Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Interocular alignment following visual deprivation in the cat Max Cynader Kittens were placed in the dark just after birth and then removed at various ages for the study of interocular alignment. It was found that kittens dark-reared for 4 months or longer were characteristically incyclotorted with respect to normal animals. Deprivation periods of less than 2 months were ineffective in producing these changes. Divergence of the visual axes was also observed in some dark-reared cats. Pupillary constriction in response to light was much more pronounced in dark-reared cats than in normal cats. This enhanced pupillary reaction persisted for at least 3 weeks after the deprived animals were brought into an illuminated, environment. When dark-reared cats were allowed a recovery period, in a normally lit visual environment, their ocular alignment changed markedly. The incyclotorsion and divergence of the visual axes disappeared, and instead, cats allowed recovery from deprivation could, display excyclotorsion and/or convergence of the visual axes. These anomalies of ocular alignment associated with the recovery from visual deprivation could, occur following periods of initial, deprivation as short as 30 days or as long as 2 years. The mechanisms and. possible significance of such anomalies are considered. Key words: interocular alignment, cyclotorsion, strabismus, cat, visual deprivation, recovery of vision I t has been known since Wheatstone's1 original demonstrations that stereoscopic depth perception depends on the neural integration of slightly dissimilar images on the two retinas. A necessary prerequisite for this achievement is accurate alignment of the two eyes. In strabismus, a condition in which the visual axes of the two eyes do not intersect on the object of regard, inappropriate alignment results in double vision (diplopia) or suppression of the input from one eye.2"4 A wealth of data has accumulated in both From the Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, N. S., Canada. Supported by Research Grants EY 02248 (National Institutes of Health) and MT 5201 (Medical Research Council of Canada). Submitted for publication Aug. 14, 1978. Reprint requests: Dr. Max Cynader, Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, N. S., Canada. 726 the clinical and experimental literature2 6 indicating that abnormal visual experience can lead to marked anomalies in both the development of the visual system and in development of visuomotor coordination. In particular, several investigators have reported abnormal alignment of the eyes in association with various deprivation conditions.7"10 In view of the evidence indicating a role for visual experience in the development of eye alignment, this report examines the consequences of rearing animals in darkness and of subsequent exposure in light for the development of interocular alignment. Previous studies of strabismus in kittens have concentrated on horizontal misalignment of the eyes. It is clear, however, that ocular deviations may be present in the vertical dimension (hypertropias) and that torsional anomalies, in which the eyes are misaligned in the plane about the line of sight, 0146-0404/79/070726+16$01.60/0 © 1979 Assoc. for Res. in Vis. and Ophthal., Inc. Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Volume 18 Number 7 Interocular alignment after visual deprivation may also have severe consequences for the organism's ability to construct a stereoscopic representation of three-dimensional space. This report emphasizes anomalies of torsional alignment associated with visual deprivation and its aftermath. Methods Subjects. The interocular alignment of 75 cats was examined in these experiments which took place over a period of 4 years. Twenty-six normal cats served as control subjects against which data from experimental animals were compared. Twenty-nine cats were studied on emergence into the light after having been reared in darkness from before the time of natural eye opening until 4 months of age. Twelve cats were deprived of vision for 8 to 24 months before their ocular alignment was examined on emergence from the dark, and eight kittens were light-deprived for short periods of time, starting at or near birth and continuing until there were 30 to 60 days of age. Some of the animals which had been reared in the dark were further studied after varying durations of exposure in a normal visual environment. Over 1200 photographs were examined to provide the data reported in this paper. Data from some of these animals have been presented elsewhere. 11 Measurement of ocular alignment. To assess interocular alignment in awake animals, cats were held upright and the eyes brightly illuminated with a small light source located 1 meter from the cats eyes behind the shoulder of the experimenter. We made strenuous efforts to keep the cat in an erect position and to photograph the animal while it looked straignt ahead at the camera, whose film plane was 0.5 meter from the animal. These photographs provided information about torsional and verge nee alignment of the eyes. By torsional alignment, we refer to the correspondence between the eyes in the frontal plane (i.e., about the line of sight). This was measured by simply extending the lines formed by the streak pupils of the two eyes and measuring the angle at which they intersected. In normal cats (Figs. 1 and 2) this angle averages 12°. Cats were called incyclotorted if this angle of intersection was greater than that of normal cats and exajclotorted if angles of intersection were less than those of normal cats, or negative in some instances. Since mediolateral and/or torsional alignment of the eyes may vary with the elevation of gaze, photographs in which the subject's gaze was not directed approximately straight ahead were excluded from analysis. 727 Although torsional alignment may be measured with ease and precision in alert cats, assessment of the relative horizontal positions of the eyes in alert cats poses several problems. Efforts to develop a procedure equivalent to the cover test used in the assessment of strabismus in human subjects2' ' have proved unreliable, since cats will not fixate stimuli as readily as human subjects. Rough estimates of the vergence alignment of the eyes are possible with the corneal reflex technique. 2 ' 4i 7i !