Download Alcohol Abuse in Individuals Exposed to Trauma: A

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Abnormal psychology wikipedia , lookup

Controversy surrounding psychiatry wikipedia , lookup

Emergency psychiatry wikipedia , lookup

Political abuse of psychiatry wikipedia , lookup

Substance use disorder wikipedia , lookup

Generalized anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup

Dissociative identity disorder wikipedia , lookup

Posttraumatic stress disorder wikipedia , lookup

Alcoholism wikipedia , lookup

Alcohol withdrawal syndrome wikipedia , lookup

Treatments for combat-related PTSD wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Psychological Bulletin
1996, Vol. 120, No. 1, 83-112
Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0033-2909/96/$3.00
Alcohol Abuse in Individuals Exposed to Trauma: A Critical Review
Sherry H. Stewart
Dalhousie University
In this article, the author critically reviews studies on the relationship between exposure to trauma,
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and alcohol abuse. After establishing that strong relationships
exist between exposure to traumatic events and alcohol problems, and particularly between the
diagnoses of PTSD and alcoholism, the author discusses various factors, theories, and possible mechanisms to account for these associations. Moreover, she discusses applicationsof these findings to the
assessment and treatment of people exposed to trauma who abuse alcohol. Finally, the author outlines novel methods for testing theoretical hypotheses and makes suggestions for methodological
improvements in future research.
siderable empirical data suggest an overlap between PTSD and
the abuse of various illicit substances (e.g., opiates; Cottler,
Compton, Mager, Spitznagel, & Janca, 1992), this review focuses only on the abuse of alcohol, given its ready accessibility.
Finally, this review examines only the development of alcoholism following trauma, although there is evidence that alcohol
abuse increases the likelihood of being a victim or a perpetrator
of trauma (Murdoch, Pihl, & Ross, 1990; Pihl, Peterson, & Lau,
1993).
This article critically reviews the evidence for an important
functional relationship between posttraumatic stress disorder
(PTSD) and alcoholism. Although several reviewers have
shown a significant overlap between certain anxiety disorders
and alcohol abuse (e.g., B. J. Cox, Norton, Swinson, & Endler,
1990; Kushner, Sher, & Beitman, 1990; Pihl & Stewart, 1991 ),
they did not consider the relationship between PTSD and alcoholism. One review examined the relationship between PTSD
and substance abuse (i.e., Keane, Gerardi, Lyons, & Wolfe,
1988 ). However, their focus was very different from this review,
assessing abuse of a wide range of chemical substances by only
Vietnam veterans exposed t o trauma. The purposes of this article are to (a) examine the empirical evidence for the cooccurrence of trauma exposure and alcohol problems and for
the comorbidity between PTSD and alcoholism across various
traumatic events; (b) critique methodological issues in the literature; (c) review theoretical mechanisms that may account
for the relationship of trauma exposure, PTSD, and alcohol
abuse; (d) discuss the implications of this research for the assessment and treatment of individuals comorbid for PTSD and
alcoholism; and (e) suggest directions for future research.
It should be made clear at the outset that this review focuses
specifically on alcohol abuse in response to psychological
trauma (e.g., sexual assault) as opposed to stressful life events
(i.e., negative events that are not necessarily traumatic, such as
divorce; Holmes & Rahe, 1967) or physical trauma (e.g., head
injury). As these latter two issues are not a focus of this article,
the interested reader is referred to reviews on the associations
between alcohol abuse and stressful life events (Beckman, 1976;
Benson & Wilsnack, 1983; Straussner, 1985 ) and alcohol abuse
and physical trauma (Solomon & Malloy, 1992). Although con-
Definitions
Throughout this review, there are frequent references to the
diagnoses of PTSD and alcoholism. I provide definitions and
base-rate prevalences of these two forms of behavioral pathology to familiarize the reader with these disorders from the
outset.
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
PTSD comprises a characteristic set of symptoms that may
develop following the experiencing, witnessing, or confronting
of an event involving actual or threatened serious injury or
death or a threat to the physical integrity of the self or others
(Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
[DSM-IV] 4th ed.; American Psychiatric Association [APA],
1994). Such events are experienced with fear, helplessness, or
horror and are exemplified by combat, sexual and physical assault, natural and man-made disasters, and accidents. The characteristic symptoms include reexperiencing the traumatic event
(intrusive thoughts, flashbacks, nightmares, and physiological
reactivity to reminders of the trauma), avoidance of reminders
of the trauma (efforts to avoid stimuli associated with the
trauma and to suppress thoughts or feelings about it), emotional numbing (inability to feel close to others, to experience
positive emotions, or to enjoy pleasurable activities), and increased physiological arousal (hypervigilance, exaggerated startle response, concentration difficulties, sleep disturbance, and
irritability). A PTSD diagnosis requires that symptoms be
present for more than l month and that the disturbance causes
I thank Richard J. McNally and Robert O. Pihl for their helpful comments on drafts of this article.
