Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Health Psychology According to the basic tenets of attribution theory people attempt to provide a causal explanation for events in their world particularly if those events are unexpected and have personal relevance (Heider, 1958). Thus it is not surprising that people will generally seek a causal explanation for an illness, particularly one that is serious. Taylor et al. (1984) interviewed a sample of women who had been treated for breast cancer. They found that 95% of the women had a causal explanation for their cancer. These causes were classified as stress (41%), specific carcinogen (32%), heredity (26%), diet (17%), blow to breast (10%) and other (28%). Women’s causal explanations for breast cancer They also asked the women who or what they considered responsible for the disease and found that 41% of the women blamed themselves, 10% blamed another person, 28% blamed the environment and 49% blamed chance. The patients were also asked whether they felt any control over their cancer and they found 56% felt they had some control. The women’s attribution of responsibility for their cancer Weiner et al. (1972) suggested that we can classify attributional dimensions along three dimensions: 1 Locus: the extent to which the cause is localized inside or outside the person. 2 Controllability: the extent to which the person has control over the cause. 3 Stability: the extent to which the cause is stable or changeable. Health locus of control, like attribution theory, also emphasises attributions for causality and control. Wallston and Wallston (1982) developed a measure of the health locus of control, which evaluates whether individuals regard their health as controllable by them or not controllable by them or they believe their health is under the control of powerful others. Health locus of control is related to whether individuals changed their behaviour and to the kind of communications style they require from health professionals. There are several problems with the concept of a health locus of control: Is health locus of control a fixed traits or a transient state? Is it possible to be both external and internal? Going to the doctor could be seen as external (the doctor is a powerful other) or internal (I am looking after my health). Unrealistic optimism focuses on perceptions of susceptibility and risk. Weinstein (1984) suggested that one of the reasons why people continued to practice unhealthy behaviours is due to inaccurate perceptions of risk and susceptibility - their unrealistic optimism. He asked subjects to examine a list of health problems and displayed what "compared to other people of your age and sex, are your chances of getting the problem greater than, about the same, or less than theirs?" Most subjects believed they were less likely to get the health problem. Weinstein (1987) described four cognitive factors that contribute to unrealistic optimism: 1. Lack of personal experience with the problem 2. The belief that the problem is preventable by individual action 3. The belief that if the problem has not yet appeared, it will not appear in the future 4. The belief that the problem is infrequent. The transtheoretical model of change emphasises the dynamic nature of beliefs, time, and costs and benefits. 1. Precontemplation: not intending to make any changes 2. Contemplation: considering a change 3. Preparation: making small changes 4. Action: actively engaging in a new behaviour 5. Maintenance: sustaining change over time Individuals would go through these stages in order but might also go back to earlier stages. People in the later stages, e.g. maintenance, would tend to focus on the benefits (I feel healthier after giving up smoking), whereas people in the earlier stages tend to focus on the costs (I will be at a social disadvantage if I give up smoking). A relationship has been found between level of education and the stage of change reached when contemplating taking regular exercise. Those people with lower levels of education tended to be at an earlier stage of change (Booth et al. 1993), and therefore it could be argued that the model could be improved by taking account educational attainment in order to help predict the length of time a person is likely to remain at the earlier stages. Support for individual components of the model. Norman and Fitter (1989) examined health behaviour screening (for example breast cervical cancer) and found that perceived barriers (the costs of attending) were the greatest predictors of whether a person attended the clinic. Several studies have examined breast selfexamination (BSE) behaviour and report that barriers (Lashley 1987; Wyper 1990) and perceived susceptibility (the likelihood of having the illness) (Wyper 1990) are the best predictors of healthy behaviour. The role of giving information as a cue to action has been researched. Information in the form of fear-arousing warnings may change attitudes and health behaviour in such areas as dental health, safe driving and smoking (e.g. Sutton 1982; Sutton and Hallett 1989). Giving information about the bad effects of smoking is also effective in preventing smoking and in getting people to give up (e.g. Sutton 1982; Flay 1985). Several studies report a significant relationship between people knowing about an illness and their taking precautions. Rimer et al. (1991) report that knowledge about breast cancer is related to having regular mammograms. Several studies have also indicated a positive correlation between knowledge about BSE (Breast Selfexamination) and breast cancer and performing BSE (Alagna and Reddy 1984; Lashley 1987; Champion 1990). Showing subjects a video about pap tests for cervical cancer was related to their actually having the pap test (O'Brien and Lee 1990'.) Janz and Becker (1984) found that healthy behavioural intentions are related to low perceived seriousness - not high as predicted (e.g. healthy adult having a flu injection) - and several studies have suggested an association between low susceptibility (not high) and healthy behaviour (e.g. many students recently have agreed to be inoculated against meningitis) (Becker et al. 1975; Langlie 1977). Hill et al. (1985) applied the HBM to cervical cancer, to examine which factors predicted cervical screening behaviour. Their results suggested that benefits and perceived seriousness were not related. Janz and Becker (1984) carried out a study using the HBM and found the best predictors of health behaviour to be perceived barriers and perceived susceptibility to illness. However, Becker and Rosenstock (1984), in a review of 19 studies using a meta-analysis that included measures of the HBM to predict compliance, calculated that the best predictors of compliance are the costs and benefits and the perceived seriousness. So there is lack of agreement over what really does help to predict health behaviour. Is health behaviour that rational? (Is toothbrushing really determined by weighing up the pros and cons?). Its emphasis on the individual (HBM ignores social and economic factors) The measurement of each component The absence of a role for emotional factors such as fear and denial. It has been suggested that alternative factors may predict health behaviour, such as outcome expectancy (whether the person feels they will be healthier as a result of their behaviour) and self-efficacy (the person’s belief in their ability to carry out preventative behaviour) (Seydel et al. 1990; Schwarzer 1992). Schwarzer (1992) has further criticized the HBM for saying nothing about how attitudes might change. Leventhal et al. (1985) have argued that health-related behaviour is related more to the way in which people interpret their symptoms (e.g. if you feel unwell and you feel it is not going to cure itself then you would probably do something about it). Becker and Rosenstock (1987) have revised the HBM and have described their new model as consisting of the following factors: the existence of sufficient motivation; the belief that one is susceptible or vulnerable to a serious problem; and the belief that change following a health recommendation would be beneficial to the individual at a level of acceptable cost. Rogers (1975, 1983, 1985) developed protection motivation theory (PMT) which expanded the HBM to include additional factors. Components of the PMT Health-related behaviours are a product of five components: Coping Appraisal self-efficacy (e.g. 