> When the eyes are illuminated by a small light source, spots of light (the reflection of the light source) are visible on the anterior corneal surfaces of the eyes. In the normal cat in the center of Fig. 1, the reflex is not perfectly centered in the pupils but falls medial to the constricted pupils. In normal human subjects, studied with the same methods, the reflex would appear to be well centered in both pupils. A miscentered reflex would imply either divergence or convergence, depending on whether it fell medial or lateral to the centers of the pupils. These observations in man imply that the normal cat of Fig. 1, in which the reflex is miscentered, exhibits a divergent strabismus. It has, however, been shown9- 12- |:! that in the cat the optical axis of the eye (i.e., the axis of optical symmetry) is not aligned with visual axis, the line between the posterior nodal point of the eye and the area centralis of the eye. The angle between the optical and visual axes is such that normal cats appear slightly diverged.13> !) This angle "alpha" varies from cat to cat, 13 and this variability inevitably limits the accuracy with which horizontal interocular alignment can be inferred from corneal reflex measurements. A second difficulty with measures of interocular alignment in alert animals is that the subject is free to binocularly fixate stimuli at different distances. Hence uncontrolled vergence movements can add variability to the assessment of interocular alignment. Since the cat has a moderate range of disjunctive eye movement, 14 this is a potential contaminant for any measure of interocular alignment in alert animals. To fully remove this contaminant would require extensive behavioral training of each cat. In the assessment of eye position, efforts were made to keep the cat attentive to the experimenter or to the light source, both of which were a known distance from the animal. Repeated measures of ocular alignment in the same cat on the same day suggest that uncontrolled vergence cannot play a major role in the in the results. The technical problems described above, however, make the corneal reflex technique a relatively crude one, with probable accuracy of no more than Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 728 Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. July 1979 Cynader Fig. 1. Interocular alignment of a normal 4-month-old kitten (center) compared with that of two dark-reared kittens (left-hand side and right-hand side). The reflections of the light source on the corneal surfaces fall medial to the centers of the streak pupils in both normal and dark-reared kittens. In both dark-reared kittens, but especially the subject illustrated on the left, the tops of the streak pupils of both eyes are rotated inward relative to the normal kitten in the center. 5°. Hence small ocular deviations would not be observed with this measure. Due to these difficulties with the corneal reflex measure, quantitative assessment of horizontal eye position was made only in paralyzed cats, where much more accurate assessments of ocular alignment could be made. Measurement of ocular alignment in paralyzed! anesthetized cats. Animals were anesthesized with intravenous Pentothal and prepared for single-unit recording by methods which have been described elsewhere." 1 >h After initial paralysis with Flaxedil (10 mg/kg), a continuous infusion of Flaxedil (10 mg/kg/hr) and 5% lactated dextrose in Ringer's was begun and maintained throughout the recording session. Neo-Synephrine (1 drop of 10% solution) was instilled into each eye, and contact lenses were inserted to protect the corneas. In most cases, the animal was held in a specially modified stereotaxic instrument so that eye bars, which could distort the measurement of ocular alignment, were unnecessary. Eye bars were, however, employed in some of the earlier experiments. After initial paralysis, lA hr was allowed before the cat was photographed with the same methods as described for "alert" cats. From measures like this, it has become clear that cats become incyclotorted (about 4° to 6° in each eye) under paralysis13 and that some divergence or convergence of the visual axes may also occur. On the whole, however, we have found that the horizontal ocular alignment as assessed in the paralyzed state is representative of that of the alert animal. Similar conclusions have been reached in a quantitative study of this problem. 16 The slit pupils having been photographed, ophthalmic atropine was instilled to paralyze accommodation and dilate the pupils. Retinoscopy was performed, and contact lenses with 4 mm diameter artificial pupils were selected to ensure that images were in good focus on a tangent screen 1.5 meters distant. At least 1 hr was then allowed for the eyes to fully stabilize before retinal landmarks were plotted with a reversing ophthalmoscope. The procedure involved plotting the locations of the areae centrales of each eye on the tangent screen as well as the location of the optic discs. One possible contaminant of this method for assessing interocular alignment is the well-known uncertainty in plotting the area centralis in the cat.12' 13 Where possible, the locations of the ophthalmoscopically plotted areae centrales were validated by recording responses from several binocularly driven single cells in the central representation of the striate cortex. In cats with a normal complement of binocularly driven neurons, the receptive field location through each eye could be determined. The separation between the two receptive fields corresponded well with the plotted separation between the areae centrales of the two eyes. Results Interocular alignment in dark-reared cats. Fig. 1 illustrates the interocular alignment of two dark-reared cats in comparison with that of a normally reared animal. At least two features distinguished the interocular alignment of these two groups of subjects. In both Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Volume 18 Number 1 Interocular alignment after visual deprivation 729 INTEROCULAR TORSION DARK-REARED CATS INTORTED • • NORMAL Normal Cats 4 Months Dark-reared 8-10 Months Dark-reared 40° 38 36 34 32 30 28 26 24 INTEROCULAR 22 20 TORSION 18 16 IN DEGREES 14 12 10 8 6 4 2° Fig. 2. Distribution of interocular torsion for normal cats in comparison with that of kittens deprived of vision for 4 months or 8 to 10 months, starting just after birth. Each square represents one kitten, although the data for one kitten in this and subsequent figures may represent the mean of up to five photographs. Interocular torsion is defined as the angle of intersection formed by extending the lines of the streak pupils. The mean of the distribution for normal cats is 11.2° (S.D. 3.0°) and that for the 4-month dark-reared group is 22.3° (S.D. 6.0°). The mean interocular torsion angle for cats dark-reared for 8 to 10 months is 27° (S.D. 4.7°). MEDIOLATERAL OCULAR ALIGNMENT Nor tial 6 - Cats Dar (-Reared Cats ;;" y 4 - • • 2 - Hi 7 6 5 — CONVERGED 4 1 0 1 SUPERIMPOSED 2 6 7 m ^^ H 8 9 10 list 15° DIVERGED Fig. 3. Comparison of the horizontal positions of the areae centrales in normal cats under paralysis with those of dark-reared cats studied under the same conditions. To derive these data, the cats were paralyzed and then the locations of the areae centrales of the two eyes were plotted on a tangent screen 57 inches away with a reversing opthalmoscope. In many cases, these measurements were further checked by determining the location of the two receptive fields of binocularly driven units. Each square represents one cat and the abscissa is in degrees. In normal cats, the areae centrales are more frequently crossed on paralysis than uncrossed (mean = 0.8° crossed; S.D. = 2.9°). The dark-reared group displays more variability than the normal population (S.D. = 5.3°) and the areae centrales are, on average, diverged relative to the normal group (mean = 4.6° uncrossed). Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 730 Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. July 1979 Cynader Fig. 4. Interocular alignment of three cats which have been kept in darkness for prolonged periods starting just after birth and then allowed a recovery period in the light after the deprivation period. The cat on the left-hand side was maintained in darkness for 24 months and then allowed a 4-month recovery period before this photograph was taken. A marked esotropia is indicated by the lateral location of the light reflex relative to the center of the streak pupils. This cat has been kept in the light for 2 years since this photograph was taken without obvious alteration in interocular alignment. The center cat is the same animal as illustrated on the right-hand side of Fig. 1, but with 2 months of exposure in a normal environment. Comparison of interocular alignment before and after the recovery period reveals a change from incyclotorsion to excyclotorsion and from normal vergence alignment to esotropia. The cat on the right represents an extreme example of the excyclotorsion associated with recovery from visual deprivation. The medial location of the light reflex relative to the streak pupils indicates, however, that vergence alignment is approximately normal in this cat. dark-reared cats, the streak pupils were incyclotorted relative to those of the normal subject. The cat on the left of Fig. 1 represents an extreme case of this torsional anomaly. In this case, the angle between the pupils was 34°, compared with that of 12° for the normal cat in the center of Fig. 1. Incyclotorsion was less marked (21°) in the cat pictured on the right but was still greater than that of the normal cat in the center. Only a few dark-reared cats overlapped with the normal population in their torsional alignment. Fig. 2 illustrates the distribution of interocular torsion in the populations of dark-reared and normal cats which have been examined. The range of interocular torsional angles was much greater in dark-reared cats than in the normal cats. As well, a marked trend toward incyclotorsion was clearly visible. This trend appeared especially marked among the animals which had been deprived for longer periods (8 to 10 months). The additional incyclotorsion in kittens deprived for 4 months averaged 11° relative to normal subjects, whereas the animals deprived for 8 to 10 months were, on average, 16° incyclotorted relative to the normal group. The trend toward incyclotorsion of the optic axes was the most consistent anomaly observed in the dark-reared group but not the only one. Several authors have commented on the tendency toward divergence among dark-reared cats,7" IOr n and the cat illustrated on the left of Fig. 1 exemplifies this trend. Examination of the positions of the corneal reflex in the two eyes of the normal cat revealed that it was somewhat medial to the streak pupils in the both eyes. In normal cats the reflex is typically miscentered by 1.25 to 1.75 mm on the corneal surfaces.7 By contrast, the reflex is markedly miscentered in both eyes of the left-hand side dark-reared subject, implying a divergence of the optic axes. Cats with obvious divergence did not, however, dominate the population of darkreared subjects. In the dark-reared cat on the right-hand side of Fig. 1, the reflex appeared to fall just medial to the pupil in each eye. This value falls within the range of ocular alignment in normal cats. On paralysis (see Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Volume 18 Number 7 Interocular alignment after visual deprivation 731 INTEROCULAR TORSION DARK-RECOVERY -INTORTED CATS NORMAL EXTORTED |::: 11 :| NORMAL CATS L DARK-REARED ^ H DARK - RECOVERY 6 I 1 CO *. -L - - r-. 1 ro NUMBER OF 5 5 II 1 [ I I I I 1 40° 38 36 34 32 30 28 26 24 22 20 18 16 I : 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 2 4° INTEROCULAR TORSION IN DEGREES Fig. 5. Comparison of interocular torsional alignment among normal cats, dark-reared cats, and dark-recovery cats. Conventions are as in Fig. 2. The mean of the distribution of interocular torsion angles is 9.8° for the dark-recovery group, which is similar to that of normal cats (11.2°). The standard deviation is however much greater for the dark-recovery group (S.D. = 10.3°) than for normal cats (S.D. = 3.0°). Methods) of the subject shown on the right of Fig. 1, the relative positions of the areae centrales in two eyes indicated a convergence of 3°. This value is well within the range observed in normal cats under the same conditions (Fig. 3). It is evident, especially in the cat on the left of Fig. 1, that conclusions about divergence or convergence of the optic axes will be markedly influenced by the elevation of the corneal reflex in the two eyes. If the reflex is positioned on the inferior corneal surface, all dark-reared cats would appear markedly divergent. By contrast, positioning the reflex on the superior surface would result in apparent convergence of the optic axes in many cases. Inattention to the elevation of the reflex when the cat is photographed may account for some of the discrepant results which have been reported in the literature.7' I0- n As discussed in Methods, the many difficulties associated with the corneal reflex measure render it unsuitable for quantitative evaluation of the relative horizontal position of the eyes. Much more accurate assessments can be made by paralyzing the subject and determining the projections of the areae centrales either by the use of a reversing ophthalmoscope or by plotting the receptive fields of centrally located, binocularly driven single units. Both of these methods were employed and resulted in data which were in good agreement. Fig. 3 summarizes the data obtained with these measures for a population of normal cats as compared -with visually