This article was supported by the Medical Research Council of Canada Grant MT- 12614.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Sherry H. Stewart, Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University,
Life Sciences Centre, 1355 Oxford Street, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada
B3H 4J 1. Electronic mail be sent via Internet to [email protected].
83
84
STEWART
clinically significant distress or impairment in important areas
of functioning.
Exposure to life-threatening and catastrophic experiences,
whether in the context of war, crime, disasters, or accidents, has
lasting consequences for many individuals. A recent epidemiological study estimated the prevalence of PTSD in the adult
population to be between i% and 2% (Helzer, Robins, &
McEvoy, 1987). Such estimates potentially yield some 2.4 to
4.8 million cases of PTSD in the United States alone.
Alcoholism
This article takes a broad view of alcohol-related problems,
reviewing the literature on both alcohol abuse and alcohol dependence. A DSM-IV diagnosis of alcohol abuse is made when
an individual's pattern of alcohol use is maladaptive, as indicated by at least one of the following symptoms within a 12month period: recurrent alcohol use that results in not being
able to fulfill major role obligations, recurrent use in physically
hazardous situations (e.g., drunk driving), recurrent alcoholrelated legal problems, or continued use despite persistent social problems, occupational problems, or both caused or exacerbated by alcohol's effects.
Alcohol dependence involves a cluster of symptoms indicating impaired control over alcohol use. In addition to the characteristic symptoms of alcohol abuse, other symptoms of alcohol dependence include the use of alcohol in larger amounts or
over a longer period than the individual intended; prior unsuccessful attempts to control the use of alcohol; a great deal of
time spent acquiring alcohol or recovering from its effects; impairment in social, occupational, or recreational activities due
to alcohol use; heightened tolerance to alcohol; withdrawal
symptoms resulting from discontinuation of alcohol use or alcohol use to relieve or avoid withdrawal symptoms; and continued alcohol use despite knowledge of having a physical or psychological problem caused or exacerbated by alcohol.
Alcohol and drug abuse are the most prevalent of all the psychiatric disorders (Robins et al., 1984). The Epidemiological
Catchment Area (ECA) study showed a lifetime prevalence rate
of alcohol abuse-dependence in the United States of 12-16%
(Robins et al., 1984).
C o m o r b i d i t y Studies
In the first half of this section, I establish an association between trauma exposure and alcohol problems. In the last half
of this section, I review literature on the association between the
diagnoses of PTSD and alcoholism. Throughout this section, I
attempt to establish whether it is trauma exposure or PTSD
that is most highly related to posttraumatic abusive drinking.
Following this section, I provide a methodological critique of
the comorbidity literature and discuss various factors that
might contribute to the different comorbidity rates seen across
studies.
Is There a Relationship Between Trauma Exposure and
Alcohol Abuse-Dependence?
Because the majority of research on the association between
trauma exposure and alcohol abuse-dependence is limited to
three specific traumatic events (i.e., disasters, assault, and
combat), the literature review is organized around them.
Disasters. The relationship between trauma exposure and
alcohol abuse has been investigated retrospectively by examining changes in alcohol consumption and various "social indices" of abusive drinking (e.g., drunk driving charges) following
natural disasters. For example, Gleser, Green, and Winget
( 1981 ) found that alcohol consumption increased by 30% in a
2-year period following the 1972 flood at Buffalo Creek, West
Virginia. Similarly, P. R. Adams and Adams (1984) found significant increases in several social indices of alcohol abuse (e.g.,
drunk driving charges and referrals to community alcohol
centers) following the Mount Saint Helens' volcano eruption in
Washington State. These data suggest that exposure to trauma
in the form of a natural disaster may, indeed, lead to increases in
excessive drinking, at least in terms of social indices of abusive
drinking.
Green, Grace, and Gleser (1985) found a relationship between exposure to grotesque death and an alcohol abuse diagnosis in a sample of disaster survivors of the 1977 Beverly Hills
Supper Club fire. They also found that the greater the degree
of involvement in rescue efforts (i.e., exposure to burned and
mutilated bodies), the greater the likelihood of alcohol abuse 2
years after the fire. Thus, studies examining social indices of
drinking or alcohol abuse diagnoses suggest a relationship between exposure to disaster and subsequent abusive drinking.