'I am confident that I can change my diet'); Response effectiveness (e.g. 'changing my diet would improve my health'); Threat Appraisal Severity (e.g. 'bowel cancer is a serious illness'); Vulnerability (e.g. 'my chances of getting bowel cancer are high'). Fear According to the PMT, there are two sources of information: 1. environmental (e.g. verbal persuasion, observational learning) and 2. intrapersonal (e.g. prior experience). This information elicits either an 'adaptive' coping response (i.e. the intention to improve one's health) or a 'maladaptive' coping response (e.g. avoidance, denial). Rippetoe and Rogers (1987) gave women information about breast cancer and examined the effect of this information on the components of the PMT and their relationship to the women's intentions to practise breast self-examination (BSE). The results showed that the best predictors of intentions to practise BSE were response effectiveness (believing that BSE would detect the early signs of cancer), severity (believing that Breast cancer is dangerous and difficult to treat in it's advanced stages) and self-efficacy (belief in one's ability to carry out BSE effectively). In a further study, the effects of persuasive appeals for increasing exercise on intentions to exercise were evaluated using the components of the PMT. The results showed that vulnerability (ill health would result from lack of exercise) and self-efficacy (believing in one's ability to exercise effectively) predicted exercise intentions but that none of the variables were related to self-reports of actual behaviour. In a further study, Beck and Lund (1981) manipulated dental students' beliefs about tooth decay using persuasive communication. Their results showed that the information increased fear and that severity (tooth decay has disastrous consequences) and self-efficacy (I can do something about it) were related to behavioural intentions (flossing and brushing regularly especially after eating). The PMT has been less widely criticized than the HBM; however, many of the criticisms of the HBM also relate to the PMT. For example, the PMT assumes that individuals are rational information processors (although it does include an element of irrationality in its fear component), it does not account for habitual behaviours, such as brushing teeth, nor does it include a role for social (what others do) and environmental factors (eg opportunities to exercise or eat properly at work). Schwarzer (1992) has also criticized the PMT for not tackling how attitudes might change (a problem with the HBM as well). Communication between health professional and patient would be redundant if the patient held beliefs about their health that were in conflict with those held by the professional. Pill and Stott (1982) reported that working-class mothers were more likely to see illness as uncontrollable. In a recent study, Graham (1987) reported that although women who smoke are aware of all the health risks of smoking, they report that smoking is necessary to their well-being and an essential means for coping with stress. Blaxter (1990) analysed the definitions of health provided by over 9000 British adults in the health and lifestyles survey. She classified the responses into nine categories: Health as not-ill: the absence of physical symptoms. Health despite disease. Health as reserve: the presence of personal resources. Health as behaviour: the extent of healthy behaviour Health as physical fitness. Health as vitality. Health as social relationships. Health as function. It was found that there was considerable agreement in the emphasis on behavioural factors as causes of illness. There was however limited reference to structural or environmental factors, especially among those from working-class backgrounds. Gender differences were also found. The women were more likely to define health in terms of personal relationships. Murray and McMillan (1988) also found that working class women made repeated reference to their families when describing cancer. Chamberlain (1997) noted a series of social class differences in his review of several studies of lay people’s perceptions of health. Lower social economic status people emphasise the role of health in their ability to work whereas higher social economic status people referred more to their ability to participate in leisure activities. Four different lay views of health emerged: 1. Lower social economic status participants only reported a view that emphasised physical aspects. 2. Both lower and higher social economic status participants gave a dualistic view in which physical and mental aspects of health were combined. 3. Predominantly higher social economic status gave a complimentary view of health, which integrated both physical and mental dimensions. 4. Higher social economic status participants gave a multiple view of health, which included physical, mental, emotional, social and spiritual directions. Stainton-Rogers (1991) used Q-sort methodology to identify the concepts used by a sample of British adults to explain health. She identified eight different accounts of health and illness: The ‘body as machine’ account which considered illness as naturally occurring and ‘real’ with biomedicine considered the main form of treatment. The ‘body under siege’ account which considered illness as a result of external influences such as germs or stress. The ‘inequality of access’ account which emphasized the unequal access to modern medicine. The ‘cultural critique’ account which was based upon a sociological worldview of exploitation and oppression. The ‘health promotion’ account which recognized both individual and collective responsibility for ill health. The ‘robust individualism’ account which was concerned with every individual’s right to a satisfying life. The ‘willpower account’ which defined health in terms of the individuals ability to exert control. 1. Humans are rational in their information processing. It is the role of perceived factors (e.g. risk, rewards, costs, etc) rather than actual risks. 2. Different cognitions are separate from and perform independently from each other. Could be because the researchers ask questions relating to each 'type' of cognition. 3. The types of cognition may not really exist nor play a part in the patient's thinking about their health; they could just be an artefact of the way the research was carried out. 4. Cognitions are not placed within a context. For example, actual social pressure and environment are not taken into account, only the individual's interpretation of social pressure and environmental influences.