Download Ecol Info Ms_revisions 2_srxd - Personal web pages

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Habitat conservation wikipedia , lookup

Ecological fitting wikipedia , lookup

Ecology wikipedia , lookup

Source–sink dynamics wikipedia , lookup

Soundscape ecology wikipedia , lookup

History of wildlife tracking technology wikipedia , lookup

Theoretical ecology wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Running Head: The Ecology of Information
THE ECOLOGY OF INFORMATION: AN OVERVIEW ON THE
ECOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE OF MAKING INFORMED DECISIONS
Kenneth A Schmidt1,*, Sasha R. X. Dall2, Jan A. van Gils3
1
Department of Biological Sciences, Texas Tech University, MS 3131, Lubbock, TX, 79424, USA
2
Center for Ecology & Conservation, School of Biosciences, University of Exeter, Tremough, Penryn,
TR10 9EZ, UK
3
Department of Marine Ecology (MEE), NIOZ Royal Netherlands Institute for Sea Research, P.O. Box
59, 1790 AB Den Burg, Texel, and Department of Plant-Animal Interactions (PAI), Centre for Limnology,
Netherlands Institute of Ecology (NIOO-KNAW), Rijksstraatweg 6, AC Nieuwersluis, The Netherlands
* Corresponding Author:
Kenneth Schmidt
[email protected]
806-742-2723
806-742-2693 FAX
1
1
ABSTRACT
2
Information is characterized as the reduction of uncertainty and by a change in the state of a receiving
3
organism. Thus, organisms can acquire information about their environment that reduces uncertainty and
4
increases their likelihood of choosing a best-matching strategy. We define the Ecology of Information as
5
the study of how organisms acquire and use information in decision-making and its significance for
6
populations, communities, landscapes, and ecosystems. As a whole, it encompasses the reception and
7
processing of information, decision-making, and the ecological consequences of making informed
8
decisions. The first two stages constitute the domains of, e.g., sensory ecology and behavioral ecology.
9
The exploration of the consequences of information use at larger spatial and temporal scales in ecology
10
has generally lagged behind the success of these other disciplines. In our overview we characterize
11
information, review statistical decision theory as a quantitative framework to analyze information and
12
decision-making, and discuss some potential ecological ramifications. Rather than attempt a superficial
13
review of the enormity of the scope of information we highlight information use in three areas: breeding
14
habitat selection, interceptive eavesdropping and alarm calls, and information webs. Through these topics
15
we discuss specific examples of ecological information use and the emerging ecological consequences.
16
We emphasize recurring themes: information is collected from multiple sources, over varying temporal
17
and spatial scales, and in many cases links heterospecifics to one another. This leads to questions where
18
further development is needed: (1) how information sources are integrated and prioritized, (2) how does
19
the spatial and temporal correlation between when and where information is obtained and acted upon
20
affect behavioral strategies, population processes, and ecological interactions, (3) how best to integrate
21
interaction and information webs between organisms.
22
23
Keywords: alarm calling, Bayesian updating, breeding habitat selection, eavesdropping, information,
24
predation risk, statistical decision theory
25
2
26
INTRODUCTION
27
The Ecology of Information is the study of how organisms acquire and use information in decision-
28
making to manage their lives of, e.g., finding food, selecting habitats, avoiding predators, and allocating
29
effort to current and future reproductive success, and its significance for populations, communities,
30
landscapes, and ecosystems. It is a burgeoning and integrative field that melds together the various
31
disciplines that deal with the reception and processing of information on the one hand and the ecological
32
and evolutionary consequences of making informed decisions on the other hand (Fig. 1). Information is
33
considered one of the central biological concepts of the twentieth century (Maynard Smith 2000,
34
Jablonska 2002) and is critical to the adaptive process (Plotkin 1997, Dall et al. 2005). The concept of
35
information (or its various subdivisions) as related to the fields of animal behavior and ecology has been
36
reviewed at least six times since 2004 (Danchin et al. 2004, Dall et al. 2005, Vos et al. 2006, Seppänen et
37
al. 2007, Bonnie and Earley 2007, and Valone 2007), and as a more general concept in biology by
38
Maynard-Smith (2000) and Jablonka (2002).
39
This set of excellent review and synthesis papers has exposed the concept of information to many
40
ecologists, and has taken the first important step of presenting, defining, and circumscribing the role(s) of
41
information in ecology as well as illustrating a diverse set of ecological contexts and organisms that
42
utilize information. They have been extremely successful in this regard; however, the limits of a single
43
review article leaves little room for exploring ecological processes or the relevance of information for
44
processes and patterns within populations, communities, and ecosystems – i.e., the spatial and temporal
45
scales that form the bulk of the ecological studies (Fig. 1). Thus, with a strong backdrop of information
46
in ecology already in place, there still remains a very real and urgent need to bridge the gap between the
47
behavioral, ecological, and conservation sciences (Fryxell and Lundberg 1998, Sutherland and Norris
48
2002) with information at its core. By way of this overview, we hope to highlight some areas of
49
successful incorporation of information as well as areas where further development is needed. However,
50
the scope of information in evolution and ecology is enormous and cannot be covered in any reasonable
51
manner in a single paper (or indeed a featured set of papers); likewise, the upsurge of interest as catalyst
3
52
for recent reviews means that a certain amount of redundancy is unavoidable. We have taken the strategy
53
of selecting a narrow set of topics to discuss the implications that are more relevant to populations,
54
communities, landscapes, and ecosystems that have not necessarily been the main subject material for
55
past reviews. But this also means we barely scratched the surface of the functional role of information in
56
ecology.
57
58
Characterizing information
59
An abstract property of events and entities that make their characteristics predictable to
60
individuals… [It] enables…individuals to make choices, select their activities …appropriately for
61
their needs and opportunities. (Smith 1977:193)
62
63
This passage of Smith (1977) expresses two elements that are typically used to characterize information:
64
(1) the reduction of uncertainty, i.e., information is that which makes the world more predictable
65
(syntactic view of information: Shannon and Weaver 1949, Danchin et al. 2004) and (2) change in the
66
state of a receiver in a functional way (semantic view: Blumstein and Bouskila 1996, Jablonka 2002; Dall
67
2005). Heterogeneity and variability limit an organism’s ability to possess complete knowledge of the
68
state of its current world or anticipate future conditions, and hence choose an appropriate strategy for the
69
actual set of conditions it will encounter. In the face of this uncertainty organisms can acquire
70
information about their physical and biotic environment (or future environment) that reduce uncertainty
71
and increase their likelihood of choosing a best-matching strategy. Implicit in this description is that for
72
information to exist and to have fitness consequences there must be both variation in environmental
73
conditions (e.g., states) and in phenotypic strategies (Stephens 1989). Moreover, it assumes organisms
74
make adaptive choices under the existence of constraints, a central premise of the field of behavioral
75
ecology (Mitchell and Valone 1990). While this last premise is demonstrably false in many cases, it is
76
often so because the genome or phenotypic plasticity fails to precisely track a changing environment (or
4
77
correctly perceive a constant environment). Ecological traps and information disruption are two
78
anthropogenic causes for these events that we discuss throughout the paper.
79
Terminology: Box 1 provides a glossary for types of information discussed throughout that is
80
based on and expanded by Wagner and Danchin (this issue). Information can come from almost any
81
source: an organism’s abiotic environment (e.g., physical cues) biotic environment (e.g., signals and
82
biological cues from con- and heterospecifics), perception of its own internal state (e.g., hunger,
83
motivation), social learning, and trial and error experience. Furthermore, even when sources of
84
information are private, such as state or motivation, information can become public through indirect
85
means following the pathway described by Seppänen et al. (2007; Box 1). For example, Wong et al.
86
(2005) demonstrated that sand fiddler crabs (Uca pugilator) use observations of threat-induced responses
87
of neighbors (actions, stage 2 in Seppänen et al. 2007; Box 1) to guide their own refuge-seeking behavior.
88
Breeding birds may preferentially settle on territories where conspecific reproductive success
89
(consequences, stage 3 in Seppänen et al. 2007; Box 1) was high in prior years. Public information based
90
on the actions or consequences of other individuals (i.e., social information; Box 1) may be actively
91
sought out by both conspecifics and heterospecifics. It is possible that the quality of information degrades
92
through this sequence (Giraldeau et al. 2002, van Bergen et al. 2004, Seppänen et al. 2007).
93
Alternatively, observations of the primary observer’s actions and consequences may provide higher
94
quality information (i.e., more reliable cues) since the primary observer confronts ambiguity (e.g., errors
95
in signal detection; Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1998) or uncertainty (Stephens 1989), and its actions may
96
be influenced by gambles (e.g., risk-sensitive behavior; Stephens and Krebs 1973) or seeking insurances
97
(Dall and Johnstone 2002) that secondary observers may wish to avoid.
98
99
Lastly, it is important to distinguish between the source of information and its content; our
terminology refers only to the former. It is entirely possible that a source of information that is social in
100
origin (e.g. the feeding performance of flockmates) can inform about non-social issues (e.g. the amount of
101
food in the environment). Identifying sources is important as, e.g., social information has unique
102
implications for ecology and evolution, such as cultural evolution (Danchin et al. 2004) or information
5
103
webs and the consequences of their deterioration (Vos et al. 2006, Holt 2007; Seppänen et al. this issue;
104
see Concluding Discussion). However, in other contexts the type of information can be safely ignored
105
relative to its content. In the later sections that discuss ecological processes we often do just that: focus
106
more on the content than the source of information.