Moreover, the data suggest that certain aspects of the trauma
(i.e., exposure to grotesque death) may be most highly associated with posttraumatic alcohol abuse.
Assault. Case reports in which alcoholism appears related
to a history of sexual assault, physical assault, or both are abundant in the literature. In this section, I review empirical studies
that examine the associations among sexual abuse, physical
abuse, or both in childhood (both intrafamilial and extrafamilial), sexual assault, physical assault, or both in adulthood (e.g., rape), and levels of alcohol abuse in adolescence or
adulthood.
Some researchers have examined rates of alcoholism in psychiatric outpatients (e.g., Mieselman, 1978; Swett, Cohen, Surrey, Compaine, & Chavez, 1991 ). For example, Swett et al.
( 1991 ) surveyed 189 women at an adult psychiatric outpatient
clinic. Women with a self-reported history of sexual abuse,
physical abuse, or both obtained significantly higher scores on
the Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (MAST; Seizer,
1971 ), a self-report measure of alcohol-related problems, than
did women reporting no such history. These results indicate
higher levels of alcohol problems in women with abuse histories. Interestingly, when the abuse occurred before the age of 18
with no subsequent abuse in adulthood, the association of abuse
history and high MAST scores persisted. Thus, even early abuse
may be associated with increased levels of problem drinking in
adult women.
Other researchers have examined the relationship between
victimization and alcohol abuse within more circumscribed
psychiatric samples. For example, Briere and Zaidi (1989) reviewed medical records of 50 women admitted to an urban psychiatric emergency room (85% of whom were unemployed).
More of the women reporting a history of sexual abuse met
DSM-III-R (3rd ed., rev., APA, 1987) criteria for alcohol
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
abuse than those reporting no such abuse histories (37% vs.
20%, respectively). However, both groups had higher rates of
alcohol abuse than lifetime prevalence rates seen in the general
female population of approximately 4% to 5% (e.g., Robins et
al., 1984), a finding that may be due to Briere and Zaidi's sample characteristics (i.e., from a psychiatric emergency room and
lower-than-average socioeconomic status [ SES ] ).
The prevalence of abuse histories in individuals (mainly
women) seeking or receiving treatment for alcoholism has also
been examined. Results consistently reveal exceedingly high
rates of self-reported sexual-abuse histories in samples of
women with alcoholism (i.e., 24-85%), although the precise
rates across studies vary widely (e.g., Cohen & Densen-Gerber,
1982; Kovach, 1986; Wilsnack, 1982, 1984). Despite these
differences, even the lowest rate is clinically significant when
compared with base rates. For example, the prevalence of incest
and sexual abuse, obtained with samples of women with alcoholism, are much higher than the 15-30% rate of incest reported by women in the general population (Finkelhor, 1979;
Gagnon, 1965 ) and the 16% rate of childhood sexual abuse reported by women referred for psychotherapy (Sheldon, 1988 ).
Covington (1983) included a matched group to control for the
effects of extraneous variables such as SES. She compared 35
women with alcoholism with 35 matched female controls without alcoholism. Significantly more women with alcoholism
than those without reported a history of incest (i.e., 34% vs.
17%, respectively).
B. A. Miller, Downs, and Testa ( 1993 ) examined the relationship between childhood victimization and subsequent alcoholism in adult women, while controlling for the effects of being in
treatment. They compared the reports of childhood victimization of a sample of women having alcoholism with those of a
sample of women without alcoholism in treatment for other
mental health problems. They found a higher rate of childhood
victimization in women with alcoholism versus the in-treatment controls, suggesting a specific link between victimization
and the later development of alcohol problems in adult women.
More important, the significant relationship between alcoholism diagnoses and childhood victimization remained, even after family background and demographic factors (e.g., parental
alcohol problems) were statistically controlled. Other studies of
samples of both male and female adolescents and adults also
support the connection between childhood victimization histories and the development of alcohol abuse problems (e.g., Flanigan, Potrykus, & Marti, 1988; Harrison, Hoffman, & Edwall,
1989; Hernandez, 1992; Hernandez, Lodico, & DiClemente,
1993; Pribor & Dinwiddie, 1992; M. R. Schaefer, Sobieraj, &
Hollyfield, 1988; Singer, Petchers, & Hussey, 1989).