deprived subjects. It is evident that the range of interocular misalignment was much greater among the group of dark-reared cats than among normal animals. Dark-reared cats as a group also appeared somewhat divergent with regard to the normally reared population. It is clear, however, that exceptions to this rule were not uncommon and that the trend toward divergence of the visual axes was less consistent than was the trend toward incyclotorsion observed among dark-reared cats. Interocular alignment in dark-recovery cats. When cats are first removed from the darkroom following prolonged visual deprivation, they give the appearance of total blindness. The animals fail to react to threatening visual stimuli, they bump into objects Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 732 Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sri. July 1979 Cynader TIME COURSE DARK RECOVERY 14 CO DC O UJ 12 8 o en cc 2 2 0 1 2 3 4 WEEKS IN LIGHT 5 6 Fig. 6. Time course of torsion changes when kittens previously deprived from just after birth are brought into the light at 4 months of age. The kittens' torsional alignment at the end of deprivation was assigned a value of 0 and represents the origin of the graph. Changes of alignment toward excyclotorsion are assigned positive values. The error bars represent one standard error. as they skitter about the floor, and they fail to pursue moving visual stimili. If such animals are maintained in a normally lit environment for the next few weeks, their capacity for visually guided behavior improves markedly.18> 19 Pari passu with the improvement in visual behavior, the ocular alignment of these animals changes. Fig. 4 illustrates the interocular alignment of three cats after they have been allowed a period of recovery in a normal visual environment following prolonged dark-rearing. A comparison of Figs. 1 and 4 reveals two major differences between the ocular alignment of these cats and subjects which have just been removed from the darkroom. First, the marked incyclotorsion characteristic of the dark-reared population was no longer evident, and the cat on the right of Fig. 4 was, in fact, markedly excyclotorted. Second, divergence of the visual axes disappeared, and instead convergent strabismus was observed (left-hand side subject in Fig. 4). The middle subject in Fig. 4 is the same kitten as illustrated on the right of Fig. 1. The 8 weeks of exposure intervening between the two photographs resulted in a change of 19° in the torsional alignment of the eyes. This animal was now clearly excyclotorted with regard to the normal cat in Fig. 1. Furthermore, the medial location of the corneal reflex in the two eyes of the dark-reared kitten has been replaced by the symmetric location of the reflex in the same animal after recovery. On paralysis, shortly after this photograph was taken, the relative positions of the areae centrales in this dark-recovery subject indicated an esotropia of 14°. The esotropia observed in the dark-recovery cats was clearly visible under paralysis as well, indicating that active contraction of the eye muscles was not necessary for its manifestation. These two alterations of interocular alignment, i.e., from incyclotorsion towards excyclotorsion and from exotropia toward esotropia, constituted the major changes observed in the interocular alignment of darkrecovery cats. Fig. 5 adds torsional data derived from the study of dark-recovery cats to the comparison Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Volume 18 Interocular alignment after visual deprivation 733 Number 7 MEDIOLATERAL OCULAR ALIGNMENT j ; : : : : | NORMAL CATS [ | ^ | DARK-REARED CATS DARK RECOVERY CATS CONVERGED SUPERIMPOSED DIVERGED Fig. 7. Horizontal interocular alignment of normal cats, dark-reared cats, and cats allowed a prolonged recovery period in the light following visual deprivation. Conventions and methods are the same as those for Fig. 3. The dark-recovery cats display a clearly measurable esotropia under paralysis. The mean of the distribution for dark-recovery cats is 10° crossed (S.D. = 4.2°) compared with 0.8° crossed for normal cats and 4.6° uncrossed for dark-reared cats which have not been allowed a recovery period. between normal and dark-reared subjects made in Fig. 2. The data indicate that cats allowed a recovery period in a normal environment were consistently excyclotOrted relative to dark-reared cats. The mean interocular torsional alignment in the population of dark-recovery cats was not significantly different from that of normal cats, but the range of torsional values was much larger among the dark-recovery group. It is particularly striking that in some cases the dark-recovery cats developed a marked excyclotorsion of the visual axes. The righthand side subject in Fig. 4 represents an extreme example of excyclotorsion associated with the recovery of vision following prolonged deprivation. The time course of the alterations in interocular alignment among the dark-recovery cats has been studied in eight animals which were maintained in darkness until 4 months of age. They were then photographed at weekly intervals following their emergence from the dark. The rate of change of interocular torsion for these animals is plotted in Fig. 6. To produce this figure, interocular torsion for each animal at the time of emergence from the dark was normalized to zero. In subsequent measures, a change to- ward excyclotorsion was assigned a positive value. The graph indicates that alterations in torsional alignment began within a few days and that the change toward excyclotorsion appeared to be largely complete within 3 weeks. It should be noted that although the smooth curve of Fig. 6 represents the mean of data derived from eight animals, intersubject variability, both in the exact timing and in the degree of torsion change with visual experience, was considerable. The variability associated with repeated measures of the same kitten on the same day was also increased during this recovery period relative to normal cats. In two of the eight cats followed in this series, torsional alignment of the eyes remained essentially unchanged during the recovery period, although clear changes in vergence alignment were observed. The source of this interanimal variability is as yet unclear. Since some of the animals were subject to single-unit analysis during the same period, it is possible that the paralysis, anesthesia, and cycloplegia associated with recording may have influenced the recovery process. It has been a consistent observation, however, that the animals which have the least marked incyclotorsion when they first emerge from the dark tend to de- Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 734 Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sri. July 1979 Cynader Fig. S. Top photograph compares the size of the pupils of a normal kitten (left) with those of a visually deprived kitten (right) under the same lighting conditions. It is evident that the normal kitten's pupils are markedly more dilated than those of the dark-reared kitten. Lower photograph compares the area of the pupils in two littermate kittens. The kitten on the right was dark-reared until 3 weeks before this photograph was taken; the kitten on the left was treated normally from birth. Under the same illumination conditions, the pupils of the previously deprived kitten are markedly constricted relative to those of the normal cat. The level of illumination under which these photographs were taken was 1.3 log ft. lamberts. velop the most marked excyclotorsion of the visual axes following light exposure. By contrast, kittens with marked incyclotorsion to begin with tend to remain strongly incyclotorted despite light exposure, showing less torsion change than other dark-reared kittens. Vergence changes in interocular alignment. At the same time as interocular torsion changes during recovery from visual deprivation, both eyes turn medialward, resulting in convergent strabismus. Fig. 7 compares horizontal ocular alignment among normal cats, dark-reared cats, and cats which have been allowed a recovery period in a normal visual environment following prolonged deprivation. The data indicate that the dark-recovery group ot cats was markedly convergent, relative both to dark-reared cats immediately after deprivation and also to normal cats. As with the torsional measures reported in such cats (Fig. 5), the range of interocular vergence values among the darkrecovery population was much larger than that of normal cats. The time course of the alterations in mediolateral interocular alignment on exposure to light was similar to that previously described for torsion changes, and in some animals, these alterations in eye position in the horizontal and torsional planes occurred together, suggesting a common basis. In other subjects, however, including the two described above, the recovery process consisted mainly of alterations in vergence alignment, with little or no torsional change. In the subject on the left of Fig. 4, a marked esotropia can be observed in the presence of normal torsional alignment. In other cases, typified by the cat shown on the right of Fig. 4, the recovery process consisted of marked torsional changes accompanied by only a small vergence change. Pupillary reactions in dark-reared cats. If a previously dark-reared cat is brought into a moderately well-lit room and allowed a few minutes to settle down, it can be observed that the pupils are much more constricted than those of normal kittens under the same conditions. Fig. 8 (top) compares pupillary reactivity of a normal kitten with that of a dark-reared kitten of similar age. It is immediately evident that the pupils in the dark-reared kitten on the right are only one fifth as large in area as those of the normal kitten under the same lighting conditions. This increased pupillary constriction under conditions of moderate illumination (1.3 log foot-lamberts) has been observed in virtually all dark-reared animals which have been examined. The enhanced pupillary reactions of dark- Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Volume 18 Number 1 Interocular alignment after visual deprivation 735 INTEROCULAR TORSION SHORT-TERM DEPRIVATION Normal NORMAL INTORTED Cats 30-60 Days Dark Reared 28° 26 24 22 20 18 INTEROCULAR 16 14 TORSION 12 IN 10 8 6 4 2° DEGREES Fig. 9. Distribution of interocular torsion for kittens dark-reared for 30 to 60 days in comparison with normal kittens. Conventions as in Fig. 2. The mean interocular torsion for the short-term dark-reared cats is 13.5° (S.D. = 4.8°). This is not significantly different from that of the normal group (mean = 11.2°; S.D. = 3.0°). reared cats described above diminished with time but could be readily observed for as long as 2 to 3 weeks after the animal was brought into the light. The lower part of Fig. 8 illustrates the relative degree of pupillary constriction in a normally reared kitten and its littermate which had spent 3 weeks in the light following 4 months of dark-rearing. It is evident that pupillary area was reduced by a factor of 3 in the previously deprived kitten relative to the normally reared kitten, even after the 3-week recovery period. Our methods are not adequate to determine whether the hyperreactive pupillary reflex which characterizes dark-reared cats eventually subsides to the level of normal or whether a small increase in excitability persists indefinitely despite continued exposure in the normal environment. Age-dependent process in interocular alignment. Several aspects of interocular alignment observed in these experiments appeared to depend on the animal's age. Fig. 9 compares the torsional alignment of kittens kept in darkness from just after birth until 60 days (or less) with that of normal cats. The data indicate that subjecting kittens to a short deprivation period did not result in the characteristic torsional anomalies observed with longer term deprivation. A comparison of the data presented in Fig. 9 with those of Fig. 2 suggests that these anomalies developed between 2 and 4 months of age. The difference between the 4 month and 8 to 10 month deprivation conditions in Fig. 2 indicates that incyclotorsion of the visual axes may increase progressively with prolonged deprivation. Although the torsional anomaly which characterizes dark-reared cats appeals to exhibit a well-defined critical period, the alterations in interocular alignment which occur when dark-reared subjects are brought into the light are, to some extent, independent of the animal's age. One animal maintained in the dark for 2 years developed a marked esotropia on subsequent exposure in a normal visual environment (Fig. 4). Similarly, esotropia and excyclotorsion were noted in two of three kittens which were visually deprived from just after birth until only 30 days of age. The main difference between the effects of short vs. long periods of deprivation starting at birth appears to be the speed with which the alignment of the eyes changes when the animal is brought into the light. The time Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Set. July 1979 736 Cynader A A 5°; .DR c- -D CR Fig. 10. Consequence of rotating each eye inward through 5° about the line of sight. The lines AB and CD represent the projections of the vertical and horizontal meridia of both eyes onto a screen under conditions of appropriate torsional alignment. With incyclotorsion of