to estimate the marginal differences in accumulation at various levels of retirement savings, we find that using a comprehensive advisor increases retirement savings by from $190,000 at the 90th percentile to $17,000 for the median household. Consistent with findings from previous studies, we find no significant increase in retirement saving among those who use a non-comprehensive advisor. These results demonstrate the importance of differentiating between advisors whose primary objective is to sell a (often underperforming) financial product versus advisors who create a comprehenive plan. We also attempt to distinguish between households approaches to planning for retirement. We do this by comparing households that a comprehensive approach, planner only approach, self-direct approach, and a no plan approach. From the results of the logistic regression models, race, education, IQ, business ownership, and risk tolerance are important to a households decision to select a retirement planning strategy. Notbaly, being Black5, being married, being highly educated, and being willing to take financial risk are some of the key determinants of a households’ decision to choose a comprehensive approach to planning retirement. Among planner households, education and IQ are also important; however, a respodnent’s race and risk profile are of no measurable value. Education and being in the highest quintile of IQ are the main characteristic that drive a households decision to take a self-directed do it yourself approach to planning for retirement. 5 Results are similar to Hanna (2011) who found that Black households are more likely to demand the services of a financial planner. 23 The use of financial professional overwhelmingly increases the likelihood that households will go through the process of calculating retirement need. The process forces households to truly sit and and evaluate where they see themselves in the future, as well as to prioritise and quantify their retirement goals and objectives. Furthermore, our results show that adapting a comprehenive, planner, or self-directed strategy may increase the likelihood of owning a tax advantage account. By taking advantage of the tax benefits from participating in these accounts, households are better able to maximise accumulation of retirement wealth. . From the quantile regression estimate, there is observe retirement wealth gains among households that own and fund tax advantage vehicles across reported quantiles. One potential criticism of the use of accumulated wealth as an outcome variable is the possibility that planners target higher-wealth clients after they have already become wealthy. We address this first by focusing on savings in sheltered retirement accounts which are less likely to be affected by sudden exogenous increases in wealth such as an inheritance. We also investigate the change in retirement retirement wealth between 2004 and 2008 in order to measure active saving during this time period. Our results are the same. We find that the use of a comprehensive approach is associated with significant increases in retirement savings accumulated during this four year period, while there is no signicant increase associated with the use of a planner only strategy. We also find that a self-directed approach (only calculating retirement wealth needs) falls short of the monetary gains of incorporating the use of a financial professional. The use of a comprehensive approach is found to consistently result in higher retirement wealth across all quantiles, but the use of a comprehensive approach in conjunction with a professional financial planner has nearly twice the magnitude of effect at each quintile of retirement savings. For 24 example, at the 90th percentile households who used a comprehensive advisor saved an additional $142,000 more for retirement all else equal, while those who had estimated their