107
108
Information, uncertainty, and phenotypic variation: Phenotypic variation is an outgrowth of
109
spatiotemporal variation in the environment and its predictability, i.e., information (Levins 1968,
110
Donaldson-Matasci et al. 2008, Lachman et al. this issue). This is especially true of phenotypic plasticity;
111
however the genome itself is a record of the past success of heritable strategies. Thus phenotypic
112
variation is influenced by (1) past information stored in the form of allelic (genetic) variation (2)
113
maternally-acquired cues (Massot and Clobert 2000, Mathis et al. 2008) capable of producing epigenetic
114
effects in one or more future generations (Gilbert and Epel 2008), and (3) current signals or cues that
115
produced variation through norm of reaction, polyphenism, or behavioral plasticity. A discussion of the
116
evolution of plasticity (and the related concept of bet-hedging as a response to uncertainty) would go far
117
beyond our overview. But we note that the form of plasticity will be influenced, in part, by the degree to
118
which the environment varies (spatiotemporal correlation) and the relative cost of incorrectly matching
119
your strategy to the environment. If change is slow (e.g., ponds tend be fishless or not during a
120
cladoceran’s lifetime) then irreversible developmental shifts (protective crest development) may be
121
favored over a sophisticated perceptual system that is costly to maintain. On the other hand, behavioral
122
flexibility may be favored if changes are rapid and frequent within an individual’s lifetime, e.g., nest
123
defense or broken wing display in response to predators that frequently move in and out of the vicinity of
124
a plover’s nest. We largely restrict our overview to behavioral plasticity. Behavior has attracted far
125
greater attention from the field of statistical decision theory (next section), and is perceived as a more
126
important proximate causal agent of higher level ecological processes. However, this perception may be
127
more of a statement about how the field of biology is fractionalized.
128
6
129
Statistical decision theory - In their review, Dall et al. (2005) promote statistical decision theory (SDT)
130
as a quantitative framework from which to analyze the use of information by organisms (also see
131
McNamara and Houston 1980, McNamara et al. 2006, and Oikos v.112, issue 2). At its heart is the use of
132
Bayesian methods (specifically Bayes’ theorem for calculating conditional probabilities) to explore how
133
organisms integrate prior expectations based on personal experience and evolutionary history (i.e., genetic
134
information) with new information to arrive at a revised, posterior expectation. Take the example of mate
135
choice: choosy females are assumed to have (perfect) knowledge of the distribution of male quality (e.g.,
136
parenting skills, parasite loads) in a population, whereas the quality of an individual male is uncertain and
137
must be sampled through observation of song, display, etc. (Getty 1996, Luttbeg 1996). The value of
138
sampling information lies in the formation of a revised posterior expectation of the individual male’s
139
quality that reflects reduced uncertainty associated with possible outcomes (Dall et al. 2005). We direct
140
our readers to reviews on the SDT framework (Dall et al. 2005) and empirical studies of Bayesian
141
behavior (Valone 2006; half the issue is in fact devoted to Bayesian foraging) for recent updates in this
142
field and it’s important to the ecology of information. We make reference to SDT throughout, but for
143
brevity we do not duplicate the material in these reviews.
144
Two salient questions not directly addressed in the reviews are: (1) what is the relationship
145
between information updating and information use and (2) how do alternatives to Bayesian updating
146
compare? To address the former, it has been suggested (to us) that SDT tackles only the question of how
147
information is updated and not how it is used (and thus its relevance for ecological processes). This may
148
be a reaction to how SDT has been used in the past, for the statement is certainly not true. For example,
149
van Gils (this issue) demonstrates that the Bayesian Potential Value Rule (Olsson and Brown 1996)
150
predicts the pattern of area-restricted search when foraging in a spatially correlated environment. For the
151
first time, we have a realistic theoretical representation of this behavior. Schmidt and Whelan (this issue)
152
used SDT to predict optimal renesting behavior in single-brooded birds. One prediction of their model
153
they called the Renester’s Paradox: habitats with greater nest failure that require more nest attempts, on
154
average, to successfully raise a brood are the very same that are selected for fewer nests attempted.
7
155
Furthermore, they show how uncertainty surrounding habitat quality and the process of information
156
updating links changes in the quality or proportion of one habitat type to behavior in the other habitat.
157
Kokko and Sutherland (2001) modeled breeding habitat selection combined with habitat
158
degradation without changing preferences (i.e., an ecological trap scenario). Notably, they demonstrated
159
that variation in how priors are governed can greatly alter the threat of extinction and place very different
160
demands on management or intervention. Their model incorporated rules-of-thumb (e.g., imprinting or
161
learned preferences) rather than take an explicit Bayesian approach. Behavioral rules may be common
162
(McNamara and Houston 1980, Bouskila and Blumstein 1992; see next section), and often perform close
163
to optimal Bayesian solutions when the two have been compared (e.g., Beauchamp 2000, Welton et al.
164
2003). However, this may not be true when the circumstances under which the rule evolved have
165
changed thus generating evolutionary traps (Schlaepfer et al. 2002; and see Conservation biology and
166
ecological traps).
167
There is little doubt that SDT (especially Bayesian approaches) need to be expanded beyond a
168
handful of contexts. To date, Bayesian methods have mostly found their way in “simple” short-term tasks
169
such as foraging or predation-avoidance (e.g., Rodríguez-Gironés and Vásquez 1997, Olsson and
170
Holmgren 2000, van Gils et al. 2003). But even here one may ask to what extent do solutions to problems
171
such as patch departure rules influence populations, communities, landscapes, and ecosystems? Olsson
172
and Brown (this issue) give us glimpse into the future by examining how information states (e.g.,
173
Bayesian vs. fixed-time foragers) sculpt the resource distribution in their environment in ways that may
174
promote or prevent species coexistence. Following Olsson and Brown’s lead in incorporating Bayesian
175
approaches and behavioral strategies in population and community models (e.g., Fryxell and Lundberg
176
1998, Sutherland and Norris 2002) will begin to close the gap between information updating and
177
ecological processes at higher scales.
178
179
From information to an Ecology of Information – That information use can have significant population
180
consequences is demonstrated in this section using a quintessential ecological question: population
8
181
persistence and the distribution and abundance of individuals (we focus only on persistence here). We
182
start with a model of metapopulation persistence in an information-free world (Bascompte et al. 2004).
183
We do not fault the authors for this; rather they developed an elegant and useful model to illustrate the
184
relationship between the persistence of a stochastic patchy population network and the number of habitat
185
patches connected through dispersal. Under the assumptions 1) that patches experience periods of density
186
independent population growth ( > 1; good years) or decline ( < 1; bad years) with equal probability, 2)
187
patches are decoupled with respect to temporal variability (i.e., good and bad years are assigned to
188
patches independently), and 3) patches are spatially coupled through even dispersal events, the geometric
189
growth rate of a population network, GEOM, composed of n patches can be approximated as the spatial-
190
arithmetic mean growth rate, ARITH, minus a sampling error for small n. If for any habit patch good and
191
bad years are equally likely and uncorrelated there is no information available to choose a patch.
192
However, if patch quality is temporally correlated (regardless if good and bad years remain equally likely
193
in the long term) then previous experience informs an individual of the likelihood the current conditions
194
will persist. Correlation makes information available; however the organism still requires the means of
195
detecting, processing, and using the information. Thus, even in a temporally correlated world even
196
dispersal is uninformed behavior. The result is that regardless of the number of patches (or the presence
197
of temporal correlation), GEOM will never exceed ARITH such that if ARITH < 1.0 the metapopulation
198
quickly becomes extinct (Fig. 2). An alternative is to adjust patch fidelity based on prior experience:
199
return to (or stay at) patches that experienced high productivity the prior year and vacate patches that had
200
poor productivity the prior year. This win-stay:lose-switch (WSLS) rule performs no better than even
201
dispersal in a world lacking temporal correlation and so prior experience is not informative. However, the
202
combination of the WSLS rule and temporal correlation (experience is informative) has dramatic
203
consequences on the persistence time of the metapopulation (Schmidt 2004; Fig. 2). While we detail this
204
single example, other excellent studies drive home the point that models of ecological processes built
205
around partially informed organisms often behave dramatically different from information-free or perfect
9
206
information scenarios (e.g., Brown et al. 1999, Vos et al. 2001, Donahue 2006), neither of which is
207
realistic or likely to be common.
208
209
INFORMATION AND ECOLOGICAL PROCESSES
210
In this section we expand on information use in three areas: (1) Ecological developmental biology briefly
211
considers environmental cues that direct phenotypic variation in morphology and physiology, (2)
212
Breeding habitat selection, which makes use of multiple sources of information collected over varying
213
temporal and spatial scales, and (3) Alarm calling and heterospecific information transfer within a
214
landscape context.
215
216
Ecological developmental biology – Some decisions in an organism’s life are made only once and are
217
irreversible. These include selecting among alternative developmental endpoints (polyphenism) and life-
218
cycle progressions timing (e.g., diapause, metamorphosis) that directly or indirectly affect morphology
219
and physiology in addition to behavior. These decisions are guided in part by environment cues (e.g.,
220
photoperiod, temperature, nutrition, predation risk, social proximity) and concern the emerging field of
221
ecological developmental biology (Gilbert 2001, Gilbert and Epel 2009). Examples include predator-
222
induced shell morphology in the barnacle Thais lamellosa (Palmer 1985), nutrition-induced polyphenism
223
in Nemoria arizonaria caterpillars that develop a cuticle resembling either an oak catkin or oak twig
224
depending on the season in which they hatch (Greene 1989), and vibrational assessment of predation risk
225
and premature hatching in embryo red-eyed tree frogs, Agalychnis callidryas (Warkentin et al. 2007); for
226
many other examples see reviews in (Pechenik et al. 1998, Gilbert 2001, Relyea 2007).
227
From an information perspective there may be little fundamental difference between these single,
228
irreversible decisions and rapidly repeatable and reversible behavioral decisions: the framework of SDT
229
can apply to either. For instance, Warkentin et al. (2007) couched their study of premature hatching in
230
red-eyed tree frogs as a signal detection problem: balancing the costs to frog embryos of missed cues
231
(snake predation on embryos) and false alarms (greater susceptibility of premature hatchlings to aquatic
10
232
predators. At the same time, developmental decisions have somewhat unique circumstances. In many
233
cases there is no direct assessment of the future environment (e.g., aquatic ↔ terrestrial) and thus limited
234
opportunity for learning. On the ecological side, there may be large latent effects stemming from
235
choosing an alternative fixed phenotype or altering developmental timing (Pechenik 2006). These effects
236
may capable of producing large population and community consequences. Lastly, global climate change
237
and other anthropogenic effects are rapidly altering the probabilistic linkages between (future) state and
238
proximate cue, and the reliability of chemical cues, which often direct development progression, is being
239
disrupted by anthropogenic substances.
