Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Teaching and Learning Research Programme Annual Conference Papers 5th Annual Conference, 22-24 November 2004 Cardiff Marriott Hotel Naturalistic Observation of Small Group Work in Key Stage 1 Classrooms. Part 1: The Social Semiotic Landscape of the Primary Classroom Steve Hodgkinson University of Brighton NB: This paper was presented at an internal TLRP conference; if you wish to quote from it please contact the authors directly for permission. Contact details for each project and thematic initiative can be found on our website (www.tlrp.org). Naturalistic Observation of Small Group Work in Key Stage 1 Classrooms . Part 1: The Social Semiotic Landscape of the Primary Classroom The physical environment of the classroom is one of many diverse social contexts encountered by a child; recognisable in many shapes and forms across the world, its core features have remained essentially unchanged throughout centuries of concomitant social change. Whilst such durability stands as a powerful testament to the success of the classroom as a mode of social organisation and learning (Kushner, Simon et al. 2001), it is also indicative of how decades of curriculum research, innovation and reform have failed to significantly change the fundamental organisational principles of the classroom (Galton, Hargreaves et al. 1999). Although as a physical environment classrooms vary enormously, they all seem to possess the same essential features that vividly reflect the discontinuities of power and authority that shape classroom organisational practices (Galton, Simon et al. 1980); (Galton, Hargreaves et al. 1999); (Hastings 2002), that legitimate teacher control of classroom ‘dialogue’ (Barnes 1976); (Mehan 1979); (Wood and Wood 1984); (Denscombe 1985); (Brierley, Cassar et al. 1992), that establish and enforce the rules governing the ownership, organisation and negotiation of knowledge, and what should be claimed as ‘valued’ knowledge (Cullingford 1991). This presentation will outline some of the social semiotic determinants of effective group work in of Key Stage 1 classrooms that have emerged from a series of naturalistic observations undertaken over a period of 2 years. Classrooms reflect to a greater or lesser extent, the dominant contemporary issues that society engages with. They illuminate debates regarding the social stratification of discourse and the disenfranchisement of certain groups of children (Bernstein 1973) (Gregory and Williams 2000); gender inequalities in talk in the classroom that reflect wider (gender-based) societal inequalities (Swann and Graddol 1994), the negative impact that political imperatives have had upon the nurturing of relationships in the classroom (Osborn 1997); (Galton and Fogelman 1998), and the value of ‘unofficial literacies’ (Delpit 1988); (Biggs and Edwards 1991); (Gregory 1999). At a macro level, such discontinuities are predicated upon, and reinforced by the reproduction and reification of middle and upper class values in schools (Bernstein 1971), and the way in which these ‘accoutrements of the culture of power’ (Delpit 1988), remain hidden from the majority, and only accessible by the few. At a micro level, these reified values cause asymmetries in the classroom, where some pupils (and adults) hold tacit knowledge not available to others. Douglas Barnes and Frankie Todd describe how this ‘ control of relevance’ is placed in the hands of dominant individuals (during pupil – pupil or teacher - pupil dialogue) and how this effectively determines who may participate in or is excluded from classroom dialogue, and crucially, frames what is deemed suitable for discussion (Barnes and Todd 1977). These discontinuities drive the currents, and establish the counter-currents of human socialisation. Pierre Bourdieu suggested that these discontinuities are deeply enshrined in extant social structures, and act in a way that very effectively casts the conditions for their reproduction (Bourdieu 1977). The social groups that coalesce around such social structures (the boundaries of which seem mainly to reflect the delineations established by social class) 2 are therefore schooled in competencies that reinforce certain discourses (identity, gender, culture, education etc.), and determine which social and cultural capital available to them. Education is just one form of social reproduction, with its own social mediations and processes. Some social groups are effectively disenfranchised from educational opportunities by pre-existing social inequalities, which allow them only a limited access to (and accumulation of) social and cultural capital. As Basil Bernstein suggested in his article ‘Education Cannot Compensate for Society’ (Bernstein 1970), the challenge for a deficit model of education, which is based upon certain cultural (and essentially middle class) values, is not to make assumptions about the value of certain sources of knowledge over others, which effectively disenfranchise some children from participating fully in the discourse of the classroom. ‘ If the culture of the teacher is to become part of the consciousness of the child, then the culture of the child must first be in the consciousness of the teacher’ (Bernstein 1970) In the context of the classroom, Lisa Delpit suggests that such inequalities are realised through a ‘culture of power’, with its own codes / rules of participation, and which reflect the rules of those that have the power (and she adds, ‘that are usually the least aware or perhaps least willing to acknowledge this power’) (Delpit 1988). This can, and often does lead to a hiatus in the dialogic process, what she calls a ‘silenced dialogue’, where certain social groups become effectively disenfranchised from the decision making process. She argues that these children needed to have access to, and also (critically) an awareness of the ‘codes’ in order to participate fully in mainstream education. A range of different discursive styles codes, and strategies are evident in the classroom (Biggs and Edwards 1991). The sociocultural origins of these styles, codes and strategies, lay their effective social stratification (Hymes 1971), and Hymes, and others have suggested (Malinowski 1923); (Gumperz 1971) that certain social groups are able to develop a colloquial language that has the same functionality, but parallels, and is distinct from the ‘mainstream’. All language (or more appropriately discourse) then, has