retirement needs but did not use a professional had accumulated $70,000 more in retirement savings. These results relate to the literature on the importance of planning in achieving longterm goals. For example, there is evidence that goal specificity increases likelihood of success (Locke & Latham, 1990) and that making a process more concrete (less abstract) increases likelihood you will follow through (McCrea et al., 2008). The process of estimating retirement needs necessarily involves the consideration of a future amount needed to fund retirement spending. Estimation of this amount can increase a household’s awareness of the consequences of failing to save and provides a more defined path to meet retirement goals. 25 References Bailey, W., Kumar, A., & Ng, D. (2011). Behavioral Biases of Mutual Fund Investors. Journal of Financial Economics , 1-27. Beshears, J., Choi, J., Laibson, D., & Madrian, B. C. (2006). The importance of default options for retirement savings outcomes: Evidence from the United States. NBER Working paper, 12009,. Becker, G. S. (1964). A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis with Special Reference to Education. New York: Comlumbia University Press. Bergstresser, D., Chalmers, J., & Tufano, P. (2009). Assessing the Costs and Benefits of Brokers in the Mutual Fund Industry. The Review of Financial Studies . Bernartzi, S., Previtero, A., & Thaler, R. (2011). Annuitization Puzzles. Journal of Economic Perspectives, Forthcoming. Bernheim, D. (1992). Is the baby boom generation preparing adequately for retirement. Bi, L., & Hanna, S. (2006). Do Financial Planners Serve the Interest of Their Clients’ Use of Financial Planner, Credit Card Balances and Liquid Assets. Consumer Interests Annual , 292-314. Bigel, K. S. (1998). The Correlations of professionalization and compensation sources with the Ethical Development of Personal Investment Planners. Financial Services Review , 223236. Bluethgen, R., Gintschel, A., Hackethal, A., & Mueller, A. (2008). Financial Advice and Individual Investors' Portfolios. SSRN , Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=968197. Bodie, Z., & Crane, D. B. (1997). Personal investing: advice, theory, and evidence. Financial Analyst Journal , 13-23. Campbell, J. (2006). Household Finance. Journal of Finance , 1553-1604. Chalmers, J., & Reuter, J. (2012). What is the Impact of Financial Advisors on Retirement Portfolio Choices and Outcomes? . SSRN , Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1785833. Cheng, P., Ibbotson, R., Milvesky, M., & Zhu, K. X. (2006). Human Capital, Asset Allocation and Life Insurance. Financial Analyst Journal , 97-109. 26 Choi, J., Laibson, D., Madrian, B. & Metrick, A. (2006). Saving for Retirement on the Path of Least Resistance. In E. McCaffrey and J. Slemrod (eds.), Behavioral Public Finance, Russell Sage, 2006. Coleman, S. (2003). Risk Tolerance and the Investment Behavior of Black and Hispanic Heads of Household. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning , 43-52. Collins, J. Michael, Financial Advice: A Substitute for Financial Literacy? (April 25, 2012). Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2046227 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2046227 Diamond, P. (1977). A Framework for Social Security Analysis. Journal of Public Economics, 56-70. Diamond, P. A., & Hausman, J. A. (1984). Individual Retirement and Savings Behavior. Journal of Public Economics, 81-114. Diamond, P. (2004). Social Security. The American Economics Review , 1-24. Elder, H. W., & Rudolph, P. (1999). Does Retirement Planning Affect the Level of Retirement Satisfaction? Financial Services Review , 117-127. Engelmann, J.B., Capra, C.M., Noussair, C., & Berns, G.S. (2009). Expert financial advice neurobiologically offloads financial decision-making under risk, PLoS ONE, 4(3): e4957. Financial Planning Process. (2012). Retrieved July 29, 2012, from Certified Financial Planner Board of Standards, Inc: http://www.cfp.net/learn/knowledgebase.asp?id=2 Finke, M., Huston, S., & Waller, W. (2009). Do Contracts Influence Comprehensive Financial Advice? Financial Services Review , 177-193. Finke, M., Huston, S., & Winchester, D. D. (2011). Financial Advice: Who Pays. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning , 18-26. Gerhardt, R., & Hackethal, A. (2009). The Influence of Financial Advisors on Household Portfolios: A Study on Private Investors Switching to Financial Advice. SSRN , Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1343607. Haliassos, M., & Bertaut, C. (1995). Why Do So Few Hold Stocks? The Economic Journal , 1110-1129. 27 Hanna, S., & Lindamood, S. (2010). Quantifying the Economic Benefits of Personal Financial Planning. Financial Services Review , 111-127. Hanna, S., Fan, J., & Chang, Y. (1995). Optimal Life Cycle Savings. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning , 2-15. Heaton, J., & Lucas , D. (2000). Portfolio Choice and Asset rices: The Importance of Entrepreneurial Risk. Journal of Finance , 1163-1198. Hung, A., & Yoong, J. (2010). Asking for Help: Survey and Experimental Evidence on Financial Advice and Behavior Change. RAND Working Paper Series WR-714-1 , Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1532993. Hung, A. A., Clancy, N., Dominitz, J., Tolley, E., Berrebi, C., & Suvankulov, F. (2008). Investor and Industry Perspectives on Investment Advisers and Broker-Dealers. RAND. Ibbotson, R., Milvesky, M., Chen, P., & Zhu, K. (2007). Lifetime financial advice: human capital, asset allocation, and insurance. Jacobs-Lawson, J. M., & Hershey, D. A. (2005). Influence of Future Time Perspective, Financial Knwoledge, and Financial Risk Tolerance on Retirement Saving Behaviors. Financial Services Review , 331-344. Jansen, C., Fishcher, & Hackethal, A. (2008). The Influence of Financial Advice on the Asset Allocation of Individual Investors. EFA 2008 Athens Meetings Paper. Kramer, M., & Lensink, R. (2012). The impact of Financial Advisors on the Stock Portfolios of Retail Investors. SSRN , Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2021883. Li, J., Phillips Montalto, C., & Geistfeld, L. V. (1996). Determinants of Financial Adequacy for Retirement. Financial Counseling and Planning , 39-48. Lusardi, A., & Mitchell, O. S. (2009). How Ordinary Consumers Make Complex Economic Decisions: Financial Literacy and Retirement Readiness. NBER . Mitchell, O. S., & Moore, J. (1998). Can Americans Afford to Retire? New Evidence on Retirement Savings Adequacy. Journal of Risk adn Insurance , 371-400. Mitchell, O., Lusardi, A., & Curto, V. (2009). Financial Literacy and Financial Sophistication Among Older Americans. Pension Research Council. Poterba, J., Rauh, J., Venti, S., & Wise, D. (2007). Defined contribution plans, defined benefit plans, and the accumulation of wealth. Journal of Public Economics, 91(10), 2062-2086. 28 Rosen, H., & Wu, S. (2004). Portfolio Choice and Health Status. Journal of Financial Economics, 457-484. Helman, R., Copeland, C., VanDerhei, J. (2012). 2012 Retirement confidence survey: Job insecurity, debt weigh on retirement confidence, savings. EBRI Issue Brief, no. 369. Mounib, E., & Satchi, T. (2000). Automating the selection of controls in case-control studies. In SAS Users Group International 25th Annual Conference. Shapira, Z., & Venezia, I. (2001). Patterns of Behavior of Professionally Managed and Independent Investors. Journal of Banking and Finance, 1573-1587. Simon, H. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99-118. Skinner, J. (2007). Are You Sure You're Saving Enough for Retirement? Journal of Economics Perspectives, 59-80. Spence, M. (1973). Job market signaling. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 87(3), 355-374. Stoughton, N., Wu, Y., & Zechner, J. (2010). Intermediated Investment Management. Journal of Finance, 947-980. Sunden, A. E., & Surette, B. J. (1998). Gender Differences in the Allocation of Assets in Retirement Savings Plans. The American Economic Review, 207-211. Willis, L. E. (2008). Against financial literacy education. Iowa Law Review, 94, Yuh, Y., Monalto, C., & Hanna, S. (1998). Are Americans Prepared for Retirement? Journal of Financial Counselling and Planning. Zhao, X. (2003). The Role of Brokers and Financial Advisors Behind Investments into Load Funds. SSRN, Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=438700 29 Figure 1 percent of household within each household group in the study. Source National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, 2008 Administration 30 Graph showing Mean Accumulated Retirement wealth and mean ∆ in accumulated reitrement wealth by housheold groups. $250,000.00 $200,000.00 $150,000.00 $100,000.00 $50,000.00 $All HHs Comprehensive HHs Planner HHs Retirement Wealth 2008 Self-directed HHs No Plan HHs ∆ in Retirement Wealth Figure 2 showing mean accumulated retirement wealth and mean change in retirement wealth by household groups. Source National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, 2008 Administration 31 Figure 3 showing average retirement wealth by survey year and by household groups. Source National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, 2008 Administration 32 Table 1 shows mean and median values of household’s economic data by group. N Income Net Worth Retirement Wealth 2008 ∆ in Retirement Wealth Income Net Worth Retirement Wealth ∆ in Retirement Wealth Full Sample 7616 Planner HHs Comprehensive HHs 836 Self-Directed HHs 1006 478 No Plan 5296 $ 75,183.96 $ MEAN 125,043.41 *** $ 101,677.28 *** $ 86,577.02 *** $ 60,118.67 $ 347,417.27 $ 798,630.15 *** $ 578,582.34 *** $ 400,752.68 *** $ 221,893.33 $ 103,978.66 $ 246,797.33 *** $ 112,668.24 ** $ 163,659.69 *** $ 62,087.12 $ 41,493.27 $ 83,500.34 *** $ 14,851.58 $ 77,597.58 *** $ 28,448.60 $ 58,000.00 $ MEDIAN 96,700.00 $ 84,400.00 $ $ 142,000.00 $ 472,500.00 $ 267,525.00 $ 1,400.00 $ 77,568.00 $ $ 12,000.00 $ - ***,**,* indicates significance at the 0.01, 0.05, and 0.1 levels, respectively Source National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, 2008 Administration 33 $ 71,600.00 $ 45,200.00 $ 210,000.00 $ 82,750.00 30,000.00 $ $ - - $ $ - 20,000.00 - Table 2 provides a list of frequencies of several households’ characteristics. Full Sample Comprehensive HHs Planner HHs Self=Directed HHs No Plan 836 478 1006 5296 % % % % 14.11 6.51 79.38 8.84 7.24 14.31 4.87 4.36 7.94 11.75 13.22 14.41 74.07 74.82 63.01 12.51 63.06 3.9 19.01 1.51 8.13 15.82 4.03 9.08 10.64 6.25 8.6 1.79 4.85 5.24 11.22 15.36 8.32 12 17.37 73.96 59.85 85.67 73.62 66.51 9.92 51.64 12.01 17.04 8.38 2.76 7.89 14.94 24.84 29.67 2.78 6.25 8.11 9.95 12.39 8.84 12.61 16.6 16.98 18.32 84.91 72.88 59.95 48.02 39.42 11.66 14.88 18.39 23.57 27.41 3.59 5.71 10.3 14.26 22.46 2 4.61 6.49 9.01 10.57 8.44 11.55 12.17 15.06 17.65 85.01 77.91 70.67 61.61 48.89 50.8 49.2 13.04 13.04 7.56 6.98 14.82 13.07 64.31 66.4 72.77 6.67 23.35 15.69 27.14 28.53 8.71 13.53 11.77 15.96 13.3 17.92 59.36 45.25 41.29 South West 16.29 28.13 37.24 17.44 11.89 14.28 12.01 14.6 8.23 8.5 6.03 7.15 14.33 13.33 13.77 14.92 65.02 63.46 67.89 62.99 Health 10.4 7.3 3.23 11.9 76.61 N VARIABLE 7616 % RACE Black Hispanic Non Black, Hispanic Marital Status (ref: ) Never Married Married Separated Divorce Widowed Education Less than high school High School Some College College Graduate School IQ Lowest IQ IQQ2 IQQ3 IQQ4 Highest IQ Gender Male Female Asset ownership Homeowner Business Owner Own Ira Region North East North Central Other ***,**,* indicates significance at the 0.01, 0.05, and 0.1 levels, respectively Source National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, 2008 Administration 34 Table 3 Likelihood of choosing