240
241
Breeding habitat selection -Spatial heterogeneity and temporal variability in the underlying factors that
242
contribute to breeding productivity (e.g., food abundance, predation risk) are widespread (e.g., Lewis &
243
Murray 1993, Schmidt et al. 2006, Simpson et al. 2008). Choice of breeding location has high fitness
244
consequences, and it is not surprising that organisms acquire information to guide their decisions.
245
Although selecting a breeding location within a hierarchy of landscapes, habitats, territories, and breeding
246
sites (e.g., nest sites, dens) differs considerably among taxa, broadly speaking the potential sources of
247
information and their use are likely to generalize (Box 2). For instance, social information, based on
248
presence (conspecific attraction; Stamps 1988, Fletcher 2006) or performance (habitat copying; Clobert et
249
al. 2001) may be advantageous because they are integrative measures, provide greater sampling power
250
(i.e., more independent sources), and, in the case of post-reproductive cues may reveal the consequences
251
of conspecifics’ decisions. Thus, prospecting (i.e., gathering local information on, e.g., reproductive
252
success; Reed et al. 1999, Ward 2005) is likely to be an important behavioral strategy that is implemented
253
throughout the year or at least those critical periods when information is least costly and most readily
254
available. Recent reviews have highlighted the apparent ubiquity of prospecting in birds where it has
255
been best studied (Reed et al. 1999). Among vertebrates, studies have shown they use conspecifics
256
presence, density (Cote and Clobert 2007), and reproductive productivity or its correlates (e.g., post-
257
breeding singing rates, Betts et al. 2008; quality/quantity of fledglings produced, Doligez et al. 2002,
11
258
Parejo et a. 2007) when choosing breeding sites or to find suitable habitat using phonotoaxis and
259
orientation (Diego-Rassila 2004). These cues may provide high quality information on habitat or site
260
quality and may be used to reduce search and settlement costs (Stamps 2001, Fletcher 2006). In addition,
261
proximate cues, e.g., presence versus density, may provide unique or complementary information on
262
different components of habitat (e.g., suitability versus intraspecific competition). Moreover, these
263
strategies are not limited to cues from conspecifics; cues from heterospecifics may also be used for these
264
same or alternative types of information or as cost saving strategies (Mönkkönen and Forsman 2002,
265
Diego-Rassila et al. 2004, Seppänen et al. 2007). Prospecting may also include assessment of habitat
266
components, such as predator activity and food availability, and breeders may eavesdrop on inadvertent
267
public cues, such as vocalizations of predators (Emmering and Schmidt in review) prey, (Simpson et al.
268
2008), or competitors (Fletcher 2008).
269
Adaptive information use depends on the level of spatial and temporal correlation which places
270
bounds on the quality or amount of information available (Doligez et al. 2003, Schmidt 2004, Donahue
271
2006; see Box 2). High quality information sources will vary widely with spatiotemporal correlation
272
relative to the timing of prospecting and constraints that limit it. For example, in the kittiwake (Rissa
273
tridactyla) Boulinier et al. (1996) observed a ~30 day window over which the proportion of successful
274
nests at a given date reflects the productivity of a breeding patch, and a peak in the number of prospectors
275
in this window. Betts et al. (2008) provide example of a window of opportunity, but with declining
276
reliability over time, to use post-breeding singing as a cue to reproductive productivity (also see Fletcher
277
and Miller (2008) for timing of social information in the cactus bug (Chelinidea vittiger)). Further
278
documentation of these relationships is important because it establishes the relationship between cue and
279
consequence, quantifies a level of cue reliability, and may document the existence of spatial and temporal
280
constraints on prospecting and information use.
281
The multitude of putative information sources within and among perceptual modalities (visual
282
auditory, chemical), ecologies (hetero- and conspecifics), spatial (personal versus public) and temporal
283
(prior and versus current) domains presents ecologists with challenges and opportunities. How are these
12
284
inputs combined? Are they redundant, complementary, or reinforcing? If data from two or more sources
285
contradict, which should take priority to maximize information acquisition? Only a handful of empirical
286
studies have examined how multiple measures of breeding success combine. In both kittiwakes (Danchin
287
et al. 1998) and collared flycatchers (Ficedula albicollis; Doligez et al. 2002) individuals differ in their
288
relative use of personal and patch (social) breeding success, revealing contextual sources of information
289
(prior success, age, sex difference) or possibly phenotypic differences among individuals (e.g., behavioral
290
syndromes). More such studies are desperately needed as well as experimental manipulations designed to
291
produce conflicting information as have been applied to foraging contexts (Kendal et al. 2004, van
292
Bergen et al. 2004, Coolen et al. 2005). In concert with this, we need further theoretical development that
293
incorporates the multitude of putative information sources seen in empirical studies, and under varying
294
scenarios of spatial and temporal predictability.
295
Ecological implications: population dynamics: Personal or public information on breeding
296
productivity may provide the information that leads to site dependent (SD) regulation, a potentially
297
widespread form of density dependence produced by the pattern in site (e.g., territory) settlement in
298
spatially heterogeneous environments (Pulliam and Danielson 1991, Rodenhouse et al.1997).
299
Information from prior success (WSLS-rule) can lead to prolonged persistence of metapopulations within
300
patchy landscapes (Fig. 2), whereas information from social cues (e.g., conspecific attraction) can deter
301
dispersal to new, high quality habitats (Ray et al. 1991, Forbes and Kaiser 1994). Nonetheless, SD
302
models assume perfect information and are phenomenological, whereas the WSLS-rule assumes prior
303
success trumps all other sources of information and is applied absolutely. A more reasonable alternative
304
is to make the WSLS rule probabilistic and conditional on context and other sources of information (e.g.,
305
Boulinier et al. 2008). Site dependent models, on the other hand, should become more mechanistic – in
306
the absence of mechanism they have no connection to the proximate source of information or its
307
spatiotemporal context, which limits their predictive power and insight into changing environments
308
(Sutherland and Norris 2002) or conservation strategies. For instance, understanding of whether a target
309
species uses information from con- or heterospecifics and the cues they use could benefit restoration of
13
310
locally extinct populations and suggest strategies (e.g., staggered release over several years) and numbers
311
of animals for reintroduction (Mihoub et al. 2009).
312
Community ecology: Species differences in the acquisition or use of information is evident when
313
comparing the few studies that have examined simultaneous responses of multiple species to manipulated
314
information pertinent to breeding habitat (or site) selection indicate (Nocera et al. 2006, Emmering and
315
Schmidt in review; also a comparison of Parejo et al. 2007 and Doligez et al. 2002). Likewise, the
316
identical cue may vary among species in its reliability, i.e., ability to forecast future reproductive success,
317
due to the interspecific variation in spatiotemporal correlation (Parejo et al. 2005, Schmidt et al. 2006).
318
Informed individuals have greater fitness than uninformed (see papers in this feature by van Gils, Olsson
319
and Brown, and McNamara and Dall), hence those species better at assessing information or whose
320
fitness is more closely linked to temporally and spatially correlated environmental parameters may (1)
321
have a competitive advantage (Olsson and Brown, this issue), (2) ameliorate rapid ecological change, but
322
also (3) be more susceptible to ecological traps (Nocera et al. 2006). Broadening the ecological context
323
beyond breeding habitat selection, acquiring information may tradeoff with other fitness enhancing
324
activities (e.g., Dukas 2002, Schmidt et al. 2008) and could lead to mechanisms of coexistence (Olsson
325
and Brown, this issue). Coexistence or competitive displacement mediated through information may
326
complement purely performance-based mechanisms (e.g., Vincent et al. 1996), and are ripe for ecological
327
investigation. Lastly, heterospecific ‘informants’ may play a role in community assembly (e.g., Elmberg
328
et al. 1997, Mönkkönen et al. 1990, Fletcher 2008) when their presence provides performance or
329
productivity-based information to other species. Migrants may especially rely on the presence of
330
residents to gauge habitat quality (Mönkkönen and Forsman 2002, Thomson et al. 2003, Forsman et al.
331
2008), and as shown by Fletcher (2008) experimental vocal cues alone were sufficient to generate
332
differences in community structure.
333
Conservation biology and ecological traps: Ecological traps are defined as the result of
334
anthropogenic processes that decouple a formerly reliable cue from habitat quality resulting in
335
maladaptive habitat choice (Schlaepfer et al. 2002, Robertson and Hutto 2006). For instance, by
14
336
orientating toward polarized light, odonates mistake asphalt surfaces, a cue ‘mimic’, for ponds and lay
337
their eggs on an unsuitable surface (Kriska et al. 1998). Statistical decision theory (Bradbury and
338
Vehrencamp 1998) provides a useful framework for examining an organism’s decision after receiving a
339
cue (or signal) based on (1) the correlation between cue and state (2) the fitness consequences of its
340
decision in those states (i.e., value of information), and (3) the commonness of the states (in Bayesian
341
terms, the animal’s prior). Under these considerations, an organism has an optimal cutoff probability
342
(i.e., minimizes the ratio of errors to correct choices weighed by their fitness value) that favors alternative
343
actions (select habitat A or B) on opposite sides of the cutoff. SDT has not been used in the context of
344
examining ecological traps, perhaps because it oversimplifies habitat selection, e.g., ignoring density
345
dependence. But SDT may be valuable because it illustrates unique pathways to a trap (1- 3 above).
346
Alternatively, adaptive behavior (the organism’s cutoff is optimal given the information available) can
347
lead to an increase in the proportion of individuals settling in the poorer habitat under any of the three
348
paths. But without an understanding of the decision process these would be incorrectly labeled as
349
ecological traps. Moreover, we do not expect to see an evolutionary response and conservation
350
management may be required.