implicit situational and cultural cues. In this sense at least, the term ‘code’ used by Delpit, seems to extend the concept of linguistic codes developed by Bernstein and Hymes (Bernstein 1971); (Hymes 1971) and adapted in the work of Labov, to incorporate a much more explicit link with the later concept of discursive practice (Foucault 1977), where a tacit selection of appropriate discourses appropriate for a particular occasion is made by the individual (Labov 1966). Delpit suggests that the relative success of this alternative style in terms of negotiating meaning, is 3 contingent on them (the ‘speakers’ of the code) being able to acknowledge and negotiate their own expertness (and here I draw the attention of the reader to Lave and Wenger’s notion of legitimate peripheral participation. Further Defining the Classroom Environment Robin Alexander has argued that certain issues raised by studying classrooms, seem removed from the influence of their cultural or geographical location. For example, he asks ‘how does one characterise the ‘typical’ whilst at the same time preserving the authentic’? He suggests that this is possible, that is to ‘capture’ both the insightful and the typical aspects of classroom ‘life’, but only if the researcher is prepared to acknowledge that cultural norms and imperatives have a powerful influence on the character of classroom ‘life’, and at the same time also be open to, (and part of) the varied forms of communication and interaction that occur in the classroom (Alexander 2000). Thus, in any social semiotic analysis of classrooms, we need to clearly identify these two facets of classroom ‘life’. First, we must make conceptually explicit just what is meant when we talk about ‘classrooms’. Are we in fact just talking about the bounded physical space in a ‘typical’ school, or are we talking about any virtual or physical space? And second, and perhaps more importantly for those of us interested in the nature of discourse, we need to recognise the enormous diversity of opportunities for meaning making, and adopt a concept of the classroom where we challenge, modify or even abandon competing discourses? Thus the classroom may be seen as a transient and dynamic discursive space, what Gunther Kress calls; “…the material expression of the motivated (cognitive and affective) choices of teachers and students from among the meaning-making resources available in a particular situation….at a given time” (Kress, Jewitt et al. 2001) Therefore it seems evident that to understand how meaning is signed and signified in the classroom, we have to first understand how different layers of contextual complexity are interwoven into the discursive practices of the classroom, and how these layers conflate in the negotiation of meaning that takes place therein. For example, the starting point of a Key Stage 1 classroom represents a ‘physical space’ that can be observed and annotated, where the temporality of space and action can be characterised; and where we might search for the ‘interactional choreography’ that facilitates the situated negotiation of meaning and the transformation of identities. 4 Classroom Discourse as a Dimension of Negotiated Meaning From a sociocultural standpoint, the attribution of negotiation of meaning and identity transformation extends to a much wider context than the physical space of the classroom. The classroom is just one of a ‘constellation’ of discourses a child engages with, and these discourses are diverse, there may well overlap in terms of membership, or they may exhibit commonalities of practice, temporality or physical location, they may have distinct ‘boundaries’ or seem to coalesce. The negotiation of meaning in the classroom is the result of an amalgamation of all these discourses, each child contributing (brokering), to a greater or lesser extent, situated meanings from many different discourses, some of which overlap with other children in the class, some which do not. We should also start with a working definition of discourse. In its narrowest sense, discourse refers to the ‘spoken and written forms of language use as social practice’ (Wood and Kroger 2000), and the way in which social perspectives are syntactically (through social structures) and semantically (in metaphorical narrative) embedded in discourse (Sacks 1984). Discourse in a broader sense may be viewed as a socially constructed knowledge of reality that moves beyond language to encompass different (extant) semiotic modes, and is also evolutionary as new discourses may develop from transformations of existing modes of representation (Kress and van Leeuwen 2001). This perhaps better characterises the complex contribution of social, cognitive and pedagogic processes to any observed discourses in the classroom, in broader terms the concept of a ‘community of discourse’, and how it might be established in the classroom (Swales 1990). Wenger developed a sociocultural theory of learning from his work on the concept of communities of practice, and identified four underlying components (meaning, practice, community and identity). His observations suggest that the identification of characteristics representing the three theoretical dimensions of a CoP (mutual engagement, negotiated enterprise, and a shared repertoire of resources) would provide a useful starting point when designing an analytical framework for observations conducted in the context of the classroom, and to the influence of social background of the learners on their participation in the community of practice (Perret-Clermont 1980); (Bell, Grossen et al. 1985) Bernstein’s characterisation of the communication that occurs in different social groups (shaped as it was by indicators such as social status, occupational function, communal 5 bonds, collective rather than individual action, physical manipulation and control rather than symbolic organisation and control), led him to suggest that in certain (lower socioeconomic) groups there was an emphasis placed on the verbal exposition of communal rather than individual identity, of the practical rather than abstract (Bernstein 1975). Although the narrow socioeconomic definition of social groups used by Bernstein would need extending to incorporate more directly other mediators of identity such as culture and religion, nevertheless, it does suggest that the discontinuities that arise between home and school discourses, may be the result of the members of these groups having to realign