Dembo and colleagues conducted extensive interviews with
juvenile detainees to determine the causal connections between
sexual abuse, physical abuse, or both and alcohol abuse (e.g.,
Dembo, Dertke, Borders, Washburn, & Schmeidler, 1988;
Dembo, Williams, Wothke, Schmeidler, & Brown, 1992). Using structural equation modeling, they obtained evidence that
both forms of abuse appear to precede and substantially contribute to the development of alcohol abuse. This finding is consistent across gender and over time (Watts & Ellis, 1993 ).
Other researchers have examined the relationship between
sexual assault, physical assault, or both and alcohol abuse in the
85
general population (i.e., non-treatment-seeking). For example,
Wilsnack and colleagues (Klassen & Wilsnack, 1986; Wilsnack,
1991 ) conducted a longitudinal survey of a large representative
American sample to investigate the causes and consequences of
women's drinking. The sample was divided into "problem" and
"nonproblem" drinkers. More than twice as many problem as
compared with nonproblem drinkers reported experiencing at
least one incident of sexual abuse before the age of 18. A history
of sexual abuse significantly predicted the onset of drinking
problems over the 5-year follow-up in those classified as nonproblem drinkers at the start of the study (i.e., 51% vs. 19% in
abused and nonabused groups, respectively). In another community study, Winfield, George, Swartz, and Blazer (1990) surveyed approximately 1,200 women from the North Carolina
site of the ECA study. All women were interviewed with the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS; Robins, Helzer, Croughan,
& Ratcliff, 1981 ) and were questioned about their histories of
sexual assault. DIS diagnoses of alcohol abuse-dependence
were significantly higher in the women assaulted versus the
women not assaulted.
Some data suggest that the nature of the abuse may differentiate individuals who later develop alcohol problems from those
who do not. For example, Covington (1983) found that incest
was more violent and more frequent, involved more perpetrators, and was of longer duration in the group with alcoholism as
compared with the control group without alcoholism. A similar
study by S. Schaefer, Evans, and Sterne (1985) compared 100
women in treatment for alcoholism with 42 matched female
controls without alcoholism on a number of childhood sexual
abuse measures. When a history of sexual abuse was reported,
the first incident occurred significantly earlier, its duration was
significantly longer, and informing a parent of the abuse was less
frequent in the women with alcoholism relative to the controls
without alcoholism. Thus, preliminary data suggest that the severity of sexual abuse may prove important in predicting the
development of alcohol abuse. Specifically, more severe levels of
abuse appear significantly associated with greater risk for alcohol problems.
Combat. An extensive literature exists on the relationship
between exposure to combat and the subsequent development
of alcohol abuse. For example, a national epidemiological study
(Boscarino, 1981) compared a group of Vietnam combat
veterans with control groups (noncombat veterans and
nonveterans), matched for important demographic characteristics, such as age, education level, and income. Combat veterans exhibited substantially higher levels of binge drinking than
the matched controls. Boman (1986) compared a psychiatric
sample of 23 male patients who had served in combat in Vietnam with a control psychiatric group of 25 male patients who
were currently serving in the military but had never been in
combat. Significantly more of the combat-exposed men than
the noncombat controls met DSM-III (3rd ed., APA, 1980)
criteria for alcohol abuse (78% vs. 36%, respectively). Because
preexisting differences between soldiers assigned to combat
duty and those not assigned might contribute to the differences
in posttrauma problem drinking (Roy, 1983), retrospective diagnoses were made for each participant for the period between
leaving high school and entering the military. About 20% of the
men in both groups met diagnostic criteria for alcohol abuse
86
STEWART
before military enlistment. The absence of a significant difference between the groups in terms of proportion with alcohol
abuse diagnoses before military service suggests that exposure
to combat, as opposed to preexisting group differences, is specifically associated with increased rates of alcohol abuse following military service.
Green, Grace, Lindy, Gleser, and Leonard (1990) examined
the relative contribution of prewar (e.g., level of education and
peer relationships in school), war (i.e., level of combat exposure), and postwar (e.g., negative life events and social
support) factors to the long-term postservice adjustment of a
group of Vietnam veterans, most of whom were not seeking
treatment. They administered the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (SADS; Endicott & Spitzer, 1978) to
obtain current diagnostic information. Prewar factors made a
significant contribution to the prediction of alcohol abuse diagnoses. However, the addition of war and postwar variables each
significantly improved the prediction. In contrast, combat
exposure made no additional contribution to the prediction of
other psychiatric disorders, including major depression, which
were largely explained by prewar factors. Thus, combat
exposure was again significantly (and quite specifically) associated with alcohol abuse, even after the consideration of other
relevant variables.