each retirement planning approach. Comprehensive Households 7616 VARIABLE N Education (Less than High School ref.) High School 1.42 * Some College 2.21 *** College 2.955 *** Graduate School 3.734 *** IQ (ref IQ lowest) IQQ2 1.213 IQQ3 1.878 *** IQQ4 1.852 *** Highest IQ 2.051 *** 1.005 Log_Income 1.064 ** Log net worth Health 0.813 Male 1.008 Age 1.038 ** Family size 0.999 Homeowner 1.218 * Business Owner 1.767 *** Own IRA 2.651 *** RISK PROFILE (ref Conservative) Aggressive 1.268 ** Moderate 1.311 ** REGION (ref. South) NE 0.789 NC 1.126 W 1.068 RACE (ref Non Black, Hispanic) BLACK 1.557 ** HISPANIC 0.936 Marital Status (ref: never married) Married 1.361 ** Separated 0.742 Divorce 1.226 Widowed 1.124 Pseudo R-Square 19.75% ***,**,* indicates significance at the 0.01, 0.05, and 0.1 levels, respectively Source National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, 2008 Administration Planner Households 7616 1.409 1.597 1.665 2.029 1.715 2.013 2.294 2.217 1.011 1.053 1.051 1.081 0.976 1.025 1.254 1.578 1.528 * ** ** ** ** ** ** ** * ** ** 0.918 0.878 1.28 1.282 1.078 Self-directed Households 7616 1.231 1.537 1.463 1.444 1.098 1.176 1.265 1.407 1.006 1.03 1.051 1.081 1.025 0.963 1.411 0.845 1.147 1.247 1.104 * ** ** ** * ** ** ** 0.764 0.541 0.437 0.336 ** *** *** *** 0.826 0.645 0.594 0.51 0.994 0.95 1.087 0.95 0.977 1.013 0.74 0.614 0.423 ** *** *** *** 0.784 0.851 ** ** *** * *** *** *** 0.918 0.878 1.099 1.076 0.927 0.892 1.196 0.882 1.16 1.119 0.697 0.967 *** 0.959 0.464 ** 0.763 1.017 8% 1.095 0.922 1.047 1.308 4% 0.829 1.318 0.962 0.774 21% ** * 35 * ** No Plan Households 7616 Table 4 shows logistic regression results of the likelihood to calculate retirement need and owing tax advantage accounts. VARIABLE Financial Planner Motivation (No Plan) Comprehensive HHs Planner HHs Self-Directed HHs N Calculate Retirement Need 7616 6.04 *** - Own IRA 7616 3.32 2.24 1.72 *** *** *** 0.52 0.65 *** *** 1.12 0.73 0.98 1.27 * RACE (ref Non Black, Hispanic) Black Hispanic Marital Status (ref: never married) Married Separated Divorce Widowed Education (Less than High School ref.) High School Some College College Graduate School IQ (ref IQ lowest) IQQ2 IQQ3 IQQ4 Highest IQ Log Income Log net worth Health Male Age Family size Homeowner Business owner Own IRA RISK PROFILE (ref Conservative) Aggressive Moderate 1.29 1.11 ** 1.19 0.95 1.14 1.31 1.23 1.67 1.86 2.01 1.07 1.26 1.30 1.48 1.01 1.04 1.03 1.07 1.04 0.97 1.35 1.06 1.60 1.32 1.23 * ** *** *** ** ** ** ** ** ** 1.22 1.50 2.17 2.19 1.19 1.35 1.94 2.18 0.99 1.11 0.65 0.81 1.06 0.97 1.60 1.72 *** ** ** 1.35 1.42 ** *** *** ** *** *** *** ** ** ** * *** *** ** ** REGION (ref. South) NE 0.83 ** 1.53 *** NC 0.92 1.26 *** W 1.10 1.23 ** Pseudo R-Square 18.21% 18.51% ***,**,* indicates significance at the 0.01, 0.05, and 0.1 levels, respectively Source National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, 2008 Administration 36 Table 5 provides quantile regression beta estimates for retirement wealth at the 90th, 80th, 70th, 60th, and median quantiles. VARIABLE Motivation (ref. No Plan) Comprehensive HHs Planner HHs Self-Directed HHs RACE (ref Non Black, Hispanic) Black Hispanic Marital Status (ref: never married) Married Separated Divorce Widowed Education (Less than High School ref.) High School Some College College Graduate School IQ (ref IQ lowest) IQQ2 IQQ3 IQQ4 Highest IQ Log Income Log net worth Health Male Age Family size Homeowner Business owner Own IRA N Quantile Regressions; Accumulated Retirement Wealth 0.8 0.7 0.6 7589 7589 7589 0.9 7589 189558 2087.24 61049.6 *** ** -3030.77 -2637.86 64480.6 -208.5 -1973 461.698 510.002 3242.07 125115 141567 -427.499 1501.56 36770.8 141049 247.331 5063.86 -945.19 2438.89 517.653 664.863 22968 -36120.2 73335.6 107809 11675.3 34998.3 -3205.1 -4800.7 *** *** *** 31250.2 -260.81 -2406.6 -5363.7 -257.11 2862.94 98265.4 107082 ** *** ** -15.588 3281.86 7752.18 