each eye through 5°, ALBL and ARBR represent the vertical meridia of the left and right eyes, respectively. C,DL and C R D R represent the horizontal meridia. The horizontal separation of the vertical meridia of the two eyes increases with distance from the areae centrales. In the upper visual fields, the left and right vertical nieridia are crossed, indicating convergence, whereas they are uncrossed in the lower visual fields, indicating divergence. Similarly the vertical separation of points along the horizontal meridia of the two eyes increases with increasing eccentricity. course for alignment changes described in Fig. 6 for kittens kept in the dark for 4 months, which required several weeks, is compressed into 3 to 4 days if kittens are deprived for just 30 days. Discussion Torsional and horizontal alignment of the eyes. Two major features distinguish the ocular alignment of dark-reared cats from that of normal subjects: incyclotorsion of the visual axes and a tendency toward divergence. We have treated horizontal and torsional alignment of the eyes separately in our descriptions of dark-reared cats. This separation is based on several considerations. First, the characteristics of the adequate visual stimuli for the elicitation of vergence or cyclotorsional eye movements are quite different. The adequate stimulus for eliciting vergence eye movements has been shown to be a stimulus movement and/or offset perpendicular to the fixation plane.20' 2I This stimulus would demand alterations in lateral alignment of the eyes without necessitating torsional changes. By contrast, the adequate stimulus for the system producing opposed cyclotorsional eye movements would be a rotation about the fixation plane (see below). Such a stimulus would demand no net vergence change, since the upper and lower field signals for vergence would counteract each other. A second reason for treating these aspects of interocular alignment separately is the observation that torsional or vergence alignment of the eyes may be altered independently during recovery from visual depriva- Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Volume 18 Number 7 Interocular alignment after visual deprivation 737 Fig. 11. The relationship between incyclotorsion and the plane over which binocular fusion is possible. If the cats eyes are aligned such that a vertical screen (solid line) is in binocular correspondence at a fixation distance of 50 cm., incyclotorsion of each eye by 2° would require that the screen be tilted toward the subject by 49° in order for binocular correspondence to be maintained. If each eye were incyclotorted by 5°, a 71° tilt of the plane (top toward the subject) would be required to negate the horizontal and vertical disparities shown in Fig. 10. tion (Fig. 4). In the various experimental conditions reported here and in the companion paper, 22 incyclotorsion has been found in association with either esotropia or exotropia and in kittens with normal vergence alignment. Torsional anomalies. At first glance, the significance of the torsional anomalies observed in the dark-reared animals in this study appears unclear. However, several geometric consequences for fused binocular vision follow immediately from alterations in interocular torsional alignment. With incyclotorsion, the degree of horizontal ocular misalignment will vary with the elevation of the visual stimulus. The deprived cats of Fig. 1 would be markedly more divergent for stimuli presented in the lower visual fields than for stimuli presented in the upper fields. A vergence movement would alter the relative horizontal position of the eyes and would also change the elevation of the retinal areas in which fused binocular overlap could occur if the cat faces a vertical screen. The situation is illustrated in Fig. 10, for a cat facing a tangent screen. If the torsional alignment of the eyes is appropriate, as in a normal cat, the vertical and horizontal meridia of the two eyes will line up (solid lines) so that an object falling anywhere along them will fall on corresponding retinal points. The dotted lines illustrate the consequences of rotating each eye inward through 5° about the area centralis. This is about the size of the torsional anomaly observed in the deprived kittens. It can be seen that horizontal disparities between the two eyes arise for stimuli presented along the vertical meridia and that the sign of these disparities is opposite in the upper and lower fields. Convergence of the visual axes would allow stimuli presented along the vertical meridian of the lower fields Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Set. July 1979 738 Cynader to fall on corresponding retinal points, the degree of convergence determining the retinal elevation at which binocular overlap occurred. Similarly, divergence would permit binocular overlap in the upper fields. Horizontal vergence movements would, however, be unable to compensate for the vertical disparities arising for stimuli presented along the horizontal meridian, or for the vertical component of the disparities arising for stimuli which fall on neither the horizontal nor the vertical meridian. These geometric considerations make it evident that inappropriate torsional alignment of the eyes presents an impediment to binocular fusion which may be as severe as that associated with inappropriate vergence alignment. In order to re-establish binocular correspondence in the face of the 5° incyclotorsion of each eye which characterizes the darkreared kitten, the top of the screen which the animal faces would have to be rotated toward him (about its center) through an angle which depended on the fixation distance. This relationship between interocular torsion and the inclination of the plane of regard was first treated by Helmholtz.23 The equation tan y = tan 6 (1) where y is the monocular torsion angle in degrees, 21 is the interpullary distance, F is the fixation distance, and d is the angle of inclination of the plane which is viewed, is derived from Helmholtz and is illustrated in Fig. 11. For a given fixation distance (50 cm in Fig. 11) and assuming a 3 cm interpupillary distance for the cat, incyclotorsion of each eye through 5° would require a 71° rotation of the plane being viewed toward the animal in order to retain binocular correspondence. Even 2° of incyclotorsion of each eye would require a 49° tilt of the plane being viewed in order for objects on that plane to continue to fall on corresponding retinal points. One would naturally assume that the plane which is in correspondence for subjects with normal torsional alignment is upright. In fact, however, recent evidence indicates that the plane which is in binocular correspondence (the vertical