351
Organisms may alternatively based habitat choice or settlement on behavioral rules-of-thumb
352
where priors, for instance, are based on learning. These rules may be more flexible and result in fewer
353
incorrect settlement decisions as shown by a theoretical analysis of Kokko and Sutherland (2001). When
354
priors were based on natal imprinting (being born is informative - philopatric preference strategy in
355
Kokko and Sutherland 2001) or the WSLS rule (learned preference strategy in Kokko and Sutherland
356
2001), individuals adjusted their habitat preferences to reduce the impact of ecological traps (modeled as
357
reduced quality of preferred habitat). Habitat preferences changed most rapidly under imprinting, but the
358
rate of preference change under WSLS still out paced the change of genetically fixed habitat preferences
359
when genetic variation was low. These analyses suggest that if cues are learned, then even if the
360
reliability of a cue is compromised organisms may rapidly readjust their cutoff threshold (Kokko and
361
Sutherland 2001), whereas if the cue use has a strong genetic component (Kriska et al. 1998) traps may
15
362
persist. It is interesting that the WSLS rule may create a trap when changes in the reliability of cues
363
occurs (WSLS base on qualitative nest success; Schmidt 2001), but also rescue a population when traps
364
were created through a decrease in the quality of preferred habitat (Kokko and Sutherland 2001). The
365
lessons here are (1) depending on the ecological context the use of cues may both initiate and rescue a
366
population from an ecological trap and (2) how priors are formed can be the difference between requiring
367
management to save population or not.
368
369
Alarm calling in a community and landscape context – Alarm signals are a ubiquitous, largely public
370
strategy of informing others (intentionally or as a by-product) of dangers (most often predation risk) in the
371
environment (reviewed in Caro 2005). Regardless of how the interaction is characterized (i.e., altruistic
372
or selfish), signals that carry information about danger or predator presence confer an advantage to
373
potential prey within perceptual range. We focus on heterospecific receivers and consider two ecological
374
implications: First eavesdropping on alarm calls to manage activity in time and space and avoid predators
375
may be common and of significant value (survival and foraging efficiency). Short-term benefits include
376
reacting with an appropriate anti-predator behavior and adjusting time allocation to scanning for predators
377
or to safer activities. It is difficult to extrapolate ecological consequences from short-term benefits, so we
378
consider the topic from the perspective of the presence of heterospecific alarm callers. Second, the
379
presence some …..of alter landscape connectivity and the resistance of habitat elements (e.g., habitat
380
edges) to facilitate movement through and within landscapes.
381
Alarm calls, eavesdropping, and predation risk: Birds in the Family Paridae (Parus, Baeolophus,
382
Poecile) are known to have high vigilance, aggressive mobbing behavior, and a sophisticated alarm
383
communication (Templeton et al. 2005) that extends to a large heterospecific audience (Langham et al.
384
2006). In the presence of black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla), downy woodpeckers (Picoides
385
pubescens) decrease vigilance by 70% thereby increasing foraging rates (Sullivan 1984; also see Telleria
386
et al. 2001 for similar patterns among blue and great tits). Likewise, white-breasted nuthatches (Sitta
387
carolinensis) visit food patches more frequently in the presence of titmice (Dolby and Grubb 2000).
16
388
Quantifying long-term advantages are difficult and far less common, but potentially far-reaching. For
389
example, Dolby and Grubb (1998) demonstrated long-term consequences for nuthatches occupying forest
390
fragments in which parids were removed for the winter, i.e., the time of year when they lead mixed-
391
species flocks. Both energetic state and survivorship declined (all mortality events were in parid-removal
392
fragments), although mortality events were rare and the difference was not significant. A similar
393
exclusion experiment, but during the breeding season, by Forsman et al. (2002) demonstrated decreased
394
reproductive productivity in pied flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) in the absence of parids. These studies
395
minimally demonstrate that there is an effect of the presence of informants that has fitness consequences,
396
ostensibly through the production of alarm calls. However, it may be more likely that it is that the
397
absence of alarm signals and presence of non-alarm vocalizations that indicates safety (e.g., Sullivan
398
1984, Moller 1992), thereby reducing stress, increasing foraging efficiency, and avoiding unnecessary
399
activity and energetic expenditure (Vitousek et al. 2007). The latest study by Hetrick et al. (this issue-a)
400
found that changes in the performance and structure of alarm calls in the Eastern Tufted Titmouse (which
401
reference predator type and magnitude of risk; also see Templeton et al. 2005) are mirrored by changes in
402
contact calls. This suggests that alarm calls themselves are not necessary to communicate perceived
403
predation risk. Certainly more research is needed to determine how fitness benefits arise in these species.
404
Predation, information, and landscape connectivity: As the preceding section suggests,
405
perception of predation risk is modified by the availability of information, such as publicly broadcast
406
alarm signals or contact calls (and also other behavioral acts such as looking upward or fleeing or moving
407
toward cover; e.g., Wong et al. 2005). It stands to reason that this information combines with the physical
408
environment, perceptual aptitudes of the organism, and the costs and benefits associated with decision-
409
making to influence (facilitate or impede) movement among resource or habitat patches (Taylor et al.
410
1993); that is, to influence the functional connectivity of landscapes (Lima and Zollner 1996, Bélisle
411
2005). Information may influence the resistance of some organisms to cross patches boundaries
412
(Desrochers and Fortin 2000, Sieving et al. 2004, Tubelis et al. 2006) and habitat gaps (Bélisle and
413
Desrochers 2002), variation that has been linked to patterns of extinction in birds (Moore et al. 2008).
17
414
Sieving et al. (2004) observed greater frequency of boundary patch crossings among songbirds when in
415
the presence of titmice. Tubelis et al. (2006) observed greater use of adjacent savannah habitat by mixed-
416
species flocks that form around sentinel species in relation to the level of predation risk (Ragusa-Netto
417
2002). Lastly, Wolters and Zuberbühler (2003) observed greater travel and broadening of niche space
418
(increased use of mid-canopy layer) in associating Campbell’s and Diana monkeys relative to isolated
419
species groups. These studies suggest the presence or absence of risk-base information may be akin to
420
landscape models that vary the quality of the matrix (e.g., Fahrig 2007). The effects may be especially
421
important in selecting migratory stopover sites (Nocera et al. 2008) where, because of lack of experience,
422
organisms are more vulnerable and less accurate at estimating predation risk (Pomeroy 2006, Pomeroy et
423
al. 2006, van den Hout et al. 2008).
424
As a consequence of the value of social information regarding predation risk, interspecific
425
sociality – from ‘loose’ attraction among heterospecifics to stable polyspecific associations - may be
426
attributed as much to the value of informants as to other ecological variables (Terborgh 1990, Goodale
427
and Kotagama 2005); however, demonstrating an effect of information per se, rather than simply selfish
428
herd, confusion, or dilution effects is difficult. Nonetheless, mixed-species groups of (most notably) birds
429
and monkeys form around specific nuclear sentinel species that signal a predator’s presence more often
430
and more reliably than others (Gaddis 1983, Bshary and Noë 1997, Goodale and Kotagama 2005)
431
suggesting that non-informational effects are insufficient to explain the phenomena (Wolters and
432
Zuberbühler 2003). Within these mixed-species groups, the risk-based information from sentinel species
433
extends to wider audiences (Goodale and Kotagama 2005, Langham et al. 2007) and leads to fitness
434
consequences (Dolby and Grubb 1998, Forsman et al. 2002) prompting their recognition as keystone
435
signalers (Hetrick et al. this issue-b). Moreover, an exchange of risk-based information in these systems
436
may represent an example of a stable resource exchange mutualism between species (Schwartz and
437
Hoeksema 1998).
438
439
CONCLUDING DISCUSSION
18
440
In this section we break away from featuring specific ecological contexts to discuss some implications of
441
information at larger ecological scales and as a central organizing principle. Because little research has
442
been conducted at these levels to date our discussion is necessarily brief and more conjectural.
443
Nonetheless, in advancing our case we highlight the need to understand the possible consequences of
444
information at these scales.
445
Information Webs: Breeding habitat selection and alarm calling are themes of the larger
446
phenomena of using proximate cues or alternative sources of information to locate areas of high resource
447
abundance and low mortality risk; decisions which often dominate the daily lives of individuals and the
448
ecological interactions among organisms (Stephens et al. 2007). The responses affect the strength of
449
species interactions (Vos et al. 2006), generate non-lethal effects of predation (Brown et al. 1999), and
450
when cues are from heterospecifics, generate trait-mediated indirect interactions (Peacor 2003). It should
451
be evident therefore that there exists an information web that complements and greatly increases the
452
complexity of food webs and interaction webs (Dicke and Vet 1999, Vos et al. 2006, Holt 2007). Of what
453
consequence then is this information for food or interaction webs, i.e., beyond an individuals’ (or
454
strategy’s) own fitness? Predator-prey models suggest that adaptive behavior tends to destabilize
455
predator-prey dynamics (i.e., simple food-web modules) unless it is based on imperfect information
456
(Brown et al. 1999, Luttbeg and Schmitz 2000). Of what consequence is food web structure for
457
information? We are not aware that this question has been properly framed before. Vos et al.’s (2001)
458
work on infochemical mimicry (increasing noise) and confusion effects in tri-trophic interactions shows
459
that information can be a decreasing function of species diversity, specifically host specialization. In high
460
diversity systems infochemicals produced from herbivore-damaged leaves attracted parasitoids to plants
461
containing many individuals of non-host species. In theoretical analyses, effects in high diversity systems
462
weaken species interactions and lead to stabilized dynamics at intermediate species richness (Vos et al.
463
2001). Hence, community structure affects information that feedbacks to lower diversity.
464
Information as a third niche axis: We contend that information share center stage with abiotic
465
conditions and biotic resources as a third set of niche axes. To take an example, sunlight is an abiotic
19
466
factor that is converted through photosynthesis and metabolic processes into a biotic resource for
467
heterotrophs. Sunlight also produces warmth and light (by definition) for activities such as foraging.
468
However, imagine a world where the daily mean number of hours of sunlight was equivalent to Earth’s
469
but randomly distributed throughout the year. Outside of H2S reduction as a source of energy, would life
470
even be possible? Perhaps, we but suggest it is the pattern (information) of sunlight in the form of
471
circadian rhythm that makes ecological systems what they are today. In a recent review, Resco et al.
472
(2009) discuss the ecological implications of plants’ ability to tell time noting that “[T]he circadian clock
473
affects gas exchange by ‘anticipating’ cycles of dawn and dusk” (Resco et al. 2009: 4; the anticipation
474
hypothesis). For instance, mutant, arrhythmic Arabidopsis show a 40% decrease in net carbon fixation
475
compared to wild-type (Dodd et al. 2005). Depending on the organism, sunlight is a consumable resource
476
or an abiotic condition; circadian rhythm is information.
