* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Genetic modelling: an analysis of a colour polymorphism in the
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
<onlogical Journal o j the Linnean .Socie(y i 19841, 80: 4 3 7 4 4 5 . With 2 figures Genetic modelling: an analysis of a colour polymorphism in the Snow Goose (Anser caerulescens) BRUCE RATTRAY AND FRED COOKE Department of Biolou, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada Received M a y 1983, accepted f o r puhlication August 1983 Previous studif\ of colour polymorphism in the Snow Goose ! , h e r cnerulescenJ failed to considrr and reject alternative hypothrses to that of a single locus with incomplete dominance. Utilizing data from a long-term study of a wild population, we test the validity of these earlier results by considering two alternatives: (1; a single locus with multiple allelism and (2) a threshold polygenic system. O u r analyses corroborate the original model, but emphasize the importance of testing all plausible hypotheses. KEY WORDS: Lesser Snow Geese - genetic modelling - polymorphism ~ Anser caerulescens CONTENTS Introduction . . . . . . . Methods . . . . . . . . Genetic models . . . . . . Data collection and analysis . . . Results . . . . . . . . . Single locus with incomplete dominance . Single locus with an allelic series , Threshold polygenic . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . Acknowledgemrnts . . . . . . Referencrs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 437 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 438 -138 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . M.3 44.j . . . . . . . . . . -145 . 4-10 441 ,441 -141 4-12 INTRODUCTION 'The mechanisms of plumage and coat coloration have been known for some time (see Fox, 1976), although, the genetic systems controlling these processes have been more difficult to determine. I n most cases the proposed mechanism has not been shown to provide a unique explanation for the observed pattern of inheritance. Indeed, Bergston & Owen (1973) note that equally adequate alternatives are often distinguished solely on the basis of presumed differences in the simplicity of the hypothesis (i.e. Occam's razor). Claiming expediency as justification, it has become common practice to shortcut the comprehensive definition of potential hypotheses by jumping to those considered, a p i o n , to be 002'C4082/84/040437 + 09 $03 011 4i7 0 1984 T h r Linntan Societ) 01 London 438 BY RATTRAY AND F. COOKE most likely. Studies of wild populations exacerbate this problem. Due to increased logistical difficulties, the field worker is willing to be ‘realistic’ and test one or two of the more plausible hypotheses. Unfortunately, even if this best guess proves to be an adequate explanation, the issue remains unresolved until the alternatives are tested and rejected (Medawar, 1969; Willson, 1981; Grant & Price, 1981). I n this paper we address the problem of the adequacy and uniqueness of an hypothesis by resolving potential genetic models for a colour polymorphism in the snow goose (Anser caerulescens). Earlier work on this species has suggested that a single locus, two allele system with incomplete dominance controls the polymorphism and plumage variation (Cooke & Cooch, 1968). However, faced with the limitations of a field study, researchers failed to rule out a polygenic mode of inheritance. Edwards (1960) discussed the conditions under which a polygenic model may mimic the pattern of inheritance expected from a major gene(s). Indeed, so general is the explanatory power of the threshold polygenic model that, although it is often invoked when nothing else appears to fit, it is seldom considered (and then rejected) when something else will do. I n this study of a different population, we tested the validity of the conclusions of Cooke & Cooch’s previous (1968) study. I n that study, they postulated the simplest genetic model consistent with the data without ruling out alternative more complex explanations. Below, we consider not only their original hypothesis, but also examine two alternatives-single locus with multiple allelism and a threshold polygenic model. METHOD Genetic models Since 1968, a long term study ofthe population biology ofthe snow goose has been in operation a t La Perouse Bay (58”24’N, 94’24’W) 25 miles east of Churchill, Manitoba. The data used in this paper were collected between 1973 and 1979, and sufficient information is available to allow the testing of multiple allelic and polygenic models. The Snow Goose is a colonial nester, with the La Perouse Bay colony varying in size from two to four thousand breeding pairs. I n addition to a colour dimorphism manifested in both adults and goslings (white and blue), continuous variation exists in the extent of pigmentation on the belly of adults of the blue colour phase. This variation may be quantified using a modified version of the scoring system suggested by Cooke & Cooch (1968). Blue phase birds with all white bellies are classified as class 2, birds with all dark bellies, breast and lower neck are class 6. Classes 3, 4, and 5 are arbitrary intermediates with increasing amounts of dark plumage on the underside (see Fig. 1). Goslings, however, may be scored only as either white or blue. Figure 1 . Plumage coloration of snow geese. Two plumage types exist: white (A) and blue (B-F). Although a continuum exists in the amount of pigmentation found on the belly of blue adults, colour classes may be assigned to quantify this variation: A, Colour class 1 , B, colour class 2; C, 3; D, 4; E, 5; F, 6. 