Additional data suggest that the level of combat exposure
may be specifically associated with an increase in abusive drinking. For example, Laufer, Yager, Frey-Wouters, and Donnellan
( 1981 ) found that severity of combat exposure was significantly
positively associated with self-reported drinking levels in a clinical sample of combat veterans. Kulka et al. (1990) replicated
this finding in a nonclinical sample of combat veterans from the
National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study (NVVRS).
Furthermore, Branchey, Davis, and Lieber (1984) showed in a
clinical sample that the association between combat exposure
and alcohol abuse persisted over an extended period after service in Vietnam. Yager, Laufer, and Gallops (1984) interviewed
a national nonclinical sample of men who were of military age
during the Vietnam War. These interviews were conducted 6 to
15 years after veterans in the sample had left military service.
When preservice background factors were controlled statistically, combat exposure severity showed a significant positive association with a measure of "daily drinking" (i.e., number of
months in the last 2 years the participant had consumed at least
a six pack of beer, a bottle of wine, or several drinks of hard
liquor almost every day). These findings suggest that the association between combat severity and heavy alcohol consumption
persists over an extended period following combat exposure,
even in nonclinical participants.
Sutker, Uddo, Brailey, Vasterling, and Errera (1994) extended this work by examining psychiatric diagnoses in military troops performing graves registration duties during the Persian Gulf War. Current and lifetime diagnoses of alcohol dependence tended to be higher in the war-zone deployed group (who
had been exposed to the handling of human remains) than in a
matched control group consisting of troops stationed stateside.
Thus, increased alcohol problems may not only be associated
with exposure to combat but also with other war-related traumatic experiences. In summary, research supports the notion
that combat exposure, severity of exposure, and other war-
related traumatic experiences are associated with increased levels of abusive drinking.
Epidemiological data. Cottler et al. (1992), using data
from nonclinical participants in the St. Louis ECA study, examined the relationship between exposure to a wide variety of
traumatic events and heavy alcohol use. "Heavy alcohol users"
were over three times more likely than "non-heavy alcohol users" and nonusers to report having been exposed to combatrelated trauma. However, the three alcohol-use groups did not
differ in terms of their reported exposure to other forms of traumatic events, including serious accident, physical attack, or witness to a killing or significant injury. These findings suggest that
rates of posttraumatic alcohol abuse may vary as a function of
the nature or type of traumatic exposure.
Summary. There is much empirical evidence attesting to an
important relationship between exposure to traumatic events
and increased abusive drinking. Moreover, the data suggest that
objective indices of trauma severity may be positively associated
with the degree of abusive alcohol consumption. Thus,
exposure to trauma, particularly severe trauma, may lead to the
development of alcohol abuse-dependence.
Is There a Relationship Between P T S D and Alcohol
Abuse-Dependence?
It is possible that the positive correlations between trauma
severity and alcohol problems do in fact reflect an association
between PTSD and alcohol abuse because trauma severity is
positively associated with the emergence of PTSD symptoms.
Thus, some researchers have suggested that an adverse psychological reaction to the trauma (i.e., PTSD symptoms) may better account for the increased abuse of alcohol following
exposure to trauma than exposure to trauma or severity of the
traumatic event, per se (e.g., LaCoursiere & Coyne, 1984). Recently, researchers have focused more directly on the relationship between the diagnoses of PTSD and alcohol abuse--dependence. In this section, I review studies on the comorbidity of
PTSD and alcoholism. Again, this review is organized with reference to the nature of the traumatic event (i.e., disaster, assault, and combat). I also review epidemiological studies spanning a wide variety of traumatic events.
PTSD due to disaster. Green, Lindy, Grace, and Leonard
(1992) conducted a 14-year posttrauma follow-up of 193 victims of the 1972 Buffalo Creek flood. Diagnoses were established with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R
(SCID; Spitzer, Williams, & Gibbon, 1987). A current PTSD
diagnosis was statistically related to an alcohol abuse diagnosis,
suggesting that participants who were experiencing significant
flood-related PTSD symptoms at the follow-up were also likely
to be abusing alcohol. Although the comorbidity pattern of
PTSD with other disorders in individuals exposed to the disaster
was similar to a previously tested sample of combat veterans
(Green, Lindy, Grace, & Gleser, 1989), a direct comparison
revealed that the individuals exposed to the disaster demonstrated lower rates of comorbid PTSD and alcohol abuse than
did the combat veterans. These data suggest that some types of
trauma may more likely lead to comorbid PTSD and alcohol
abuse than others. This result is consistent with Cottler et al.'s
( 1992 ) findings that heavy drinking was most highly associated
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
with prior exposure to combat trauma, as opposed to other
types of traumatic events.