477
Circadian rhythm of sunlight presents an extreme example, so consider something more mundane
478
but still exciting to most ecologists: species interactions. Predators kill or exert non-lethal effects such as
479
fear on their prey, but predators may also produce inadvertent social cues to their location in time and
480
space (i.e., information). Such information can enable prey to find spatial or temporal refugia from
481
predators; it is no wonder that predators (and prey) may behave with ‘purposeful unpredictability’ (sensu
482
Roth and Lima 2007; also see Gripenberg et al. 2007). Within the framework of consumer-resource
483
interactions, prey that acquire information to use to avoid predation can sustain zero-net population
484
growth at a higher density of predators, i.e., higher P* (Holt 1994). This in turn increases predators’ R*
485
(minimum prey abundance to achieve zero-net population growth) since informed prey are more difficult
486
to capture (Brown et al. 1999). The process can also start the other way around: information about prey
487
lowers a predator’s R*. If any of these feedback loops lowers, say the predator’s population density, there
488
may be reduced value to acquire information by the prey and their vulnerability goes up. The idea that
489
limiting factors (R* and P*, resources and predator, respectively) influence coexistence places
490
information as a critical element influencing community structure through population processes such as
20
491
competitive exclusion (Tilman 1980, Holt et al. 1994); a process itself considered as a fundamental
492
property of the ecological niche (Leibold 1995).
493
Information as an Ecosystem Process: In the preceding section we pointed out that arrhythmic
494
Arabidopsis shows a decrease in net carbon fixation. Resco et al. (2009) would have us scale up these
495
effects suggesting that the circadian clock in plants drives gas exchange at the level of biosphere-
496
atmosphere interactions. If we accept this premise, if only to explore the consequences, then (1) Vos et
497
al. (2001) demonstrates that information can drive (in non-linear fashion) diversity and (2) Resco et al.
498
(2009) suggests information can drive ecosystem process. The point we wish to make is that ecologists
499
currently take a fairly rigid casual interpretation of ecosystem process-diversity relationships: diversity
500
drives the former. Yet these ignore the potential role of information as direct and indirect (e.g.,
501
information  diversity  ecosystem process) driver.
502
Noise and Info-disruption: Modern anthropogenic processes are greatly accelerating the loss of
503
species richness and diversity (Pimm et al. 2006, Bradshaw et al. 2009). Less appreciated is the loss of
504
information, including information processing (i.e., disruption) and transmission (e.g., low urban signal-
505
to-noise ratios; Rabin et al. 2006, Slabbekoorn and Ripmeester 2008). For example, info-disruption
506
(Lürling and Scheffer 2007) is the disturbance to chemical information transfer caused by pollutants, such
507
as heavy metals, surfactants, and pesticides. [Info-disruption is analogous to endocrine-disruption, which
508
itself is essentially a signal detection problem at the level of chemical recognition]. In aquatic systems
509
these substances are known to negatively affect anti-predator responses to chemical alarm signals in fish
510
(impaired avoidance), algae (reduced protective colony formation), and cladocerans (inhibited protective
511
crest development); see Lürling and Scheffer (2007). The noise associated with wind turbines interferes
512
with acoustic alarm calls among California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi) and subsequently
513
affects vigilance patterns and flight to burrow behavior (Rabin et al. 2006). Urban noise similarly affects
514
song (signal) efficiency in birds in turn affecting foraging-vigilance tradeoffs (Quinn et al. 2006) as well
515
as lowered abundance and reproductive success near highways (Slabbekoorn and Ripmeester 2008).
516
Species that communicate at frequencies above urban noise are little affected, and may come to dominate
21
517
urban communities leading to faunal homogenization (Slabbekoorn and Ripmeester 2008). Invasive
518
species can also alter information flows, such as in the acoustic-orienting parasitoid fly (Ormia ochracea)
519
selecting for a song-less morph of the field cricket (Teleogryllus oceanicus); see Zuk et al. (2006). Lastly,
520
phenological mismatching due to global climate change is a consequence of information disruption that
521
has received considerable attention (e.g., Both et al. 2009, Brooks 2009). Mismatching occurs when the
522
timing of developmental or behavioral processes, such as hibernation, migration, or reproduction, is
523
altered triggered directly (temperature) or indirect from climatic cues (e.g., flowering phenology)
524
Examples such as these are unfortunately too common, and the prospect of deteriorating
525
information webs led Holt (2007) to ask whether this was the “next depressing frontier in conservation?”
526
The sum of these effects can be large indeed and need prompt attention by conservation biologists. Not
527
just for conserving species but also the preservation of animal cultures (Laiolo and Tella 2007). We
528
recognize the value of genetic information by preserving genomes; it is time now to expand this
529
conservation priority to non-genetic biological information.
530
531
Conclusions: With one or more major reviews published each year since 2000 (see Introduction) one
532
may ask whether information is a passing fad or a general theme (we’re not arguing the only theme)
533
around which to organize empirical and theoretical research and conservation priorities in ecology and
534
evolution. We believe the latter, but the answer will only come with further development of an Ecology
535
of Information Framework, one that integrates all the sub-disciplines in Figure 1. Through our overview
536
and the papers that follow in this special feature on the Ecology of Information, we hope to have …….
537
538
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS (566)
539
KAS’s research in the ecology of information is supported by a grant from the National Science
540
Foundation (DEB 0746985). JAvG was supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific
541
Research (NWO) and by the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences (KNAW). This is
542
publication xxxx of the Netherlands Institute of Ecology (NIOO-KNAW) and xxx of the Centre for
22
543
Wetland Ecology. This paper was improved by comments of Luc-Alain Giraldeau and an anonymous
544
reviewer. Lastly, we are grateful to Per Lundberg and Linus Svensson for their support of this special
545
feature.
546
547
23
548
LITERATURE CITED
549
550
Abbott, K. R. 2006. Bumblebees avoid flowers containing evidence of past predation events. – Can. J.
Zool. 84:1240-1247.
551
552
Andruskiw, M., Fryxell, J. M., Thompson, I. D. and Baker, J. A. 2008. Habitat-mediated variation in
predation rate by the American marten. – Ecology 89:2273-2280.
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
Bascompte, J., Possingham, H. and Roughgarden, J. 2002. Patchy populations in stochastic
environments: Critical number of patches for persistence. – Am. Nat. 159:128-137.
Beauchamp, G. 2000. Learning rules for social foragers: implications for the producer-scrounger game
and ideal free distribution theory. – J. Theor. Biol. 207:21-35.
Bélisle, M. 2005. Measuring landscape connectivity: the challenge of behavioral landscape ecology. –
Ecology 86:1988-1995.
Bélisle, M. and Desrochers, A. 2002. Gap-crossing decisions by forest birds: an empirical basis for
parameterizing spatially-explicit, individual-based models. - Landscape Ecol. 17:219-231.
Betts, M. G., Hadley, A. S., Rodenhouse, N. and Nocera, J. J. 2008. Social information trumps
vegetation structure in breeding-site selection y a migrant songbird. - Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 275: 22572263.
Blumstein, D. T. and Bouskila, A. 1996. Assessment and decision-making in animals: a mechanistic
model underlying behavioral flexibility can prevent ambiguity. - Oikos 77:569-577.
Bonnie, K. E. and Earley, R.L. 2007. Expanding the scope for social information use. - Anim. Behav.
74: 171-181.
Boulinier, T., Danchin, E. Monnat, K.-I., Doutrelant, C. and Cadio, B. 1996. Timing of prospecting and
the value of information in a colonial breeding bird. - J. Avian Biol. 27:252-256.
Boulinier, T., McCoy, K. D., Yoccoz, N. G., Gasparini, J. and Tveraa, T. 2008. Public information
affects breeding dispersal in a colonial bird: kittiwakes cue on neighbors. - Biol. Lett. 5:538-540.
Bouskila, A. and Blumstein, D. T. 1992. Rules of thumb for predation hazard assessment: predictions
from a dynamic model. - Am. Nat. 139:161-176.
Bradbury, J. W. and Vehrencamp, S. L. 1998. Principles of animal communication. - Sinauer Associates,
Inc, Sunderland, MA, USA.
Bradshaw, C. J. A., Sodhi, N. S. and Brook, B. W. 2009. Tropical turmoil: a biodiversity tragedy in
progress. – Frontiers in Ecol and the Environ. 7:79-87.
24
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
Brown, J. S. 1988. Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition. –
Behav. Ecol. Sociobio. 22:37-47.
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
Caro, T. 2005. Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. - University of Chicago Press.
Brown, J. S. 1992. Patch use under predation risk I: models and predictions. – Ann. Zoo. Fenn. 29:301309.
Brown, J. S., Laundré, J. W. and Gurung, M. 1999. The ecology of fear: optimal foraging, game theory,
and trophic interactions. - J. Mamm. 80:385-399.
Bshary, R., and Noë, R. 1997. Red colobus and Diana monkeys provide mutual protection against
predators. – Anim. Behav. 54:1461-1474.
Coolen, I., Ward, A. J. W., Hart, P. J. B. and Laland, K. N. 2005. Foraging nine-spined sticklebacks
prefer to rely on public information over simpler social cues. Behav. Ecol. 16:865-870.
Dall, S. R. X. 2005. Defining the concept of public information. – Science 308:353-354.
Dall, S. R. X. and Johnstone, R. A. 2002. Managing uncertainty: information and insurance under the
risk of starvation. – Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B 357:1519-1526.
Dall, S. R. X., Giraldeau, L. A., Olsson, O., McNamara, J. M. and Stephens, D. W. 2005. Information
and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. - TREE 20:187-19.
Danchin, E., Giraldeau, L. A., Valone, T. J. and Wagner, R. H. 2004. Public information: From nosy
neighbors to cultural evolution. - Science 305:487-491.
Danchin, E., Boulinier, T. and Massot, M. 1998. Habitat selection based on conspecific reproductive
success: implications for the evolution of coloniality. – Ecology
79:2415-2428.