rather than to renegotiate their identities. Thus an exploration of the concept of identity is critical to further understanding the underlying processes of collective, negotiated meaning. Wenger (Wenger 1998) posits identity as a ‘nexus of multi-membership’, a composite of our negotiated experiences of the world, and reflecting, like the changing landscapes of a long journey, our passage through life. As such, the notion of identity is more than just a reification of the social discourses of different communities, which often represents their ‘public’ persona, but also the participation in and (lived) experience of being a member of these different communities. Identity represents a duality of identification (investment of the self by association with and differentiations within various practices) and negotiability (the degree to which we become invested in, and are able to fashion meaning making). Sometimes, these elements of identity do in fact seem to coalesce, and become something tangible. For example, in Life Narratives (which may be viewed in this context as a reflection of our individual trajectories through the various CoPs we are members of) often include (retrospectively) the ‘logical steps’ that we assume were necessarily there, and that represented coherence and closure at different times in our lives (Linde 1993). There is no doubt however, that whatever perspective we view discourse from, the view is contextual and dialogic in nature (Bakhtin 1981). Traditionally, socio-cognitive research has focussed upon the individual’s role in the ‘interpretation’ of competing discourses, whether alone or as a member of a group, and not, with a few exceptions, on the group as a collective (Doise and Mugny 1984) (DeVries 1997). Thus the social context is seen, essentially as Piaget had suggested in his earlier work (Piaget 1932) as an arena for the individual to structure their own understanding by comparing and interpreting their views against the views of others (relational and social decentring) (Perret-Clermont 1980); (Smith 1993); (Mackie and Smith 1998). Previous work by Doise and Mugny suggested that the contrasting views held by children about the same concept or event, lead to a cycle of sociocognitive conflict, conflict resolution and subsequent cognitive restructuring. In this way, the 6 intentional learning of an individual occurs in two phases; first (interpersonally) through interaction with other people or artefacts, a dynamic that establishes what Vygotsky has termed the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD). This ZPD represents a period of intellectual reciprocity consistent with Doise and Mugny’s conflict – resolution stage. Second, there is an intrapersonal phase (cognitive restructuring) where new knowledge is incorporated into the individuals’ cognition (Vygotsky 1978); (Tharp and Gallimore 1988). Whilst it is apparent from his writing that Vygotsky placed great emphasis than Piaget on the primacy of the social world over the individual (and the importance of cultural tools and artefacts) for cognitive development (Cole and Wertsch 1996), the lack of a collective context for learning in his ‘sociocultural’ theories of cognitive development is problematic. From a socio-cultural perspective, all human endeavour is viewed as intrinsically social, and the synthesis of, alignment to, and reproduction of competing discourses then becomes situated in the context of the group rather than the individual. Here group is used in its most informal sense to represent a practice, a community, a coalescence of identities. Groups are not therefore identified by rigid boundaries of size, location, time or style, but by the negotiation of, and participation in meaning making, they are the simultaneous articulation of structure and discontinuity. The meaning making that occurs in these ‘communities of practice’ is thus a transformation, reflecting changing participation in and alignment to the discourse(s) of the community. If meaning making then refers to our changing ability to engage in different discourses that delineate and structure our ‘world’, what is such engagement contingent upon? James Gee (Gee 1992) talks about semantic mediational theories (mediating between words and the world) as being essentially ideological (involving assumptions about ‘value’, tacit or otherwise), setting up as they do central and marginal cases, hierarchies of experiences, things, and people. These are essentially cultural models (Holland and Quinn 1987) that establish definitions and norms, but they also reveal counter definitions and identities that are ‘threats’ to the norms of a culture (Holland and Skinner 1987). By a process of heurisis, or what Tharp calls the ‘Great Cycle of Social Sorting’ (Tharp, Estrada et al. 2000) some practices become accepted, privileged and protected (enshrined as social practices by those that are not marginalised by them, and in turn creating (inducing) the cultural models). Social practices, and the cues that are aligned with them, vary across both social groups and across time (Rogoff and Lave 1984) (Giddens 1991). They form ‘threads’ running through the different discourses we participate in (discourses are interpreted here to include people, tools and artefacts, ways of talking and interacting, values and interpretations) that might sometimes coalesce into broader notions, such as say success (Gee 1992). 7 Practice then is about shaping and reshaping of shared historical and social resources. Situated social practices provide nested and hierarchical apprenticeships (for example, the social practice of reading at home acts as an apprenticeship into the social practice of literature) that are shaped by complex and reciprocal patterns of discourse. That these social practices moderate, and are in turn moderated by different discourses, is a reflection of the way in which discourses too are ideological (related to the distribution of power and the hierarchical structures present in society) (Gee 1996). Bernstein observed that ‘how a society selects, classifies, distributes, transmits and evaluates educational knowledge it considers public, reflects both the distribution of power and the principles of social control’ (Bernstein 1975). The ‘validation’ of particular forms of educational knowledge is illustrated in what Bernstein calls the ‘collected curriculum’, and exemplified for example in contemporary primary education by the discourses that underpin the