Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Corinne Stevens. P5577126 TMA01 To what extent has children’s development been seen as a social process? Child development refers to the biological and psychological changes occurring during period between birth and adulthood, where individuals progress from immature dependency to mature independence. Much debate has been provoked over whether development is a natural or social process, or a combination of both, with past and present theories arguing that it is not solely a natural process, as suggested by nativist theorists, but rather, a creation of society, where knowledge gained from experience and interaction with other individuals or the environment is the integral force for development. An example of this is the natural process of childbirth being affected by a combination of social factors such as quality of the mother’s diet, availability of emotional and healthcare support, thus impacting on child development before birth. In Western society today, child development research recognises distinct psychological and physical differences between children and adults. However, historically, this has not always been the case, thus raising questions on whether childhood is a “recent invention”. Historian, Philippe Aries argued that Western European, medieval images of children displayed no distinction between child and adult, whereas post fifteenth century images became increasingly focussed on their “special natures and needs”, therefore demonstrating a change in awareness and attitude towards children during this time. Sahar (1990) and other historians rejected this, accusing Aires of generalising from limited evidence, exclusive to the aristocratic community. Nonetheless, Aires noted that there had been a visible change in attitudes towards children and their development which, consequentially, could also be responsible for stirring interest in the origins of, and historical explorations of child development now. “The immaturity of children is a biological fact but the ways in which that immaturity is understood is a fact of culture….childhood is ….constructed and reconstructed both for and by children” (James and Prout, 1997, p.15) James and Prout’s statement acknowledges the natural aspect of development but also pays attention to variables, such as culture, other individuals and children themselves being also active in development. Historically, the principal theories regarding child development stem from four conflicting viewpoints which attempted to characterize the fundamental nature of humankind as personified in the newborn child. These arguments were based on development as “control and discipline”, “natural stages”, “experience” and “interaction”. I aim to discuss and evaluate these theories and their contributions to modern day practice in order to ascertain just how much development has been viewed as a social process. Firstly, English philosopher, Thomas Hobbes (1588-1679) adopted the nativist viewpoint that children were born “inherently sinful”, with a natural urge to be impulsive and reckless, believing that development should be controlled by “discipline and strict training”. This was a predominantly religious viewpoint where it was believed that obedience to God was achieved via obedience to the parents, thus resulting in the severe punishment of children in order to make them “good” and “obedient”. A letter sent by Susanna Wesley to advise her son John Wesley (founder of Methodism) in 1732 stated “Break their will betimes;…make him do as he is bid, if you whip him ten times running to effect it” thus maintaining this view. Nineteenth century evangelist and philanthropist, Hanna More upheld the nativist perspective by describing children as individuals with naturally “corrupt natures” and “evil dispositions” requiring correction via education. However, unlike Hobbes, she argued that education of children was better attained if children “were engaged in kindness”, (Smith. M. K, 2002), therefore offering small bribes as a method of control, rather than strict discipline. This demonstrates a shift in attitudes regarding affection for children and how they develop, with opinions also changing concerning the very controversial methods used to control, employed by Hobbes and Wesley, therefore supporting James and Prouts claim. French philosopher and nativist, Jean- Jacques Rousseau (1718-1778) believed children to be born inherently innocent, describing them as “noble savages” with a natural sense of right and wrong until corrupted by society, which he regarded as a negative influence on development. In his book Emile, or On Education (1762) he also acknowledged differences between children and adults, suggesting an “Age of Nature that lasts from birth to twelve years of age, when children’s natural innocence should be respected; they should be able to play and run all day”. Kindergarten movement pioneer, Friedrich Froebel, also supported “natural stages” theory, arguing that “The child, the boy, man, indeed, should know no other endeavor but to be at every stage of development wholly that this stage calls for…” (1885). Woodhead (1998) states that the scientific concept of development as natural stages still impacts today for recommendations and advice on “developmentally appropriate” procedures within early years education. Regardless of this, the fact that Rousseau’s recognised the negative impact of society on development suggests that he could have also foreseen that development could be a social process too. The English philosopher and physician, John Locke (1632-1704) in his “Essay Concerning Human Understanding” (1690), considered children to be born neither sinful nor innocent, but rather, a ‘tabula rasa’ (blank slate) with the capability to progress into mature citizens. This, according to Locke, was conditional on what nurture and environmental experiences a child received during development. Locke acknowledged personal astuteness and disposition, but believed that an individual’s knowledge came from experience via an interactive process with people and environments, and it was these experiences that fashioned the outcome of development for better or worse. He added that the provision of appropriate environments for the development to take place was a parent’s responsibility, arguing parents to be the best teachers to assist a child’s understanding in how to make sensible choices and decisions on reaching adulthood. Locke’s theories are still debated and applied to the failings in parenting and teaching today, with those influenced by him, insisting that the dominant aspect of development is experience. Locke’s Theory is categorized as empiricist, where all knowledge is dependent on experience, thus upholding child development to be a social process. German philosopher, Immanuel Kant (1724-1804), disagreed with the empiricist views of John Locke, despite agreeing that experience was a vital factor for learning. Kant believed children naturally possessed mental structures or categories of understanding, designed exclusively to decipher information