Dicke, M. and Vet, L. E. M. 1999. Plant-carnivore interactions: evolutionary and ecological
consequences for plant, herbivore and carnivore. - In: Olff, H., Brown, V. K. and Drent, R. H. (eds.),
Herbivores: between plants an predators. Blackwell Science, Oxford, pp. 483-520.
Diego-Rassila, F. J. and Luengo, R. M. 2004. Heterospecific call recognition and phonotaxis in the
orientation of the marbled newt, Triturus marmoratus. – Behav. Ecol. Sociobio. 55:556-560.
Dodd, A. N., Salathis, N., Hall, A. Kevei, E. Toth, R. Nage, F., Hibberd, J. M., Millar, A. J and Webb, A.
R. R. 2005. Plant circadian clocks increase photosynthesis, growth, survival, and competitive advantage.
- Science 309:630-633.
Dolby, A. S. and Grubb, T. C., Jr. 1998. Benefit to satellite members in mixed-species foraging groups:
an experimental analysis. – Anim. Behav. 56:501-509.
25
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
Dolby, A. S. and Grubb, T. C., Jr. 2000. Social context affects risk taking by a satellite species in a
mixed-species foraging group. - Behav. Ecol. 11:110-114.
Donahue, M. J. 2006. Allee effects and conspecific cueing jointly lead to conspecific attraction. –
Oecologia 149:33-43.
Donaldson-Matasci, M. C., Lachmann, M. and Bergstrom, C. T. 2008. Phenotypic diversity as an
adaptation to environmental uncertainty. – Evol. Ecol. Res. 10:493-515.
Dukas, R. 2002. Behavioural and ecological consequences of limited attention. - Phil. Trans. R. Soc.
Lond. B 357:1539-1547.
Elmberg, J., Pöysä, H., Sjöberg, K. and Nummi, P. 1997. Interspecific interaction and co-existence in
dabbling ducks: observations and an experiment. – Oecologia 111:129-136.
Fahrig, L. 2007. Non-optimal animal movement in human-altered landscapes. – Funct. Ecol. 21:10031015.
Fletcher, R.T., Jr. 2007. Species interactions and population density mediate the use of social cues for
habitat selection. - J. Anim Ecol. 76:598-606.
Fletcher, R.T., Jr. 2008. Social information and community dynamics: nontarget effects from simulating
social cues for management. – Ecol. Appl. In press.
Fletcher, R.T., Jr. 2006. Emergent properties of conspecific attraction in fragmented landscapes. – Am.
Nat. 168:297-219.
Fletcher, R.T., Jr, and Miller, C. W. 2008. The type and timing of social information alters offspring
production. - Biol. Lett. 4: 482-485.
Forbers, L. S. and Kaiser, G. W. 1994. Habitat choice in breeding seabirds: when to cross the
information barrier. – Oikos 70:377-384.
Forsman, J. T., Hjernquist, M. B., Taipale, J. and Gustafsson, L. 2008. Competitor density cues for
habitat quality facilitating habitat selection and investment decisions. – Behav. Ecol. 19:539-545.
Forsman, J. T., Seppänen, J.-T. and Mönkkönen, M. 2002. Positive fitness consequences of interspecific
interaction with a potential competitor. – Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269:1619-1623.
Fryxell, J. M. and Lundberg, P. 1998. Individual behavior and community dynamics. - Chapman and
Hall.
Gaddis, P. K. 1983. Differential usage of song types by plain, bridled, and tufted titmouse. – Ornis
Scand. 14:16-23.
26
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
Getty, T. 1996. Mate selection by repeated inspection: more on pied flycatchers. – Anim. Behav.
51:739-745.
Gilbert, S. F. 2001. Ecological developmental biology: developmental biology meets the real world. –
Develop. Biol. 233:1-12.
Gilbert, S. F. and Epel, D. 2008. Ecological developmental biology. – Sinauer Associates, Sutherland,
MA.
Giraldeau, L.-A., Valone, T. J. and Templeton, J. J. 2002. Potential disadvantages of using socially
acquired information. - Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 357:1559-1566.
Goodale, E. and Kotagama, S. W. 2005. Alarm calling in Sri Lankan mixed-species flocks. – Auk
122:108-120.
Greene, E. 1989. A diet-induced developmental polymorphism in a caterpillar. – Science 243:643-646.
Gripenberg, S., Salminen, J-H. and Roslin, T. 2007. A tree in the eyes of a moth – temporal variation in
oak leaf quality and leaf-miner performance. – Oikos 116: 592-600.
Hetrick, S. A. 2006. Investigation of tufted titmouse (Baeolophus bicolor) anti-predator vocalizations. M. Sc. Thesis, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.
Hetrick, S. A., Sieving, K. E. and Avery, K. L. 2009. Anti-predator communication by the tufted
titmouse (Baeolophus bicolor) I: encoding of predation risk information. – Oikos: this issue.
Holt, R. D. 2007. IJEE Soapbox: The unraveling of nature’s information webs: the next depressing
frontier in conservation? – Israel J. Ecol Evol. 53:229-236.
Holt, R. D., Grover, J. and Tilman, D. 1994. Simple rules for interspecific dominance in systems with
exploitative and apparent competition. – Am. Nat. 144: 741-771.
Jablonka, E. 2002. Information: Its interpretation, its inheritance, and its sharing. - Philos Sci
69:578-605.
Kendal, R. L., Coolen, I. and Laland, K. N. 2004. The role of conformity in foraging when personal and
social information conflict. - Behav. Ecol. 15:269-277.
Kokko, H. and Sutherland, W. J. 2001. Ecological traps in changing environments: ecological and
evolutionary consequences of a behaviorally mediated Allee effect. – Evol. Eco. Res. 3:537-551.
Koops, M. A. 1998. Misinformation and assessment uncertainty in the ecology of information use. - PhD
Thesis,. Univ. of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Canada.
27
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
Koops, M. A. 2004. Reliability and the value of information. – Anim. Behav. 67:103-111.
Kriska, G., Horváth, G. and Andrikovics. 1998. Why do mayflies lay their eggs en masse on dry asphalt
roads? Water-imitating polarized light reflected from asphalt attracts Ephemeroptera. - J. Exp. Biol.
201:2273-2286.
Kunert, G., Otto, S., Röse, U. S. R., Gershenzon, J. Weisser, W. W. 2005. Alarm pheromone mediates
production of winged dispersal moprphs in aphids. – Ecol. Lett. 8:506-603.
Lachmann, M., Donaldson-Matasci, M. C. and Bergstrom, C. T. 2009. The fitness value of information.
– Oikos: this issue.
Laiolo, P. and Tella, J. L. 2007. Erosion of animal cultures in fragmented landscapes. – Frontiers
in Ecol and the Environ. 5:68-72.
Langham GM, Contreras TA, Sieving KE. 2006. Why pishing works: Titmouse (Paridae) scolds elicit a
generalized response in bird communities. - Ecoscience 13:485-496.
Leibold, M. A. 1995. The niche concept revisited: mechanistic models and community context. - Ecology
76:1371-1382.
Levins, R. 1968. Evolution in changing environments. – Princeton University Press, New Jersey.
Lewis, M.A. and Murray, J. D. 1993. Modeling territoriality and wolf-deer interactions. – Nature
366:738-740.
Lima, S. L. 1984. Downy woodpecker foraging behaviour: efficient sampling in simple stochastic
environments. Ecology 65: 166-174.
Lima, S. L. and Bednekoff, P. A. 1999. Temporal variation in danger drives antipredator behavior: the
predation risk allocation hypothesis. – Am. Nat. 153:649-659.
Lima, S. L. and Zollner, P. A. 1996. Towards a behavioral ecology of landscapes. – Trends Ecol. Evol.
11:131-135.
Lürling, M. and Scheffer, M. 2007. Info-disruption: pollution and the transfer of chemical information
between organisms. – Trends Ecol. Evol. 22:374-379.
Luttbeg, B, 1996. A comparative Bayes tactic for mate assessment and choice. - Behav. Ecol 7:451-460.
Luttbeg, B. and Schmitz, O. J. 2000. Predator and prey models with flexible individual behavior and
imperfect information. – Am. Nat. 15: 669-683.
28
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
Massot, M. and Clobert, J. 2000. Processes at the origin of similarities in dispersal behavior among
siblings. – J. Evol. Biol. 13:707-719.
804
805
806
Nocera, J. J., Taylor, P. D. and Ratcliffe, L. M. 2008 Inspection of mob-calls as sources of predator
information: response of migrant and resident birds in the Neotropics. - Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62: 17691777.
Mathis, A., Ferrari, M. C. O., Windel, N. Messier, F. and Chivers, D. P. 2008. Learning by embryos and
the ghost of predation future. – Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0754.
Maynard Smith, J. 2000. The concept of information in biology. - Phil. Sci. 67:177-194.
McNamara, J. M. and Dall, S. R. X. 2009. Information is a fitness enhancing resource. – Oikos: this
issue.
McNamara, J. M., Green, R. F. and Olsson, O. 2006. Bayes’ theorem and its application in animal
behavior. – Oikos 112:243-251.
McNamara, J. M. and Houston, A. 1980. The application of statistical decision theory to animal
behavior. – J. Theor. Biol. 125: 385-395.
Mihoub, J-B., Le Gouar, P., and Sarrazin, F. 2009. Breeding habitat selection behaviors in
heterogeneous environments: implications for modeling reintroduction. – Oikos (in press).
DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2008.17142.x
Mitchell, W. A. and Valone, T. J. 1990. The optimization research program: studying adaptations by
their function. – Q. Rev. Biol. 65:43-65.
Moller, A. P. 1992. Interspecific responses to playback of bird song. - Ethology 90:315-320.
Mönkkönen, M. and Forsman, J.K. 2002. Heterospecific attraction among forest birds: a review. Ornithol. Sci. 1:41-51.
Mönkkönen, M., Helle, P. and Soppela, K. 1990. Numerical and behavioral responses of migrant
passerines in experimental manipulation of resident tits (Parus spp.): heterospecific attraction in northern
breeding bird communities? - Oecologia 85:218-225.
Moore, R. P., Robinson, W. D., Lovette, I. J. and Robinson, T. R. 2008. Experimental evidence for
extreme dispersal limitation in tropical forest birds. - Ecol. Lett. 11:960-968.