National Literacy Strategy, and in the dominant discourses of the education system which deny the value of home or school communities of practice, their rich social and cultural capital, and where the spontaneous use of genres, registers and other symbolic transformations that are characteristic of children’s play are much more widely exhibited (Wood 2002). Bourdieu and Passeron 1990 suggest that individuals bring various forms of capital to their communities in the form of attitudes, values, experiences, brokered knowledge wealth etc. Such a process is both dynamic and bilateral, and perhaps in some circumstances, may also act to reproduce and reinforce inequalities in, and boundaries to participation in educational discourses. Therefore, the complex semiotic landscape that contextualises a socio-cultural description of engagement with different discourses, necessitates a reassessment of the role of the classroom as a context for meaning making. It is clear that whilst classrooms are more often than not designed as utilitarian spaces (Bennett, Andreae et al. 1980) and not specifically as contexts for social interaction that children would recognise from other aspects of their lives, the atmosphere they (classrooms) create has a profound influence upon the children that occupy them. 1.4 The Dialectical Context of the Classroom Cedric Cullingford describes classrooms as ‘visually crowded spaces’ that reflect the juxtaposition of children’s work, books, administrative information (lists, rotas events) and equipment (Cullingford 1991). Often these colourful wall displays seem to have a purely decorative function, their significance as artefacts or tools is diminished, and therefore rarely considered or reflect upon by the children (Cullingford, 1978). 8 Classrooms cannot be viewed simply as physically bounded environments, but as dialectical contexts, with multiple layers of collaboration, negotiation, interpretation and transformation that together constitute meaning making (Kress, Jewitt et al. 2001). Bronfenbrenner’s notion of the concentric layers of context surrounding an individual during development (Bronfenbrenner 1979). This has been adapted more recently by Cole (Cole 1996) to illustrate how cultural contexts might also influence individual behaviour, and how the boundaries between these contexts may not be necessarily as distinct as Bronfenbrenner had originally suggested. Tudge 1997? emphasised the importance of both the hierarchical and temporal dimensions of the relationships between these layered contexts. Thus it is not enough to focus on an individual, group or community, without understanding how these layers of context relate to one another. Studies of Classroom Interaction Early studies of the dynamics of classroom interaction tended to focus on the relationship between the pedagogical approach of the teacher, and the nature of the responses elicited by this approach from the class (Bellack, Kliebard et al. 1966). Typically, a triadic pattern was often found, with the teacher initiating a discussion by positing a question (which in fact was often rhetorical) to the class (the Initiation step). Individual students could then respond with a suggested answer (the Response step), and this response would then be ‘evaluated’ by the teacher (the Feedback step). This triadic pattern became widely known as the IRF model (Sinclair and Coulthard 1975), and may be observed in many contemporary classrooms. However, questions may be asked and are used in classroom settings for a number of different reasons. They may act as a method of confirming that others have appropriated the necessary factual information or perhaps understood instructions given to them (procedural questions). Teachers may also confine or stimulate discussion and the (joint) negotiation of meaning that takes place in the classroom by the nature of the questions they ask (Orsolini & Pontecorvo, 1992) (Wells 1993). The work of Douglas Barnes has provided a much clearer understanding of the role of questioning in the classroom (Barnes 1976) (Barnes and Todd 1977). Barnes found that teachers often ask questions which are closed, that is questions that assume that are predicated on their being one correct answer. He (and later others such as Neil Mercer and Rupert Wegeriff) suggested that questions which were more exploratory (open) in nature, and which allowed the students to speculate about the different possible answers to the teacher’s question, fostered a more critical engagement with the underlying ideas Mercer, 1994 Barnes & Todd, 1995. The 9 ORACLE (Observational Research and Classroom Learning Evaluation) studies of primary classrooms (Galton, Simon et al. 1980) (Galton, Hargreaves et al. 1999) provides evidence that, regrettably, in nearly thirty years of pedagogical research and innovation since the classroom observations by Barnes took place, questioning in the context of the classroom is largely a means of checking the appropriation of facts or instructions. The ORACLE studies indicated that between the initial work in the mid seventies, and the subsequent follow up study some twenty years later, the percentage of closed and procedural questions as a proportion of all questions asked by teachers changed little (95% in 1976, 90% in 1996), even though the overall contact time allocated to questioning had increased over the same period. These data indicated however, that questions relating to general on-task supervision had decreased dramatically, being largely replaced by closed questions, and a small increase in the use of exploratory questions. Galton and co-workers suggest that such patterns are indicative of a contemporary primary classroom where; “Today’s teachers devote even more of their time to telling pupils facts and ideas or giving directions than their counterparts of twenty years ago………..at the expense of silent interactions such as monitoring pupil’s progress and listening to pupils read” (Galton, Hargreaves et al. 1999) This inertia, the limited evidence that the student is an active participant in the negotiation of meaning, is particularly notable given the (contemporaneous) changes that have occurred in the theoretical perspectives used to describe the dialogic nature of meaning making in the classroom (Wells, 1999). For some, this change of perspective focuses on the learner being guided in learning in a premeditated way by a more capable mentor (Kumpulainen & Wray, 2002), which incidentally takes