from the environment, with the dual interaction between child and environment being the motivating force of development. Furthermore, he argued that children are neither passive recipient’s of sensory stimuli, nor passively following a predetermined, biological sequence. Instead, he claimed that children have the capacity to make their own choices, in addition to being able to select their own environments as they mature, which both consequentially, can affect how others respond to them, hereby supporting the two way process of interaction. In today’s society it is widely acknowledged that children are active agents in their own development, and Kant’s theory has become influential in researching this interaction between nature and nurture. As a result, this has set the tone for the ‘transactional models’ of development, where researchers are investigating the complex duality of “cause and effect” to which the development process is observed as neither exclusively natural nor exclusively social, but instead, a combination of both. The continuous evolution of the hypothesis of childhood in our culture is demonstrated by changes to government legislation, both currently and historically. For example, The Factory Act 1833 implied the beginnings of legal child protection, challenging working conditions for children and the exploitation of children in the work place, eventually contributing to a rise in schools for factory children to attend instead. Prior to this, it was customary for poorer families to encourage children from as young as five to contribute to the family income by either doing chores in the home or going out to work long hours either in factories or away from home as servants. Economic changes and trade union influences, also contributed to the worse forms of child labour being abolished by end of the nineteenth century, thus demonstrating that attitudes had shifted to childhood being a time for play and education rather than work and being “useful”. School eventually became compulsory as a result and has remained so to this day. Today, the emotional appeal of child rearing is appreciated far more than the economic benefits. Children’s physical, social and emotional progression is now subject to continuous monitoring and manipulation by parents, teachers and an assortment of other people who associate with, or advocate on behalf of children, resulting in future social and economic competencies being predicted by an assortment of tests and exams. Furthermore, implementation of various, frequently revised legislations, designed to protect or inform about children, demonstrates that concepts surrounding child development are frequently evolving and changing. This has resulted in a craving for information pertaining to both the natural and social processes of development, not only in Western societies, where much of the research has taken place, but also in the poorer, more rural countries around the world where child labour is still commonplace. The Six Cultures Project, initiated by Barbara and John Whiting in 1976 provided comprehensive details on children’s behaviour, work, play and social interactions, in addition to evidence of adult attitudes towards children in poorer countries. Their findings revealed differentiated customs and beliefs shaping children, therefore presenting a variety of different childhoods globally. Some beliefs emerged from nativist standpoints, such as children being deemed as vulnerable and weak dependents, lacking awareness and knowledge, whereas children being viewed as fundamental economic resources evolved from the social perspective. The Gusii tribe in Kenya who viewed children as economically essential was highlighted in the research, with observations showing an equal value placed on work and play as integral to development. The Development Niche Framework, presented by Super and Harkness (1986) to compare cross cultural development of communities within the USA and Kenya displayed similar findings. Concentrating on the physical and social settings of a child’s everyday life, culturally dominated traditions and child rearing procedures, and the psychology of the caregivers and others within the community, results showed the framework to be useful in demonstrating the treatment of, and the physical and social progression of individuals interacting within the cultural constraints of their own environments. For example, the seemingly “natural” process of motor development also has social objectives, dependent on the cultural initiatives displayed within the development niche. In Western society niches that omit the concept of work, children’s motor skills may develop playfully within the confines of a safe environment, therefore protecting them from any dangers. In poorer countries however, the same skills may be acquired by engaging in the same activities as the adults, exemplified by the Bangladeshi girl’s recollection, “When I was a child, I used to cry for the hammer. So my mother bought me a hammer and I started breaking bricks.” (Woodhead. 1998). Despite demonstrating how development may occur, this statement exhibits weakness in the “framework” for ascertaining what constitutes as work, play or learning in poorer communities. Furthermore, the dangerous nature of the work raises moral debate regarding child rights and protection. Nonetheless, Woodhead highlights the continuing importance of work, which in poorer cultures is viewed to enhance development, equipping children with valuable skills, thus enabling them to contribute within the family, the community and eventually to the overall economy, therefore verifying that social processes are contributory to development. To conclude, knowledge gained from observing natural processes of development has contributed to massive improvements in the care and treatment of children globally. Conversely, others argue that child development, until recently has been observed exclusively and selfishly from an adult perspective, without seeking the child’s opinion. The influence of the UNCRC has challenged some theories and presumptions regarding development to be ethically unacceptable, especially “control and discipline” theories which would now raise serious safeguarding concerns. Furthermore, “ages and stages” theories that do not take into consideration the child’s right to respect and consultation on issues concerning them have also been disregarded. Consequently, the UNCRC has now made child development research a global priority, promoting children’s rights and encouraging children to become active participants in their own development and the decisions that may affect it. This strengthens James and Prout’s claim of development being “constructed” and “reconstructed” by social processes. 2018 words. Sources: Woodhead M (2005). “Children and Development” in Oates, J, Wood, C and Grayson, A(eds). Psychological Development and Early Childhood, Oxford, Blackwell/The Open University. Smith, M. K. (2002) 'Hannah More: Sunday schools, education and youth work', the encyclopedia of informal education, http://www.infed.org/thinkers/more.htm.