Nocera, J. J., Forbes, G.J. and Giraldeau, L. A. 2006. Inadvertent social information in breeding site
selection of natal dispersing birds.- Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 273:349-355.
29
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
829
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
Olsson, O. and J. S. Brown. 2009. Smart, smarter, smartest: foraging information states and coexistence.
– Oikos: this issue.
Olsson, O. and J. S. Brown. 2006. The foraging benefits of information and the penalty of ignorance. Oikos 112: 260-273.
Olsson, O. and N. M. A. Holmgren. 2000. Optimal Bayesian foraging policies and prey population
dynamics: some comments on Rodríguez-Gironés and Vásquez. - Theor. Popul. Biol. 57: 369-375.
Olsson, O., Wiktander, U., Holmgren, N. M. A. and Nilsson, S.. 1999. Gaining ecological information
about Bayesian foragers through their behaviour. II. A field test with woodpeckers. - Oikos 87: 264-276.
Palmer, A. R. 1985. Adaptive value of shell variation in Thais lamellose: effect of thick shells on
vulnerability to and preference by crabs. – Veliger 27:349-356.
Parejo, D., White, J., Clobert, J., Dreiss, A. and Danchin, E. 2007. Blue tits use fledgling quantity and
quality as public information in breeding site choice. – Ecology 88:2373-2382.
Parejo, D., Danchin, E. and Aviles, J. M. 2005. The heterospecific habitat copying hypothesis: can
competitors indicate habitat quality? – Behav. Ecol. 16:96-105.
Peake, T. M. 2005. Eavesdropping in communication networks. - In: McGregor P, (ed.), Animal
Communication Networks. Cambridge University Press, pp. 13-37.
Peacor, S. D. 2003. Phenotypic modifications to conspecific density arising from predation risk
assessment. – Oikos 100:409-415.
Pechenik, J. A., Wendt, D. E. and Jarrett, J. N. 1998. Metamorphosis is not a new beginning: larval
experience influences juvenile performance. – BioSci. 48:901-910.
Philippi, T. and Seger, J. 1989. Hedging one’s evolutionary bets, revisited. – TREE 4:41-44.
Pimm, S., Raven, P., Peterson, A., Sekercioglu, C. H. and Ehrlich, P. R. 2006. Human impacts on the
rates of recent, present, and future bird extinctions. – Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 29:10941-10946.
Plotkin, H. C. 1997. Darwin machines and the nature of knowledge. – Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge,
MA.
Pomeroy, A. C. 2006. Tradeoffs between food abundance and predation danger in spatial usage of a
stopover site by western sandpiper, Calidris mauri. - Oikos112:629-637.
Pomeroy, A. C., Butler, R. W. and Ydenberg, R. C. 2006. Experimental evidence that migrants adjust
usage at a stopover site to trade off food and danger. - Behav. Ecol. 17:1041-1045.
30
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
860
861
862
863
864
865
866
867
868
869
870
871
872
873
874
875
876
877
878
879
880
881
882
883
884
885
886
887
888
889
890
891
892
893
894
895
Pulliam, H. R. and Danielson, B. J. 1991. Sources, sinks, and habitat selection – a landscape perspective
on population dynamics. – Am. Nat. 137: S50-S66.
Quinn, J. L., Whittingham, M. J., Butler, S. J. and Cresswell, W. 2006. Noise, predation risk
compensation and vigilance in the chaffinch Fringilla coelbs. – J. Avian Biol. 37:601-608.
Rabin, L. A., Coss, R. G. and Owingsm D. H. 2006. The effects of wind turbines on antipredator
behavior in California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). – Biol. Conser. 131:410-420.
Ragusso-Netto, J. 2002. Vigilance toward raptors by nuclear species in bird mixed flocks in a Brazilian
savannah. - Stu. Neotrop. Fauna E. 37:219-226.
Ray, C., Gilpin, M. and Smith, A. T. 1991. The effect of conspecific attraction on metapopulation
dynamics. – Biol. J. Linn. 42:123-234.
Reltea, R. A. 2007. Getting out alive: how predators affect the decision to metamorphose. – Oecol.
152:389-400.
Resco, V., Hartwell, J and Hall, A. 2009. Ecological implications of plants’ ability to tell the time. –
Ecol. Lett. 12 (in press). doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01295.x.
Robertson, B. A. and Hutto, R. L. 2006. A framework for understanding ecological traps and an
evaluation of existing evidence. – Ecology 87:1075-1085.
Rodenhouse, N. L., Sherry, T. W. and Holmes, R. T. 1997. Site-dependent regulation of population size:
a new synthesis. - Ecology 78:2025-2042.
Rodríguez-Gironés, M. A. and Vásquez, R. A. 1997. Density-dependent patch exploitation and
acquisition of environmental information. - Theor. Popul. Biol. 52: 32-42.
Roth, T. C., III and Lima, S. L. 2007. Use of prey hotspots by an avian predator: purposeful
unpredictability? – Am. Nat. 169: 264-273.
Schlaepfer, M. A., Runge, M. C. and Sherman, P. W. 2002. Ecological and evolutionary traps. – Trends
Ecol. Evol. 17:474-480.
Schmidt, K. A. 2001. Site fidelity in habitats with contrasting levels of nest predation and brood
parasitism. - Evo. Ecol. Res. 3:633-648.
Schmidt, K. A. 2004. Site-fidelity in temporally correlated environments enhances population
persistence. - Ecol. Lett. 7:176-184.
Schmidt, K. A. and Whelan, C. J. 2009. Information and reproductive decision making: evolutionary
ecology of optimal renesting behavior. – Oikos: this issue.
31
896
897
898
899
900
901
902
903
904
905
906
907
908
909
910
911
912
913
914
915
916
917
918
919
920
921
922
923
924
925
926
927
928
929
930
931
932
933
934
935
936
937
938
Schmidt, K. A., Lee, E., Ostfeld, R. S. and Sieving, K. E. 2008. Eastern chipmunks increase their
perception of predation risk in response to titmouse alarm calls. – Behav. Ecol. 19:759-763.
Schmidt, K. A., Ostfeld, R. S. and Smyth, K. N. 2006. Spatial heterogeneity in predator activity, nest
survivorship, and nest-site selection in two forest thrushes. - Oecol. 148:22-29.
Schwartz, M. W. and Hoeksema, J. D. 1998. Specialization and resource trade: biological markets as a
model of mutualisms. – Ecology 79:1029-1038.
Seppänen, J-T., Forsman, J. T., Mönkkönen, M. and Thomson, R. L. 2007. Social information use is a
process across time, space, and ecology, reaching heterospecifics. – Ecology 88:1622-1633.
Seppänen, J-T., Mönkkönen, M, Forsman, J. and Lusseau, D. 2009. Niche overlap, information
networks, and limiting dissimilarity. – Oikos: (this issue).
Shannon, C. E. and Weaver, W. 1949. The mathematical theory of communication. - University of
Illinois Press, Urbana, IL, USA.
Sieving, K. E., Contreras, T. A. and Shaffer, T. L. 2004. Heterospecific facilitation of fortes-boundary
crossing by mobbing birds in North-central; Florida. – Auk 121:738-751.
Simpson, S. D., Meekan, M. G., Jeffs, A., Montgomery, J. C. and McCauley, R. D. 2008. Settlementstage coral reef fish prefer the higher-frequency invertebrate-generated audible component of reef noise. –
Anim. Behav. 75:1861-1868.
Slabbekoorn, H. and Ripmeester, E. A. P. 2008. Birdsong and anthropogenic noise: implications and
applications for conservation. - Mol. Ecol. 17:72-83.
Smith, W. J. 1977. The behavior of communicating: an ethological approach. - Harvard Univ. Press,
Cambridge, MA, USA.
Sullivan, K. A. 1984. Information exploitation by downy woodpeckers in mixed-species flocks. –
Behavior 91:294-311.
Sutherland, W. J. and Norris, K. 2002. Behavioral models of population growth rates: implications for
conservation and prediction. - Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 357:1273-1284.
Stamps, J. A. 2001. Habitat selection by dispersers: integrating proximate and ultimate approaches. - In:
Clobert, J., Danchin, E., Dhondt, A. A. and Nichols, J. D. (eds), Dispersal. Oxford university Press, pp.
230-242.
Stephens, D. W. 1989. Variance and the value of information. – Am. Nat. 134:128-140.
32
939
940
941
942
943
944
945
946
947
948
949
950
951
952
953
954
955
Stephens, D. W., Brown, J. S. and Ydenburg, R. C. (eds). 2007. Foraging behavior and ecology. –
University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.
956
957
958
959
960
961
962
963
964
965
966
967
968
969
970
971
972
973
974
975
976
977
978
979
980
981
Tilman, T. 1982. Resource Competition and Community Structure. – Princeton Univ. Press.
Taylor, P. D., Fahrig, L., Henein, K. and Merriam, G. 1993. Connectivity is a vital element of landscape
structure. - Oikos 68:571-573.
Templeton, C. N. and Greene, E. 2007. Nuthatches eavesdrop on vocalizations in heterospecific
chickadee mobbing calls. - PNAS 104:5479-5482.
Templeton, C. N., Greene, E. and Davis, K. 2005. Allometry of alarm calls: Black-capped chickadees
encode information about predator size. - Science 308:1934-1937.
Terborgh, J. 1990. Mixed flocks and polyspecific associations: costs and benefits of mixed groups of
birds and monkeys. – Am. J. Primatol. 21:87-100.
Thomson, R. L., Forsman, J. and Mönkkönen, M. 2003. Positive interactions between migrant and
resident birds: testing the heterospecific attraction hypothesis. – Oecologia 134:431-438.
Tilman, D. and J.A. Downing. 1994. Biodiversity and stability in grasslands. - Nature 367:363-365.
Tubelis, D. P., Cowling, A., and Donnelly, C. 2006. Role of mixed-species flocks in the use of adjacent
savannas by forest birds in the central Cerrado, Brazil. – Austral Ecology 31: 38-45.
Valone, T. J. 1989. Group foraging, public information, and patch estimation. – Oikos 56:357-363.
Valone, T. J. 2007. From eavesdropping on performance to copying behavior of others: a review of
public information use. - Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62:1-14.