place within the collective activities of a learning community. From such a perspective then, learning remains firmly situated in the individual, but occurs and is structured in a social context (Brown and Palinscar 1989; Brown et al., 1996). For example, Brown and colleagues have developed an approach over many years that encompasses the notion of the conditional scaffolding of an individual student’s learning (Wood, Bruner & Ross, 1976), but within the context of small group discussions, so called ‘communities of learners and thinkers’ (Brown and Campione 1990). This reciprocal approach to learning has been widely explored (and been given favourable reviews, (Rosenshine & Meister, 1994) within the context of the classroom, but does not engage with the wider connotations of being a member of a learning community. More recent work has attempted to bridge this epistemological divide by structuring group activities in the 10 classroom so that they provide opportunities for collective argumentation and meaning making (based on the work of Miller, 1987 and developed by Brown & Renshaw, 1996). This pedagogical approach acknowledges the importance of the transformative nature of dialogue in the negotiation of meaning, and importantly, situates this negotiation of meaning within the context of a community of learners, and not an individual (Newman, Griffin & Cole, 1989; Rogoff, 1990; Lave & Wenger 1991). This marks a line in the sand, a transition from a sociocognitive, individualistic perspective of learning, where individual knowledge is shaped by comparison, interpretation and conflict resolution, a journey that we all undertake, a rite of passage. We now start to embrace a more sociocultural perspective of learning, one that emphasises learning as taking place through our joint engagement in actions and interactions. As such learning may now be thought of as the production, reproduction and transformation of social structures (Wenger 1998), we negotiate access to, and legitimate participation in different social configurations (social groups) such as our family, the school class, work, teams, countries etc. Whilst sociocognitive approaches to classroom interaction have tended to investigate influences on the ‘learning’ of the individual in different social contexts, sociocultural approaches have highlighted the importance of the collective activities of the social groups found in the context of a classroom (Resnick 1987) (Brown, Collins et al. 1989) (Bielaczyc and Collins 1999). This is particularly interesting for those advancing group work as an appropriate and effective facilitator of collaborative learning, as almost all of the major studies of formal (structured) group work in the classroom over the last thirty years, have adopted a predominantly sociocognitive perspective (Kutnick and Rogers 1994) Mercer, 1995} (Mercer 1995). Mercer, reflecting upon this fact, and the fragmented nature of our current understanding of classroom discourse suggests that; “…. the kind of theory {of how talk is used to guide the construction of knowledge} which would be most helpful to researchers and teachers would need to do three things. First it must explain how language (sic) is used to create joint knowledge and understanding. Secondly, it must explain how people help other people to learn. And third, the theory must deal with the special nature and purpose of formal education” {Mercer, Op. Cit.} In a similar way then, any theoretical treatise on the ‘value’ of group work in the classroom that is developed from a sociocultural perspective, must also clearly identify how group participation facilitates the negotiation of understanding, and the constraints of such an approach in the context of the primary classroom. 11 1.6 Group Membership Groups establish or are established, evolve and dissipate in classrooms according to a complex set of social imperatives. (Aldridge 2001). Once in the classroom, groups may be established out of school friendships, or from seating arrangements, whilst others emerge as a consequence of particular teaching styles or as a result of specific assessments of ability; each represent distinct but overlapping opportunities for, and influences on the collaborative negotiation of meaning. Research on the opportunities for, benefits of and issues with groups in the classroom is vast, but to a significant extent it reflects the recent move towards sociocultural theories of teaching and learning (Mercer, 2000; Kumpulainen & Wray, 2002), and the cognitive (and sociocognitive) theories of individual learning of Lev Vygotsky and Jean Piaget (Cole and Wertsch, 1996). Although both perspectives attribute a degree of importance to social context in learning, sociocultural and cognitive / sociocognitive perspectives on the nature of groups and group work are distinct enough to be compared and contrasted within the overarching organisational framework of classroom-based group work. General surveys of classroom-based group work (Galton, Simon et al. 1980) Galton & Williamson, 1992; (Galton, Hargreaves et al. 1999), have found consistently that the implementation of grouping practices (by the teacher) in the primary classroom rarely extended beyond that of being an organisational device. There was little evidence from this work that the opportunities for collaborative activity was realised when children were seated together. A clear message to emerge from the (ORACLE: Observational Research and Classroom Learning Evaluation) research, was that the ‘value’ of group work as a pedagogic tool was to a large extent determined by several key factors. First, there should be a clear identification of its ‘fitness for purpose’ (Edwards, 1994). Edwards suggests that practitioners should view the value of group work in terms of whether or not it has the potential to enhance pupil learning, whether it will achieve the desired outcomes. However, by focussing too directly on the prescribed outcomes of group work, there is a danger that the exploratory dialogue so central to the development of a child’s critical thinking, is largely suppressed or even absent (Barnes 1976) (Fisher, 1993; Mercer, 1994; 2002). Second, there should be evidence of prior classroom planning and organisation (Doyle, 1986), so Bennett et al. (1984) have suggested that classroom-based group work needs to have clearly identified goals, which are based on some prior assessment of a knowledge deficit. Thus, the purpose of structured group work (from a neo-Vygotskian perspective at least) is to act as a constructive framework for individual learning.. Such an approach is consonant with the 12 experimental group work designs used by (Slavin 1983; 1990), the Jigsaw Model (Aronson 1978), the Group Investigation approach (Johnson & Johnson 1975), and with the notion of individualised Assisted Performance developed by Tharp & Gallimore (Tharp and Gallimore 1988); all represent staged applications of the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) model (Vygotsky 1978). However, and significantly, in adopting this approach, any training in the broader group working skills that would support general classroom-based collaborative group work, and that focuses specifically upon understanding group processes, has been omitted (Webb, 1989; Bennett & Dunne, 1990?; Kutnick & Marshall, 1993). But this does not mark the true epistemological boundary between sociocognitive and sociocultural perspectives on group work, merely a relatively brief period of evolution in pedagogical practices to accommodate a more socio-constructivist viewpoint of learning, one that is a reflection of the greater attention now paid to the social context in which learning takes place. A key element of any sociocultural approach to group work is a recognition of the central importance of the group as a collective, a whole, an organism that, whilst exhibiting many separable characteristics and elements, is nonetheless more than just the individuals that it is composed of, more than the sum of the unique contributions these individuals make to it. Hence the discursive choreography of meaning making that is so evident when we observe group work in the classroom (Barnes and Todd 1977), reflects the synergistic interaction of many individual learning trajectories (Wenger 1998) rather than simply an amalgamation of parts. Methodology The strategies, methods and materials employed by a qualitative researcher in the conduct of their research have often been likened to a Bricolage, an emergent construction of closely interwoven and reflexive practices that embody (in full representation) the problem they have before them (Becker 1989; Orr 1996). Thus Levi – Strauss described the anthropologist (and as such, by custom and practice we can extend this to a generalisation for the qualitative researcher) as a Bricoleur, as one who adopts a position of establishing the fitness for purpose of different resources available to them throughout the course of their research. In this process, experience and practice evolve in a hermeneutic cycle rather than being an extant set of practices that are applied at any one particular point in time; ‘His (sic) universe of instruments is closed and the rules of his game is to always make do with “whatever is at hand”, that is to say with a set of tools and materials which is always 13 finite and is also heterogeneous because what it contains bears no resemblance to the current project or indeed to any particular project, but is the contingent result of all the occasions there have been to renew or enrich the stock…….He has to turn (back) to an already existent set made of tools and materials, to consider or reconsider what it contains and, finally and above all, to engage in a sort of dialogue with it…..’ (Levi-Strauss 1966) The qualitative researcher is engaged at any one moment in time in multiple and evolving practices and discourses, working between different but overlapping theoretical perspectives, and in constructing a personal narrative of their journey. Thus qualitative research is best construed as a collection of interpretive practices containing the distinct traces of individual historical location and situated meaning, and difficult to refine to a single discourse. Qualitative research also seems to be held in a constant tension by the embrace of dominant ideologies that stream across its boundaries from all directions, driven by changing ethical, economic and political doctrines. These embraces are reified in the rites of passage. Much of the debate centres on how the negotiation of meaning is interpreted by the different methodological practices of the qualitative researcher, the deconstruction – reconstruction heuresis (Goffman 1974). All texts (that is discourses realised through different modalities) are open to interpretation to a greater or lesser extent in the practices of the researcher, and whilst different texts afford different interpretations and negotiable meanings, they are always multimodal in their representations, and they can always be ‘read’ in different ways (Kress, Jewitt et al. 2001; Kress and van Leeuwen 2001). Thus whilst the research methodology and methods described in this chapter circumscribes the interpretation of how meaning is negotiated in the classroom, because it necessarily adopts a particular epistemological position (that is, one framed by a sociocultural theory of collectives), it will be argued here that other interpretations have no greater legitimacy when considering how the complex interaction of discourses within the classroom resolve to the negotiation of meaning. For example, theories of cognition that render the individual as a scientist, a traveller locked into predefined cognitive trajectories (Piaget and Inhelder 1958), or as existing in a definable and monolithic social context (the home, the classroom) (Cole, Gay et al. 1971; Cole, Hood et al. 1978; Bronfenbrenner 1979), bear no greater or lesser resistance to scrutiny in an interpretivist framework, they are simply different and competing accounts. Observing at what happens in a classroom during the course of a lesson, whilst 14 employing a naturalistic observational framework, is therefore the construction of a narrative, a journey in words and pictures, the traces of interwoven discourses. Methods Adopted The approach adopted in this study was to undertake naturalistic observation of a number of Key Stage 1 classrooms (or their equivalent) at various stages over a period of two years. Classrooms were observed for varying periods (60 to 120 minutes at a time) using a fixed video camcorder, audio recorder and comprehensive fieldnotes taken. Some of these data are presented here, and the nascent themes identified from these observational data, will be used to further define the developmental dynamics of naturalistic groups in a second round of comparative analysis undertaken in classrooms in the UK, Germany, Italy and France). Summary