440 B. RATTRAY AND F. COOKE The hypotheses may be stated as follows. (1) Single locus with incomplete dominance-Cooke & Cooch (1968) suggested that the polymorphism is controlled by a single locus with two alleles and incomplete dominance. Hence ‘Blue’ birds are either homozygous dominant (BB) or heterozygotes (Bb). As ‘B’ is incompletely dominant with respect to ‘b’, darker bellied birds are predominantly but not exclusively homozygous dominant (plumage classes 5 and 6). Heterozygotes are phenotypically light bellied (classes 2 to 4) and white birds are homozygous recessive. (2) Single locus with multiple allelism-the plumage variation within the blue colour phase may also be explained by the existence of an allelic series at a single locus. Hence, the plumage continuum could be a consequence of the interaction of a set of ‘n’ alleles; the simplest case consists of three alleles and a dosage effect. For example: ‘go’does not produce any pigmentation, while ‘B” and ‘B2’produce X , and X, units of pigment, respectively. The production of any one allele is then cumulative with respect to the production of the second allele found at the locus. (3) Threshold polygene-if we assume additive interactions among loci, variation within the blue phase may be accounted for by the small, cumulative effects of ‘k’ loci. However, the explanation of the polymorphism itself requires the existence of a threshold such that individuals exceeding this critical value exhibit the blue phase plumage. The amount of pigmentation on the belly will then increase as the extent to which the threshold is exceeded becomes larger. Data collection and analysis The field season runs from early May to August, with two principal periods of data collection. At hatch (late June) the phenotypes of both parents and their goslings are scored and recorded. In August, during their flightless moult, adults and young of the year are marked with a coded colour band. This allows for individual identification of birds over successive years. This system has been employed since 1972; currently over one-third of the colony residents are individually marked. Cooke & Mirsky (1972) have previously demonstrated that certain aspects of the breeding biology of this species, principally non-random mating, precludes the use of gene frequency analysis. Moreover, they observed the phenomenon of two or more females laying eggs in the same nest. Such intraspecific parasitism (or dumping) results in families of questionable genetic integrity. Hence, when examining pedigrees a confounding effect is introduced, allowing for the ad hoe explanation of anomolous observations. Consequently, two of the more traditional techniques for genetic analysis (gene frequency and pedigrees) cannot be utilized in this study. However, if we consider all possible mating combinations as disjunct classes, we may tabulate the results of individual pedigrees to provide an offspring colour ratio per mating type (i.e. the ratio of the number of white to blue goslings per mating type). If we assume that the probability of a female either parasitizing or being parasitized is independent of her colour class, then although the values of the ratio per mating type may change, this will have no effect on comparisons among mating types. Therefore, any trends existing in the colour offspring ratios will be independent of the dump phenomenon. Finally, as COLOUR POLYMORPHISM I N SNOW GEESE 44 1 Mirsky (1972) and Finney (1975) have shown that larger clutches are more likely to contain dumped eggs, nests with more than five eggs were excluded from the analyses. RESULTS Single locus with incomplete dominance In a Blue by Blue cross involving two heterozygote birds (Bb), one would expect a 3 : 1 (blue to white) ratio in the offspring. Considering a set of randomly sampled Blue x Blue crosses, this ratio (now calculated for the population) should vary from 3 : 1 (blue to white) to zero, depending upon the proportion of heterozygotes present. Since the model postulates incomplete dominance, the proportion of heterozygotes present in light bellied birds should approach 100°/o and then decline to some low frequency in the dark bellied plumage classes. This gradient should be reflected in the offspring colour ratio when calculated per plumage class (e.g. 3 x 3, 4 x 4).Table 1 gives the results of such a set of crosses and clearly shows the predicted trend (P<0.05). Cooke & Cooch (1968), observed similar results using data from a different population. If the polymorphism and the plumage variation are determined as suggested above, then given White x Blue matings which produced at least one white gosling (i.e. the blue parent is an heterozygote), the distribution of the number of white offspring per nest should approximate a truncated binomial distribution (Hogben, 1931; Mirsky, 1972). This is a direct consequence of the postulated independence of a gosling's colour relative to the colour(s) of its siblings. A comparison of the observed and expected distributions of white offspring shows