Valone, T. J. 2006. Are animals capable of Bayesian updating? An empirical review. – Oikos 112:252259.
van Bergen, Y., Coolen, I. and Laland, K. N. 2004. Nine-spined sticklebacks exploit the most reliable
source when public and private information conflict. – Proc. R. Soc. B 271:957-962.
van den Hout, P. J., Spaans, B. and Piersma, T. 2008. Differential mortality of wintering shorebirds on the
Banc d’Arguin, Mauritania, due to predation by large falcons. - Ibis 150 (Suppl. 1):219-230.
van Gils, J. A. 2009. State-dependent Bayesian foraging on spatially auto-correlated food distributions.
Oikos: this issue.
van Gils, J. A., Edelaar, P., Escudero, G. and Piersma, T. 2004. Carrying capacity models should not use
fixed prey density thresholds: a plea for using more tools of behavioural ecology. Oikos 104: 197-204.
33
982
983
984
985
986
987
988
989
990
991
992
993
994
995
996
997
998
999
1000
1001
1002
1003
1004
1005
1006
1007
1008
1009
1010
1011
1012
1013
1014
1015
1016
1017
1018
van Gils, J. A., Schenk, I. W., Bos, O. and Piersma, T. 2003. Incompletely informed shorebirds that face a
digestive constraint maximize net energy gain when exploiting patches. Am. Nat. 161: 777-793.
Vincent, T. L. S., Scheel, D., Brown, J. S. and Vincent, T. L. 1996. Trade-offs and coexistence in
consumer-resource models: It all depends on what and where you eat. – Am. Nat. 148:1038-1058.
Vitala, J. Korpimäke, E, Palokangas, P and Koivukam, M. 1995. Attraction of kestrels to vole scent
marks visible in ultraviolet-light. – Nature 373: 425-427.
Vitousek, M. N., Adelman, J. S., Gregory, N. C. and St Clair, J. J. H. 2007. Heterospecific alarm call
recognition in a non-vocal reptile. - Biol. Lett. 3: 632-634.
Vos, M. Berrocal, S. M., Karamaouna, F. Hemerik, L. and Vet, L. E. M. 2001. Plant-mediated indirect
effects and the persistence of parasitoid-herbivore communities. - Ecol. Lett. 4:38-45.
Vos, M., Vet, L. E. M., Wäckers, F. L., Middelburg, J.J., van der Putten, W. H., Mooij, W. M., Heip, C.
H. R. and van Donk, E. 2006. Infochemicals structure marine, terrestrial, and freshwater food webs:
implications for ecological informatics. – Ecol. Informatics 1:23-32.
Wagner, R. H. and Danchin, E. 2009. A taxonomy of information. – Oikos: this issue.
Ward, K. P. 2005. Habitat selection by dispersing yellow-headed blackbirds: evidence of prospecting
and the use of public information. - Oecol. 145:650-657.
Welton, N. J., McNamara, J. M. and Houston, A. I. 2003. Assessing predation risk: optimal behavior and
rules of thumb. - Theor. Pop. Biol. 64:417-430.
Wolters, S. and Zuberbühler, K. 2003. Mixed-species associations of Diana and Campbell’s monkeys:
the costs and benefits of a forest phenomenon. – Behavior 140:371-385.
Wong, B. B. M., Bibeau, C. Bishop, K. A. and Rosenthal, G. G. 2005. Response to perceived predation
threat in fiddler crabs: trust thy neighbor as thyself? – Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 58:345-350.
Zuk, M., Rotenberry, J. T. and Tinghitella, R. M. 2006. Silent night: adaptive disappearance of a sexual
signal in a parasitized population of field crickets. – Biol. Lett. 2:521-524.
34
1019
FIGURE LEGENDS
1020
Figure 1: Domains of the disciplines that investigate information in biology at the organism-level. The
1021
figure is adapted in part from Koops (1998).
1022
1023
Figure 2: Consider information as a change in the probability (; or probability distribution) of an event
1024
or state, e.g.,  might be a prey’s estimate of the probability a predator is present in a forest patch
1025
occupied by the prey or male quality as judged by a female’s observations of male display. An individual
1026
has a prior estimate, PR, which is altered (PO; posterior) as information is processed from an observation
1027
(e.g., an alarm call). PO decays over time (this functional form is illustrated as D) as information is
1028
temporally discounted, and eventually settles back to the prior value (PR). The form of the decay curve
1029
will be highly variable and specific to context. Information may be used to permanently assign a state
1030
(e.g., an individual’s gender or a fishless pond) that may result in a permanent developmental switch
1031
(polyphenism) or time its development
1032
time or space (behavioral). Lastly, norm of reaction
1033
Evolutionary traps are produced when one or more of these three responses (PR, PO, and D) no longer
1034
matches its environment because the type or reliability of the cue or the state it ‘refers’ to has changed.
. Alternatively, states repeatedly cycle between states in
1035
1036
Figure 3: Mean persistence time ( SE) as a function of the number of component patches in the network
1037
in a temporal autocorrelated landscape ( = 0.7). Dispersal is even (black line) or using the WSLS rule
1038
(for more details see Schmidt 2004).
1039
1040
1041
1042
35
1043
1044
FIGURE 1
Information Source
Sensory ecology,
Cognition,
Psychology
Transmission
Reception
Translation
Perception
Evaluation
Decision, i.e., response
of the receiver

Behavioral , developmental
and life history changes
Consequence
Populations
(public info)
Evolution
(genetic info)
Communities
Evolutionary Ecology
Ecosystems
1045
1046
1047
1048
1049
1050
1051
1052
36
1053
1054
1055
1056
FIGURE 2
PO

Polyphenism or Norm of Reaction
D
behavior
Norm of reaction
PR
cue or
signal
1057
1058
1059
1060
1061
Time (or space)
37
1062
1063
1064
1065
FIGURE 3
Mean population persistence (yrs)
2000
WSLS, rho = 0.7
1500
1000
500
Even dispersal, rho = 0.7
0
2
1066
1067
1068
1069
1070
1071
1072
1073
1074
1075
3
4
5
6
7
Number of patches
38
8
1076
1077
Box 1: Glossary of terms: Note, these definitions closely follow Wagner and Danchin (this issue)
1078
Cues – A detectable fact that is non-intentionally produced. Includes facts produced by physical agents
1079
or inadvertently produced by biological agents.
1080
1081
Signals – A trait or behavior of a signaler evolved specifically to alter the behavior of the receiver in a
1082
way to benefit the signaler. The change in receiver behavior should also have evolved to enhanced
1083
receiver fitness.
1084
1085
Public information – Information that is in the public domain and potentially available to any organism.
1086
1087
Private information - Information that is undetectable to other organisms through direct means. Private
1088
information may become public through indirect means. This follows a sequence (using the model by
1089
Seppänen et al. 2007): 1) observation of an event or state by a primary observer; 2) a decision that is
1090
manifest via change in behavior (i.e., an action) of the primary observer; 3) the consequence of the
1091
action. For example, Wong et al. (2005) demonstrated that sand fiddler crabs (Uca pugilator) use
1092
observations of threat-induced responses of neighbors (stage 2 in the sequence above) to guide their
1093
own refuge-seeking behavior. Birds settling on territories where conspecific reproductive success was
1094
high in prior years (see Breeding habitat selection) is an example of observing the consequences (stage
1095
3) of past decisions by conspecifics.
1096
1097
Socially acquired Information – Information extracted from other individuals (con- or heterospecific) be
1098
they signals or (inadvertent) cues (including actions and consequences). Note: all social information
1099
must be public.
1100
39
1101
Interceptive and Social Eavesdropping (Peake 2005) – A mechanism of acquiring social information from
1102
signals (i.e., communication) between two (or more) individuals. In interceptive eavesdropping
1103
individuals acquire information about their environment (e.g., an alarm call gives information about the
1104
presence of a predator), whereas in social eavesdropping individuals acquire information regarding the
1105
social relationship between the communicating parties (e.g., dominance hierarchy, kinship).
1106
40
1107
Box 2: Breeding habitat selection/settlement strategies as a function of spatial heterogeneity and
1108
temporal predictability. Four regions are identified across what is in reality a continuum. In the top
1109
row, high variation in individual (fine scale) site quality exists, whereas patch quality, averaged over the
1110
many individual sites it contains, is low. Fidelity to successful sites is favored provided temporal
1111
predictability is high (top right). Likewise, dispersal from unsuccessful sites is favored and individuals
1112
should prospect for future sites not patches since patch reproductive success has little spatial variation.
1113
We call this the win-stay, lose-prospect strategy (WSLP). When spatial variation is higher between
1114
patches than sites (bottom row) individuals should prospect for information on patch reproductive
1115
success provided temporal predictability is high (bottom right). Fidelity or dispersal should be linked
1116
closely to patch reproductive success rather than an individual’s own success. When temporal
1117
predictability is low an individual should prospect during the pre-breeding season for current, proximate
1118
cues of reproductive potential at the scale of greatest spatial: at sites (top left) or patches (bottom left).
1119
Conspecific attraction is only favored when spatial variation is higher between patches than sites,
1120
assuming individuals preempt sites by occupation. It further requires that some individuals (call them
1121
prospectors) use patch reproductive success as a settlement cue. In other words, conspecific attraction
1122
is an information-scrounger strategy that requires information collected by the information-producer
1123
strategy of prospectors (Dall et al. 2005).
1124
1125
1126
1127
41
1128
1129
1130
1131
BOX 2
Spatial
heterogeneity
Site
Fine:
site > patch
Patch
1132
1133
1134
1135
1136
1137
1138
1139
1140
1141
1142
1143
Coarse:
patch > site
(1)
(2)
Fidelity: none
Info: none
Conspecific attraction: no
Prospect: pre-breeding;
prospect at sites
Fidelity: to site
Info: pers. repro. success
Conspecific attraction: no
Prospect: pre- or postbreeding; prospect at sites
(3)
(4)
Fidelity: none
Info:
none
PRS
high
Conspecific attraction: ?
Prospect: pre-breeding;
Prospect at patches
Fidelity: to patch
Info: patch repro. success
Conspecific attraction: yes
PRS pre- or postProspect:
breeding; prospect at
patches
low
high
temporal predictability
42