86348.1 463.163 2101.94 -1397.9 4128.05 746.849 1557.4 12869.3 -17439 50445.5 ** * ** *** *** *** ** 66283.3 13924 19655.1 *** * ** 44331.5 7585.25 7930.25 * -2496.3 -2910.9 * -512.6 -706.91 *** *** *** 15791.8 -851.07 -3094.1 -5076.2 158.919 3139.37 54735.2 70451.2 *** ** * 6945.35 -519.86 -1582.9 -4116.6 *** *** 522.864 1901.01 35156 51172.1 *** ** -16.149 -103.12 *** ** ** 2557.51 -247.68 -823.9 -1507.3 *** *** 110.784 574.216 16390 39369.4 ** *** ** ** ** ** *** *** 34717.4 16284.5 ** ** 11481.1 8665.33 ** ** 4122.23 4924.39 ** ** 1464.72 1905.32 ** ** 5461.22 2730.29 -1538 ** 5081.12 2850.01 -1131.5 ** * 3914.12 2205.79 -985.55 *** ** 1326.13 1083.42 -439.48 ** ** * *** *** *** ** -91.392 -42.198 1706.54 18954.2 196.062 237.473 -554.09 -286.26 56.8444 243.659 1065.61 -3169.7 10030.9 *** ** ** ** ** ** ** ** *** * ** ** 17162.1 4585.28 2218.01 -1390.5 1327.54 6066.85 53353.6 597.61 1121.71 -2693.6 1710.26 435.488 1129.3 8276.87 -11385 27699.9 *** ** *** -307.72 183.966 5948.12 35062.2 379.207 661.504 -1511 -106.91 178.43 665.848 4189.82 -7461.5 13969.6 *** Median 7589 ** *** *** *** ** ** *** *** *** ** ** ** ** RISK PROFILE (ref Conservative) Aggressive Moderate 60049.9 21574.7 REGION (ref. South) NE 2589.59 NC 886.293 W -183.262 ***,**,* indicates significance at the 0.01, 0.05, and 0.1 levels, respectively Source National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, 2008 Administration 37 Table 6 provides quantile regression beta estimates for change retirement wealth at the 90th, 80th, 70th, 60th, and median quantiles. VARIABLE Motivation (ref. No Plan) Comprehensive HHs Planner HHs N Quantile Regressions; ∆ Accumulated Retirement Wealth 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.9 7062 7062 7062 7062 142553 776.258 70137.5 *** Self-Directed HHs ** RACE (ref Non Black, Hispanic) Black -2746.22 Hispanic -4060.72 Marital Status (ref: never married) Married 36816.4 *** Separated 969.662 Divorce -624.261 Widowed -757.877 Education (Less than High School ref.) High School -200.596 Some College 1030.88 College 112482 *** Graduate School 126175 *** IQ (ref IQ lowest) IQQ2 -932.537 IQQ3 1567.33 IQQ4 13185 Highest IQ 75574 *** Log Income 234.71 Log net worth 3224.98 *** Health -867.206 Male 3154.54 Age 533.049 Family size 1233.18 Homeowner 13920.3 ** Business owner -23139.6 Own IRA 35776.9 ** RISK PROFILE (ref Conservative) Aggressive 51349.5 Moderate 15303.6 REGION (ref. South) NE 4297.91 NC 2420.89 W -656.806 ***,**,* indicates significance at the 0.01, 0.05, and 0.1 levels, respectively Source National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, 2008 Administration 70525 5339.59 24388 *** ** -473.16 -1492.8 17903.4 -5.3956 -1381.5 -2064.6 -457.98 988.471 59804.2 77086.7 281.648 1670.97 4677.99 39468.5 315.068 1385 -1477 2496.26 398.451 795.124 5607.84 -7240.3 29652.7 34444.6 2490.13 9330.86 *** ** 252.464 -363.04 *** *** *** *** *** ** ** 8312.79 -606.75 -959.29 -1455 179.956 1054.15 39608.7 47197.8 -891.12 427.17 2954.21 25203 328.741 533.508 -1246.3 1084.35 95.0878 345.513 2642.23 -7060.1 11541.5 17718.7 3737.26 1992.89 *** ** -153 -131.93 *** *** *** *** *** *** ** 1921.11 -321.03 -368.06 -929.48 -21.255 369.114 20509.8 27824.3 -180.55 3.0087 976.634 14517 137.303 140.925 -249.65 103.51 2.7609 130.865 399.717 -2731.7 2421.56 17009.1 3153.58 3607.51 2548.57 648.213 843.063 2506.24 1517.77 -438.37 1041.17 1487.94 -524.35 483.778 826.102 -391.28 38 Median 7062 6000 0 0 *** ** 0 0 *** *** *** *** *** *** ** ** ** ** 0 0 0 0 0 0 100 5100 0 0 0 3900 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 *** *** *** *** ** Table 7 showing results of the case control study. Analysis of Variance for Variable RW Classified by Variable COMP COMP N Mean 1 785 215309.233 0 785 157644.092 Source DF Sum of Squares Mean Square Among 1 1295958888425 1.295959E12 Within 1557 2.452497906E14 1.575143E11 39 F Value Pr > F 8.2276 0.0042