no significant differences between them (Table 2) (P>0.50). Single locus with an allelic series Traditionally, multiple allelism is distinguished from multiple loci by observing segregation rather than random assortment of the genes. Even if linkage is present, recombination between loci must occur at some low frequency. Unfortunately, such a test is rarely possible due to the difficulty of finding appropriate families. In this study, mature plumages of complete families were never obtained. Almost all observations consist of a single daughter and her Table 1 . Offspring colour ratio: Blue x Blue matings Offspring N0 Mating nc\ts 2x2 3x3 4x4 5x5 hx6 0 12 14 112 I44 ~- No. White No Blue Rdtlo* White Blue ~~ ~ 21 81 37 9 54 352 40 1 520 - 1 : 2.6 1 : 4.3 1 : 10.8 1 : 57.8 *A11 crosses, except 3 x 3, h a w ratios significantly different from 1 : 3 (PiO.01). Rank correlation ~ P<0.05 B. RATTRAY AND F. COOKE 442 Table 2. Number of nests containing four offspring in which 1, 2, 3 or all 4 are white Number of White offspring Mating Number of crosses 6x 1 Observed Expected 5x 1 Observed Expected 4x 1 Observed Expected 3x1 Observed Expected Probability of observation 2 3 4 6 5.07* 7 7.6 3 5.07 3 1.26 P>0.50 9 10.68 18 16 7 10.68 6 2.64 P>0.50 21 25.88 45 38.8 23 25.88 8 6.46 P>0.50 12 18 18 9 12 6 3 P>0.50 1 19 40 97 45 12 *Expected values were calculated on the basis of a truncated binomial as observations of zero white offspring were not made (From Hogben, 1931.); e.g. Where f=no. of offspring (family size), r=no. of recessives ( i x . whites), P,,= probability of family size ‘s’ having ‘r‘ recessives: S! parents. Despite this limitation, these data are sufficient to allow testing of this hypothesis. Consider first a three allele, single locus model. Regardless of dominance specifications, this model cannot account for a gradient of offspring colour ratios related to parental phenotype (Table 1). Although it does allow for a 3 : 1 ratio (blue to white) in the offspring of 3 x 3 matings, it requires 4 x 4,5 x 5 and 6 x 6 matings to produce blue goslings only. The observed trend could be accounted for by postulating a larger number of alleles. This, however, leads to a second prediction: with an increase in the number of alleles, a correlation between parental and offspring plumage (i.e. the extent of belly pigmentation) should be observed. For a fixed range in the variation of the phenotype, an increase in the number of codominant alleles requires the difference in the dosage provided by any two consecutive alleles to be reduced. It follows that the average change in phenotype measured by comparing homozygotes to heterozygotes must also decrease. As the effect of the heterozygote is reduced, the result should be a tighter relationship between parental plumage and that of their offspring. This may be tested by regressing the colour class of blue phase offspring on the average colour class of their parents (i.e. the midparent (Fig. 2)). No significant relationship is found (P>0.10). Threshold polygenic Given that quantitative variation is observed in plumage coloration, the probability of a future gosling being blue should increase with the magnitude of the parental deviation from the threshold. Moreover, we would expect these probabilities to be reflected in the calculated offspring colour ratios of crosses involving birds of similar plumage (e.g. 3 x 3, 4 x 4). Threshold inheritance COLOUR POLYMORPHISM I N SNOW GEESE 2. . 7 . 5 6. I I I 40 45 50 ‘2 . 443 u 55 60 Midporent colour score Figure 2 Regression of blue offspring colour class on midparent colour class. Y=3.65+0 33 X, N = 62. Not significant a t the level. predicts a positive correlation between the offspring colour ratio and the colour class of the parents. Like the single locus, incomplete dominance model, a model of threshold inheritance predicts a positive correlation between the offspring colour ratio and parental colour class. The two can be distinguished by considering only offspring colour ratios from heterozygous parents. Here, the threshold model-in contrast to the single locus model-still predicts a strong correlation between offspring colour ratio and parental colour class. The offspring colour ratios calculated for each parental colour class for a sibship of four are presented in Table 3. No significant differences exist between classes and no trend of any kind is evident. DISCUSSION Turning first to the single locus, incomplete dominance model, the results of our analyses are in complete agreement with those of Cooke & Cooch (1968). Not only is incomplete dominance sufficient to explain the observed trend in the Table 3. Offspring colour ratios by colour phase, for ‘heterozygotes’ in White (Wh) x Blue (Bl) matings Mating No. of crosses I x2 0 I x3 45 97 1 x4 1x5 1x6 40 19 Offspring colour ratio (Bl : W h ) * 1 : I .22 1 : 1.20 1 : 1.28 I : 1.17 *As heterozygote crosses producing ‘zero’ white offspring are not includrd in this analysis, the expected ratio of white t o bluc goslings is more than one ( I j . B. RATTRAY AND F. COOKE 444 offspring colour ratio (Table l ) , it also successfully predicts an invariant offspring colour ratio with respect to the colour class of mated pairs when using White x “heterozygote” matings (Table 3). The multiple allelic hypothesis does not fare as well. In