PTSD due to assault. Kovach (1986) showed that nearly
40% of women attending Alcoholics Anonymous who reported
histories of incest, met D S M - I I I criteria for PTSD (about 12%
of a sample of I 17 women). She postulated that childhood incest may result in delayed onset PTSD, which in turn may contribute to the development of alcohol dependence. Kovach unfortunately did not assess her comparison group to determine if
PTSD was more likely to be found among women with alcoholism who were exposed to incest than among women with alcoholism who did not report a history of incest. Thus, Kovach's
postulation o f a PTSD link between childhood incest and adult
alcoholism remains speculative on the basis of her data alone
(Hurley, 1991 ).
Clark and Jacob (1992) examined comorbid psychiatric diagnoses in a sample of adolescents meeting D S M - I I I - R criteria
for alcohol abuse-dependence. Twenty-seven percent of the adolescents with alcoholism were diagnosed with PTSD, the most
frequent anxiety disorder diagnosis in the sample. Consistent
with Kovach's (1986) postulation of a link with childhood histories of sexual abuse, PTSD, and the later development of alcohol abuse problems, 88% of the adolescents comorbid for
PTSD and alcoholism reported prior histories of sexual abuse,
physical abuse, or both.
Also relevant to the review of the relationship of alcohol
abuse and PTSD due to assault are the emerging data on the
effects of exposure to other forms of violent crime. Kilpatrick
and Resnick (1993) reviewed data from a large national probability sample of adult women. The women were divided into
three groups: those with PTSD who experienced a crime, those
without PTSD who experienced a crime, and those not having
experienced a crime. All participants were assessed for the presence of alcohol problems. Independent of PTSD status, experiencing a crime increased the risk of developing alcohol problems. PTSD was an additional major risk factor for alcohol
problems within the groups who experienced a crime. Those
with PTSD were 3.2 times more likely than those without PTSD
and 13.7 times more likely than those not having experienced a
crime to report serious alcohol problems. These results suggest
that criminal victimization may increase the risk for alcohol
abuse among adult women. Moreover, those who experience a
crime and display extreme emotional distress following the
crime (i.e., those with PTSD) may be at highest risk for developing major alcohol problems.
PTSD due to combat. Many studies have examined the
overlap between combat-related PTSD and alcoholism. In fact,
the majority of research on the relationship between PTSD and
alcoholism has been conducted with combat veterans. For example, Branchey et al. (1984) reported that alcohol abuse is
the most prevalent coexisting psychiatric diagnosis in combat
veterans with PTSD. In the studies involving clinical samples
of male combat veterans with PTSD, rates of alcohol abusedependence are extremely high (4 I-85%), although the precise
rates vary widely across studies (e.g., Davidson, Kudler, Saunders, & Smith, 1990; Davidson, Swartz, Storck, Krishnan, &
Hammett, 1985; Escobar et al., 1983; Hendin & Haas, 1984;
Keane, Caddell, & Martin, 1983; Keane, Gerardi, et al., 1988;
Sierles, Chen, McFarland, & Taylor, 1983; Streimer, Cosstick, &
87
Tennant, 1985). The rates of alcoholism in these studies much
exceed the lifetime prevalence rates for alcohol abuse-dependence in the general male population of approximately 19-29%
(Robins et al., 1984).
The overlap between combat-related PTSD and alcoholism
has also been examined in nonclinical samples of veterans. In
the NVVRS ( Kulka et al., 1990), conducted with a nonclinical
sample of Vietnam veterans, 74% of the male veterans with
PTSD and 29% of the female veterans with PTSD also met
SCID (Spitzer et al., 1987) lifetime criteria for alcohol abuse.
The corresponding figures for current alcohol abuse diagnoses
(i.e., within the previous 6 months) were 22% for the men with
PTSD and 10% for the women with PTSD. Alcohol abuse was
the most common comorbid disorder among men with PTSD,
and the third most common comorbid disorder among women
with PTSD. Similar figures were obtained in the Center for Disease Control Vietnam Experience Study, also conducted with a
nonclinical sample of Vietnam veterans: 70% of Vietnam combat veterans who had ever met the criteria for PTSD also met
lifetime criteria for alcohol abuse (cited in Davidson & Fairbank, 1993). Thus, the strong relationship between PTSD and
alcohol abuse is evident, even in nonclinical samples.
Streimer et al. (1985) attempted to directly determine
whether alcohol abuse is associated with PTSD or merely with
exposure to combat. They examined the medical records of a
random sample of 126 inpatients who had served in Vietnam.