horopter) is tilted away from subjects with normal torsional alignment.24' 25 These data, which appear similar for cats, owls, and humans, indicate that the degree of tilt of the vertical horopter depends on the fixation distance. At the fixation distance described above, the horopter would be tilted away from the normal subject by about 70°. It has been suggested that the inclination of the vertical horopter away from the organism may serve the function of keeping the terrain which is being traversed in binocular correspondence as the subject locomotes. The consequence of incyclotorsion, as in the case of the dark-reared cat, would be to tilt the vertical horopter, so that it was approximately upright, rather than tilted away from him, as in the normal cat. In this sense, the alterations of torsional alignment observed in the dark-reared cats may represent a "best guess" of the inclination of the plane of binocular correspondence made in the absence of visual input. When the dark-reared cat is brought into a normally illuminated environment and allowed a recovery period, the incyclotorsion which characterizes the recently deprived animal disappears. During the change from incyclotorsion toward excyclotorsion of the visual axes, the inclination of the plane over which binocular correspondence can be maintained alters as well. The vertical horopter tilts away from the animal as incyclotorsion lessens, and in the kittens which become excyclotorted relative to normal cats, the vertical horopter may come to lie almost horizontal. It is tempting to regard the torsion changes of the dark-recovery kittens as an adaptive modification, i.e., to surmise that the kitten is attempting to compensate for the presumed diplopia induced by the different orientations of visual stimuli on the retinas. If these torsional alterations represent efforts to reduce diplopia, however, they must be singularly unsuccessful. Few of the dark-recovery animals eventually achieve normal Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Volume 18 Number 7 Interocular alignment after visual deprivation torsional alignment. Individual subjects may pass through a period of normal alignment during the recovery process only to continue past this point toward excyclotorsion of the visual axes. The significance of this overshoot is still unclear. The results from the darkrecovery cats may be placed in context, however, by noting that the time course of the alterations in torsional alignment during recovery from visual deprivation correlates well with the time course of the recovery of feature specificity by neurons in the visual cortex of kittens which have been dark-reared until 4 months of age and then allowed a recovery period in a normal visual environment.2(i> 27 Vergence anomalies. A trend toward increased divergence in dark-reared cats has been noted by some authors, 7 ' l7 although others have not consistently observed this phenomenon in such animals.10> I6 It should be noted that the corneal reflex measures used in most of these studies are relatively inaccurate. Moreover, the systematic torsional misalignment of the visual axes in dark-reared cats must complicate corneal reflex measures of esotropia since the distance between the center of the pupil and the reflex would vary with the elevation of the light source and the cat's angle of regard in the vertical plane. For these reasons, our quantitative date for vergence alignment in dark-reared cats have been gathered by determining the positions of the areae centrales in paralyzed and anesthetized cats. These data agree with those of other workers in indicating that dark-reared cats are on average divergent with regard to normal cats but show that there is considerable overlap between the two populations in this regard. Though the development of esotropia following exposure to light in previously darkreared kittens has been a consistent observation (Figs. 4 and 5), the mechanism underlying this phenomenon remains uncertain. Our working hypothesis is, however, that esotropia is associated with the pupillary anomalies exhibited by dark-reared animals. It has been established for man, cats, and monkeys 739 that the occurrence of pupillary constriction is often associated with convergence of the visual axes and with accommodation. These three reactions occur together when an object approaches the observer, and so have been referred to collectively as the "near reflex."28 This triadic response is the basis for the well-known observation that stimuli demanding accommodation elicit convergence and pupillary constriction as well as the appropriate accommodative response. The linkage between these accommodative and vergence reactions during normal usage provided the basis for Donders' 29 explanation of the relationship between hypermetropia and esotropia. It was known, even in his time, that hypermetropic subjects often developed convergent strabismus. Donders' view was that the constant accommodative effort required for clear vision in hypermetropia would result in tonically increased convergence as well. This tonically increased convergence would lead ultimately to esotropia. It may be that a similar mechanism leads to esotropia in the dark-recovery kittens. According to this view, constant demands to constrict the pupil (due to the kittens' increased reactivity to light) would demand tonically increased convergence. This would result eventually in convergent strabismus. This hypothesis is a tentative one and requires many further experiments before it can be accepted. It does, however, enable us to account for the divergence which occurs in dark-reared cats in the same terms as the convergence associated with recovery from deprivation. According to this hypothesis, the initial exotropia results from an absence of stimulation to the pupillary system while the animal is in the dark, whereas the esotropia which develops when the animal is brought into the light is a result of overstimulation of this system. The esotropia which characterizes the darkrecovery cats can be observed both in alert animals (Fig. 4) and in the paralyzed and anesthetized subjects (Fig. 7). Since paralysis removes any active contribution of the muscles to eye position, this finding implies that Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sri. July 1979 740 Cynader the anatomical position of rest of the eyes can be altered by the unusual exposure history. The resting length of the relaxed muscles may thus be subject to modification under these circumstances. Time-dependent processes in the development of ocular misalignment. In recent years much attention has been directed toward the concept of the critical period, with reference to the idea that