general, regardless of the number of alleles or their dominance relations, a multiple allelic model produces a pattern of inheritance which conflicts with at least one of the following observations: the frequency of white goslings decreases in crosses 2 x 2 through to 6 x 6 (Table 1); no heritable relationship exists between the colour class of blue offspring and that of their parents (Fig. 2); and crosses of light bellied x light bellied, light bellied x dark bellied and dark bellied x dark bellied birds can produce both light bellied and dark bellied offspring (Table 4). this necessitates rejection of this model. Table 4. Mating combinations with the blue phase-light bellied (LB) colour classes 2-4, and dark bellied (DB), colour classes 5 and 6 ~~ ~ Offspring Cross No. of crosses LB DB LBxLB LBxDB DB x DB 4 31 26 61 1 10 3 21 22 4 Two testable predictions may be made from the threshold polygenic model: first, a relationship should be found when regressing offspring colour scores on parental values, second, a positive correlation should exist between the offspring colour ratio and the colour class of the parents. As noted earlier, the regression of offspring colour scores on parental scores is not significant (Fig. 2). This is inconsistent with a model of polygenic inheritance. This model does nevertheless, successfully account for the observed trend in offspring colour ratios in Table 1. However, if we examine a set of White x Blue matings (Table 3), this model’s prediction of a correlation between the offspring colour ratio and the Blue parental colour class fails to materialize. As a consequence, the threshold polygenic model must be considered incorrect and be rejected. We may now conclude that the single locus, incomplete dominance model is both adequate and unique in its ability to explain the plumage dimorphism and its variation in the Snow Goose. However, some mention should be made of what we have called “the dangers of failing to consider alternatives to the first acceptable hypothesis”. What could we have concluded if, in addition to the single locus model, one of the alternate hypotheses had proven to be an adequate explanation of the dimorphism? This would place us in the uncomfortable position of having too many ‘right’ answers. Most researchers invoke Occam’s Razor and select the simplest alternative (Dunbar, 1980; Ruse, 1979). Unfortunately, some vagueness exists as to the definition of ‘simplest’. Although it is frequently implied, Occam’s Razor is not intended to select the correct or most likely hypothesis. Instead, given models of equal explanatory power, it chooses whichever model is more readily tested and hence, if incorrect, most COLOUR POLYMORPHISM IN SNOW GEESE 445 quickly eliminated (Kneale, 1949; Popper, 1968). I n this manner Occam’s Razor provides the most expedient null hypothesis. Finally, our corroboration of the single locus model should be seen as especially strong support when it is recognized that the two studies described were performed on different populations. In addition, and of equal importance, we have considered and rejected the alternative hypotheses which previously had remained untested. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Both authors are grateful for comments received from members of the Queen’s University Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Group. Special thanks must go to J. C. Davies, J. Eadie and C. S. Findlay for criticisms of an earlier version of the manuscript. REFERENCES BERGSTON, S. A. & OWEN, D. F., 1973. Polymorphism in the Arctic Skua (Stercorarius parasiticus) in Iceland. Ibis, 115: 87-92. COOKE, F. & COOCH, F. G. 1968. The genetics of polymorphism in the Snow Goose (Anser caerulescens caerulescens) . Evolution, 22: 289-300. COOKE, F. & MIRSKY, P. J., 1972. A genetic analysis of Lesser Snow Goose families. Auk, 89: 863-871. DUNBAR, M. J , , 1980. The blunting of Occam’s Razor, or to hell with parsimony. Canadian Journal of < o o ~ o ~58: , 123-128. EDWARDS, J. H., 1960. The simulation of mendelism. Acta Gmetica, 10: 63-70. FINNEY, G. H., 1975. Reproductive strategies of the Lesser Snow Goose, Anser caerulescens caerulescens. Unpubl. Ph.D. thesis, Queens University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada. FOX, D. L. 1976 Animal Biochroms and Structural Colours. California: University of California Press. GRANT, P. R. & PRICE, T. D., 1981. Populational variation in continuously varying traits as an ecological genetics problem. American < o o l o , ~ ,21: 795-81 1. HOGBEN, L., 1931. The genetical analysis of familial traits. I. Single gene substitutions. Journal ofGenelics, 25: 97-1 12. KNEALE, W. E., 1949. Probability and Induction. New York: Macmillan and Co. MEDAWAR, P. B., 1969. Induction and Intuition in Scientijc Thought. Philadelphia: American Philosophical Society. MIRSKY, P. J., 1972. Colour dimorphism in the Lesser Snow Goose, Anser caerulescens caerulescens. Unpubl. M.Sc. thesis, Queens University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada. POPPER, K. R., 1972. The Logic of SCientiJic Discovery. New York: Harper and Row. RUSE, M., 1979. Falsifiability, consilience and systematics. Systematic < o o l o ~ , 29: 530-536. WILLSON, M. F. 1981 Ecology and Science. Bulletin ofthe Ecological Society of America, 62: 4-13. 21