of Findings That collaboration in the classroom might provide a context for the negotiation of meaning has of course been recognised for a long time, but the contemporary philosophy of the educational Establishment still largely reflects an essentially individualistic approach to learning; an approach originally espoused by the Thatcher government as necessary to ensure future economic success. It also follows that, if, according to our sociocultural perspective we situate the negotiation of meaning in the evolving practice of communities, then it is important to understand how (and why) groups actually form, and how some are able transform themselves into communities of practice. The focus here then, is not on the individual within the group (because in sociocultural terms, negotiation of meaning is not a reflection of individual perspective), but rather on the group itself. To view discourse at the level of the individual is problematic as it creates a composite identity of the individual rather than what is in fact a series overlapping but distinct identities. These identities are in a constant state of flux, and reflect a child’s simultaneous membership of many communities of practice. Put more directly, who the child is, is resolved in multiple arenas (only one of which is the classroom), where they negotiate and contribute (to different extents) to meaning making. Identity is about how learning changes who we are and about the creation of personal histories. If we intend to model this mutual negotiation of meaning in the context of the classroom, then it might be better to start with an analysis of how the various texts that articulate the discourses of the classroom (the teacher, fellow pupils, the family etc.) coalesce around the three central strands of mutual engagement, shared repertoires and joint enterprise. Initial evidence suggests that groups of children that come together in the classroom exhibit some all, or none of these strands. In the absence of specific group working skills, it seems to be in the groups of children that are physically remote from the 15 influence of the teacher that one finds the greatest degree of mutualitiy, sharing of ideas, observations and misconceptions, and where there is a sustained effort. This proximity effect is marked and persistent, and requires further exploration in further comparative observations as it constitutes a powerful suppressor of the development of group working skills in the classroom. To the teacher however, these more remote groups are also perceived as constituting a redundancy of effort (if one simply assesses the success of the lesson in terms of time on or off task, or whether the completion of a specific task has been achieved to the level set by the teacher). This might partially explain the reluctance of teachers to engage in truly collaborative or cooperative groupwork where there is an absence of preliminary group -working skills training. It is also highly likely that teachers could enable effective groupworking in the classroom by reassessing their role during groupwork, and by viewing the discourses (from the school, the home and the community) that are expressed in the more remote groups and absent in the groups near to the teacher, as a positive influence on the negotiation of meaning rather than as a deficit in the learning process. 16 Bibliography Aldridge, S. (2001). Social Mobility: A Discussion Paper. London, Performance and Innovation Unit: 45. Alexander, R. (2000). Culture and Pedagogy: International Comparisons in Primary Education. Oxford, Blackwell. Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). Epic and Novel: Towards a Methodology for the Study of the Novel. The Dialogic Imagination: Four Essays by M.M. Bakhtin. M. Holmquist. Austin, University of Texas Press. Barnes, D. (1976). From Communication to Curriculum, Penguin. Barnes, D. and F. Todd (1977). Communication and Learning in Small Groups. London, Routledge and Kegan Paul. Becker, H. S. (1989). "Tricks of the Trade." Studies in Symbolic Interaction 10: 481 490. Bell, N., M. Grossen, et al. (1985). Sociocognitive Conflictand Intellectual Growth. Peer Conflict and Psychological Growth. M. Berkowitz. San Francisco, Jossey-Bass. Bellack, A., H. Kliebard, et al. (1966). The Language of the Classroom. New York, Teachers College Press. Bennett, N., J. Andreae, et al. (1980). Open Plan Schools: Teaching, Curriculum: Design. Windsor, NFER Publishing. Bernstein, B. (1970). Education Cannot Compensate for Society. New Society. 387: 344-347. Bernstein, B. (1971). On the Classification and Framing of Educational Knowledge. Knowledge and Control. M. F. D. Young, Collier-Macmillan. Bernstein, B. (1973). Class, Codes and Control 2: Applied Studies Towards a Sociology of Language. Primary Socilaization, Language and Education. B. Bernstein. London, Routledge and Kegan Paul. 2. Bernstein, B. (1975). Class, Codes and Control Volume 3: Towards a Theory of Educational Transmissions. Primary Socialization, Language and Education. B. Bernstein. London, Routledge and Kegan Paul. 3. Bielaczyc, K. and A. Collins (1999). Learning Communities in Classrooms: A Reconceptualisation of Educational Practice. Instructional design Theories and Models. C. Reigeluth. Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum. Biggs, A. and V. Edwards (1991). "I Treat Them All the Same: Teacher-Pupil Talk in Multiethnic Classrooms." Language and education 5(3): 161-176. 17 Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge. Brierley, L., I. Cassar, et al. (1992). No, We Ask You the Questions. Thinking Voices: The Work of the National Oracy Project. K. Norman. London, Hodder & Stoughton. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments of Nature and Design. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press. Brown, A. L. and J. C. Campione (1990). "Communties of learning and Thinking, or a Context by Any Other Name." Contributions to Human Development 21: 108-126. Brown, J. S., A. Collins, et al. (1989). "Situated Cognition and the Culture of Learning." Educational Researcher 18: 32-42. Cole, M. (1996). Culture in Mind. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press. Cole, M., J. Gay, et al. (1971). The Cultural Context of Learning and Thinking. New York, Basic Books. Cole, M., L. Hood, et al. (1978). Ecological Niche Picking: Ecological Invalidity as an Axiom of Experimental Cognitive Psychology. San Diego, University of California / The Rockefeller University. Cole, M. and J. Wertsch (1996). Beyond the Individual-Social Antimony in Discussions of Piaget and Vygotsky. Massey, Massey University Virtual Faculty. Cullingford, C. (1991). The Inner World of the School. London, Cassell. Delpit, L. (1988). "The Silenced Dialogue: Power and Pedagogy in Educating Other People's Children." Harvard Educational Review 58(3): 280-298. Denscombe, M. (1985). Classroom Control: A Sociological Perspective. London, Allen & Unwin. DeVries, R. (1997). "Piaget's Social Theory." Educational Researcher 26(2): 4-17. Doise, W. and G. Mugny (1984). The Social development of the Intellect. Oxford, Pergamon Press. Foucault, M. (1977). The Archeology of Knowledge. London, Tavistock. Galton, M. and K. Fogelman (1998). The Use of Discretionary Time in the Primary School. Research Papers in Education. Galton, M., L. Hargreaves, et al. (1999). Inside the Primary Classroom: 20 Years On. London and New York, Routledge. Galton, M., B. Simon, et al. (1980). Inside the Primary Classroom. London, Routledge and Kegan Paul. 18 Gee, J. P. (1992). The Social Mind: Language, Ideology, and Social Practice. New York, Bergin & Garvey. Gee, J. P. (1996). Social Linguistics and Literacies: Ideology in Discourses. London, Falmer Press. Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and Self-Identity: Self and Society in the Late Modern Age, Stanford University Press. Goffman, E. (1974). Frame Analysis: An Essay on the Organisation of Experience. New York, San Francisco, Harper & Row. Gregory, E. (1999). "Myths of Illiteracy: Childhood Memories of Reading in London's East End." Written Language and Literacy 2(1): 89-111. Gregory, E. and A. Williams (2000). City literacies:Learning to Read Across Generations and Cultures. London and New York, Routledge. Gumperz, J. (1971). Language in Social Groups. Selected Essays. A. S. Dil. Stanford, Stanford University Press. Hastings, N. (2002). Re-organising Primary Classroom Learning, Taylor & Francis. Holland, D. and N. Quinn (1987). Cultural Models in Language and Thought. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Holland, D. and D. Skinner (1987). Prestige and Intimacy: The Cultural Models behind American's Talk about Gender Types. Cultural Models in Language and Thought. D. Holland and N. Quinn. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Hymes, D. H. (1971). Competence and Performance in Linguistic Theory. Language Acquisition: Models and Methods. R. Huxley and E. Ingram. London New York, Academic Press. Kress, G., C. Jewitt, et al. (2001). Multimodal Teaching and Learning: The Rhetorics of the Science Classroom. London and New York, Continuum. Kress, G. and T. van Leeuwen (2001). Multimodal Discourse. London, Edward Arnold. Kushner, S., B. Simon, et al. (2001). The Evaluation of the TTA School-Based Research Consortia. Bristol / Southhampton, UWE / University of Southampton: 116. Kutnick, P. and C. Rogers (1994). Groups in Schools. Groups in Schools. P. Kutnick and C. Rogers. London, Cassell: 216. Labov, W. (1966). The Social Stratification of English in New York. Washington DC, Center for Applied Linguistics. 19 Levi-Strauss, C. (1966). The Savage Mind. Chicago, Chicago University Press. Linde, C. (1993). Life Stories: The Creation of Coherence. New York, Oxford University Press. Mackie, D. and E. Smith (1998). "Intergroup Relations: Insights from a Theoretically Integrative Approach." Psychological Review 105(3): 499-529. Malinowski, B. (1923). The Problem of Meaning in Primitive Languages. The Meaning of Meaning. C. Ogden and I. Richards. London, Routledge & Kegan Paul. Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons: Social Organisation in the Classroom. Cambrdige, MA, Harvard University Press. Mercer, N. (1995). THE Guided Construction of Knowledge. Clevedon, Multilingual Matters Ltd. Orr, J. (1996). Talking About Machines. New York, Cornell University Press. Osborn, M. (1997). Policy into Practice and Practice into Policy:Creative Mediation in the Primary Classroom. Teachers and the National Curriculum. G. Haelsby and G. McCulloch. London, Cassell. Perret-Clermont, A. N. (1980). Social Interaction and Cognitive Development in Children. London, Academic Press. Piaget, J. (1932). The Moral Development of the Child. London, Routledge. Piaget, J. and B. Inhelder (1958). The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence. New York, Basic Books. Resnick, L. B. (1987). "Learning in School and Out." Educational Researcher 16: 1320. Rogoff, B. and J. Lave (1984). Everyday Cognition: Its Development in Social Context. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press. Sacks, O. (1984). Seeing Voices. Harmondsworth, Penguin. Sinclair, J. and M. Coulthard (1975). Towards an Analysis of Discourse. London, Oxford University Press. Smith, E. (1993). Social Identity and Social Emotions: Towards new Conceptualisations of Prejudice. Affect, Cognition and Stereotyping: Intercactive Procsesses in Group Perception. D. Mackie and D. Hamilton. San Diego, Academic. Swales, J. (1990). Genre Analysis: English in Academic and Research Settings. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. 20 Swann, J. and D. Graddol (1994). Gender Inequalities in Classroom Talk. Language, Literacy and Learning in Educational Practice. B. Steirer and J. Maybin, Clevedon: Multiliginual Matters. Tharp, R., P. Estrada, et al. (2000). Teaching Transformed: Achieving Execellence, Fairness, Inclusion, and Harmony. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Tharp, R. G. and R. Gallimore (1988). Rousing Minds to Life: Taeching, Learning and Schooling in Social Context. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge MA, Harvard University Press. Wells, G. (1993). "Re-evaluating the IRF Sequence: A proposal for the Articulation of Theories of Discourse for the Analysis of Teaching and Learning in the Classroom." Linguistics and Education 5: 1-37. Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of Practice: Learning, Meaning and Identity. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Wood, E. (2002). Learning Cultures and Discourses in Early Childhood. British Educational Research Association, University of Exeter. Wood, H. and D. Wood (1984). "An Experimental Evaluation of the Effects of Five Styles of Teacher Conversation on the Language of Hearing-Impaired Children." Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 25: 45-62. Wood, L. and R. Kroger (2000). Doing Discourse Analysis. London, Sage. 21