the visual system is especially sensitive to environmental manipulation at certain stages in its development. Several aspects of interocular alignment appear to depend on the age of the subject. The incyclotorsion characteristic of dark-reared cats has not been observed consistently in kittens less than 60 days of age (Fig. 9). On the other hand, kittens reared in the dark for 8 to 10 months seem, as a group, more incyclotorted than kittens maintained in darkness for 4 months. These data suggest that the torsional alignment of visually deprived kittens is initially normal and remains so until 60 to 120 days of age. During this period the incyclotorsion appears and is then maintained (and may increase progressively) as deprivation is prolonged. Similar conclusions have been reached by Olson and Freeman16 in a longitudinal study of the development of ocular alignment in dark-reared kittens. The characteristic response of the deprived organism to light exposure, namely, increased convergence and excyclotorsion of the visual axes, can be observed following deprivation periods as short as 30 days or as long as 2 years. These data indicate that there can be no tight "critical period" in early development during which interocular misalignment, including esotropia and excyclotorsion, can be induced. At first glance this would appear inconsistent with observations in the clinical literature which indicate that certain types of strabismus develop only at particular ages.2"4' 30 There is evidence, however, that one effect of maintaining animals in darkness is to allow the visual system to retain its capacity for modifiability beyond the duration of the naturally occurring critical period.11' 27 The data showing that eye alignment is modifiable even in adult cats after 2 years of visual deprivation may be understood in the context of the persisting immaturity retained by the visual system so long as deprivation is continued. Despite the fact that esotropia and excyclotorsion may develop in dark-reared cats of any age after exposure to light, the speed with which ocular alignment changes depends on the age of the animals. Following 30 to 60 days of darkrearing, marked alterations in ocular alignment may occur within a few days after the animal is exposed to a normally lit environment, whereas longer term deprived cats manifest similar changes only over weeks or months. I thank Dr. K. I. Beverly for valuable discussion and Colleen Clattenburg for assistance in the preparation of this report. REFERENCES 1. Wheatstone, C : Contributions to the physiology of vision. Part the first: On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phenomena of binocular vision, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 11:371, 1838. 2. Duke-Elder, S., and Wybar, K.: Ocular motility and strabismus. In System of Ophthalmology, St. Louis, 1973, The C. V. Mosby Co., vol. 6. 3. Alpern, M.: Movements of the eyes. In Davson, H., editor: The Eye, New York, 1969, Academic Press, Inc., vol. 3. 4. Burian, H.M., and Von Noorden, G.K.: Binocular Vision and Ocular Motility: Theory and Management of Strabismus, St. Louis, 1974, The C. V. Mosby Co. 5. Hubel, D.H., and Wiesel, T.N.: The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens, J. Physiol. 206:419, 1970. 6. Barlow, H.B.: Visual experience and cortical development, Nature 258:199, 1975. 7. Sherman, S.M.: Development of interocular alignment in cats, Brain Res 37:187, 1972. 8. Blake, R., Crawford, M.L.J., and Hirsch, H.V.B.: Consequences of alternating monocular deprivation on eye alignment and convergence in cats, INVEST. OPHTHALMOL. 13:121, 1974. 9. Olson, C , and Freeman, R.: Development of eye alignment in cats, Nature 271:446, 1978. 10. Kalil, R.: Dark rearing in the cat: Effects on visuomotor behaviour and cell growth in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus, J. Comp. Neurol. 178:451, 1978. 11. Cynader, M., Berman, N., and Hein, A.: Recovery of function in cat visual cortex following prolonged deprivation, Exp. Brain Res. 25:139, 1976. 12. Vakkur, J.G., and Bishop, P.O.: The schematic eye in the cat, Vision Res. 3:357, 1963. Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017 Volume 18 Number 7 Interocular alignment after visual deprivation 741 13. Bishop, P.O., Kozak, W., and Vakkur, J.G.: Some quantitative aspects of the cat's eye: axes and plane ol relerence, visual field coordinates and optics, J. Physiol. 163:466, 1963. 14. Stryker, M.P., and Blakemore, C.: Saccadic and disjunctive eye movement in cats, Vision Res. 12:2005, 1972. 15. Cynader, M., and Berman, N.: Receptive-field organization of monkey superior colliculus, J. Neurophysiol. 35:187, 1972. 16. Olson, C , and Freeman, R. D.: Eye alignment in kittens, J. Neurophysiol. 41:848, 1978. 17. Pettigrew, J. D.: The effect of visual experience on the development of stimulus specificity by kitten cortical neurones, J. Physiol. 237:49, 1974. 18. Van Hof-Van Duin, J.: Development of visuomotor behaviour in normal and dark-reared cats, Brain Res. 104:233, 1976. 19. Timney, B., Mitchell, D.E., and Giffin, F.: The development of vision in cats after extended periods of dark-rearing, Exp. Brain Res. 31:547, 1978. 20. Rashbass, C., and Westheimer, G.: Disjunctive eye movements, J. Physiol. 159:149, 1961. 21. Westheimer, G., and Mitchell, D.E.: The sensory stimulus for disjunctive eye movements, Vision Res. 9:749, 1969. 22. Cynader, M.: Role of visual cortex in interocular alignment, INVEST. OPHTHALMOL. VISUAL SCI. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 18: 742, 1979. v. Helmholtz, H.: In Southall, P., editor: Treatise on Physiological Optics, New York, 1962, Dover Publications, vol. Ill, p. 349. Nakayama, K.: Geometry of binocular vision. Paper presented at third annual Interdisciplinary Conference on Vision, Jackson Hole, Wyo., Jan. 1978. Cooper, M., and Pettigrew, J.D.: A Neurophysiological determination of the vertical horopter in the cat and owl, J. Comp. Neurol. 184:1, 1979. Mustari, M., and Cynader, M.: Rapid recovery from the effects of visual deprivation in cat parastriate cortex. Neuroscience 3:570, 1977 (abst). Cynader, M.: Extension of the critical period in cat visual cortex. Presented at Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, April, 1977. Jampel, R.S.: Representation of the near response on the cerebral cortex of the macacque, Am. J. Ophthalmol. 48:573, 1959. Donders, F.C.: On the Anomalies of Accommodation and Refraction of the Eye, London, 1864, The New Syndenham Society. Taylor, D. M.: Is congenital esotropia functionally curable? Trans. Am. Ophthalmol. Soc. 70:529, 1972. Downloaded From: http://iovs.arvojournals.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/iovs/933085/ on 06/12/2017