Only 29% of the sample met D S M - I I I criteria for a primary
diagnosis of alcohol abuse-dependence. However, the rate of alcohol abuse-dependence diagnoses rose to 38% in those also
diagnosed with combat-related PTSD, suggesting that alcoholism is more closely linked with PTSD than with combat
exposure, per se.
Boman (1986) also examined the relationship between combat-related PTSD and alcohol abuse. Vietnam veterans with
PTSD, Vietnam veterans without PTSD, and military personnel without PTSD who had not served overseas and who were
receiving psychiatric treatment were compared for rates of
D S M - I I I diagnoses of alcohol abuse, established with a standard psychiatric interview. The three groups were comparable
on important demographic variables (e.g., age and SES). Diagnoses of alcohol abuse were more prevalent in the Vietnam veterans with PTSD (78%) than in the veterans without PTSD
(72%) and more prevalent in the Vietnam veterans without
PTSD than in the nonveteran, non-PTSD controls ( 36% ). The
groups did not differ with respect to alcohol abuse diagnoses
before military enlistment. None of the non-PTSD Vietnam
veterans had been exposed to life-threatening combat, whereas
all those in the PTSD group had been exposed, introducing a
confound between combat exposure and a PTSD diagnosis.
Thus, it remains unclear as to whether the somewhat higher
rate of alcohol abuse in the Vietnam veterans group with PTSD
compared with the Vietnam veterans group without PTSD was
due specifically to PTSD symptoms or to the effects of combat
exposure itself.
McFall, Mackay, and Donovan (1992) compared MAST
(Selzer, 1971 ) scores in a sample of 108 combat-exposed Vietnam veterans and a sample of 151 Vietnam-era veterans who
had not been assigned combat duty. No significant differences
in scores on this measure were found between the groups. How-
88
STEWART
ever, the results were quite different when combat-exposed Vietnam veterans were divided into those who did and did not suffer
from PTSD, according to scores on the Mississippi Scale for
Combat-Related PTSD ( M - P T S D ; Keane, Caddell, & Taylor,
1988). Combat veterans with PTSD scored significantly higher
overall on the MAST than did combat veterans without PTSD,
suggesting greater levels of problem drinking in the group with
PTSD than in the combat veterans group without PTSD. These
data provide some preliminary evidence that PTSD, rather than
combat exposure, is linked to alcohol abuse problems. Correlational results also support this interpretation. In the combatexposed group, MAST scores were significantly positively correlated with the severity of PTSD symptoms but were not significantly associated with any of the objective indices of severity
of combat experience (i.e., scores on a combat scale, months in
combat, or age in combat). This study provides further evidence that the relationship between trauma and alcohol problems appears to be mediated by the individual's psychological
reaction to the trauma (i.e., PTSD symptoms) rather than by
the severity of the trauma.
Green et al. (1989) examined the degree to which a variety of
war-related stressor experiences could predict PTSD and other
comorbid diagnoses in veterans recruited from clinical and
nonclinical sources. Comorbid PTSD and alcohol abuse were
predicted by involvement in or exposure to mutilation, grotesque deaths, or both. This result held even after prewar diagnoses were statistically controlled. Sutker, Uddo, Brailey, Allain, and Errera (1994) also provided evidence that exposure to
grotesque death is strongly associated with comorbid PTSD and
alcoholism. Using the SCID (Spitzer et al., 1987), Sutker,
Uddo, Brailey, Allain, et al. found that almost h a l f o f a sample
of troops who performed graves registration duties in the Persian Gulf War met DSM-III-R criteria for PTSD. Many of
those diagnosed with PTSD also displayed symptoms ofcomorbid substance abuse disorder (including high rates of alcohol
abuse). These results are consistent with the increased rates of
alcohol abuse observed in fire disaster workers exposed to
burned and mutilated bodies (Green et al., 1992 ).
Epidemiological comorbidity studies. All the studies above
focus on individuals exposed to specific types of traumatic
events, making it difficult to establish the overall comorbidity of
PTSD and alcohol abuse-dependence across traumatic events.
Several recent epidemiological studies overcome this limitation
(see reviews by Davidson & Fairbank, 1993, and Kilpatrick &
Resnick, 1993). In the St. Louis ECA (Helzer el al., 1987), it
was found that participants with PTSD were 1.6 times more
likely than those without PTSD to be diagnosed with a previous
or concurrent alcohol abuse disorder. The Mount St. Helens Epidemiological Study similarly found a strong association between PTSD and alcohol abuse diagnoses: 27% of those with
PTSD were diagnosed with a concurrent alcohol abuse disorder
(cited in Davidson & Fairbank, 1993). Another national probability sample study examined the relationship between PTSD
and several indices of alcohol abuse (e.g., car accident, blackout, guzzling for effect, and drinking first thing in the morning).
Participants who currently met criteria for PTSD scored higher
on all the measured indices of alcohol abuse than controls without PTSD (cited in Kilpatrick & Resnick, 1993). Finally, a
study by Breslau and Davis (1992) divided a young adult com-
munity sample meeting criteria for PTSD into those with
chronic versus nonchronic PTSD: Participants in the chronic
group were 2.7 times more likely than those in the nonchronic
group to meet diagnostic criteria for alcohol abuse--dependence, although both groups had a higher prevalence of these
alcohol disorders than a matched control group without PTSD
(Breslau, Davis, Andreski, & Peterson, 1991 ). Thus, severity of
PTSD symptoms (when defined in terms ofchronicity) appears
positively associated with comorbid alcoholism. On the basis of
these epidemiologic data, Davidson and Fairbank and Kilpatrick and Resnick have concluded that there is a high rate of
comorbidity between PTSD and alcohol abuse across a variety
of traumatic events.
Another study replicates and extends these epidemiological
findings. Warshaw et al. (1993) examined the prevalence of
trauma exposure, PTSD, and other diagnoses in a sample of
patients with anxiety disorder. On the basis of SCID interviews,
researchers divided patients into one of three groups: trauma
exposed with PTSD, trauma exposed without PTSD, and no
history of trauma exposure. The highest rates of alcohol abusedependence were seen in the group with PTSD and the lowest
rates in the group without a history of trauma exposure, although all groups showed higher rates of alcohol abuse-dependence than rates seen in the general population (Robins et al.,
1984). Thus, exposure to trauma in general, and the presence
of PTSD in particular, appears to increase the risk for an alcohol disorder, over and above the risk associated with the presence ofanotber anxiety disorder (see Pihl & Stewart, 199 l, for a
review of the relationship between anxiety disorders, other than
PTSD, and alcohol abuse).
Summary. Evidence suggests that exposure to a wide variety of trauma frequently leads to the development of alcohol
abuse. Moreover, some data support the notion that trauma severity is positively associated with level of alcohol abuse. Finally, it appears that psychological response to trauma (i.e.,
PTSD) may be more highly associated with alcohol abuse than
exposure to trauma or the severity of trauma exposure, per se.
M e t h o d o l o g i c a l C r i t i q u e o f C o m o r b i d i t y Studies
In this section, methodological issues in the comorbidity
studies are critiqued. This critique considers methodological issues related to operational definitions of alcoholism, trauma,
and PTSD and the methods for assessing these clinical conditions, sample selection concerns, inclusion of control groups,
statistical control of third variables, and specificity of overlap
of alcoholism, trauma, and PTSD. All of these issues must be
considered when planning future studies examining the comorbidity of trauma, PTSD, and alcoholism. This section concludes with a consideration of factors that may contribute to
the significant variability in comorbidity rates across studies.
Specifically, I review evidence suggesting that the nature of the
traumatic event, certain individual difference variables, or both
might be related to the development of alcohol abuse or PTSD
plus alcohol abuse following exposure to a traumatic event.
Operational Definitions and Assessment Issues
Although research suggests that exposure to traumatic events
frequently overlaps with alcohol problems, and that the diagno-
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
ses of PTSD and alcohol abuse frequently co-occur, the rates of
overlap vary markedly across studies. Factors that may contribute to this variability include differences in the definitions of
alcoholism, trauma, and PTSD and differences in the way they
were assessed across studies.
Alcoholism. A major methodological limitation in early
prevalence studies was the frequent omission of the operational
criteria used to assess alcoholism (Yeary, 1982). When such
criteria were reported, they varied considerably across studies.
Criteria for defining alcohol abuse-dependence have included
attendance at Alcoholics Anonymous meetings (e.g., Kovach,
1986), changes in overall rates of alcohol consumption in regions exposed to disaster (e.g., Gleser et al., 1981 ), a variety of
social indices of alcohol abuse (e.g., E R. Adams & Adams,
1984), scores on self-report measures of alcohol-related problems (e.g., McFall et al., 1992), and the use of D S M - I I I or
D S M - I I I - R criteria for alcohol abuse-dependence. Rates of
comorbid alcoholism and PTSD obtained using D S M - I I I criteria are difficult to compare with those obtained using D S M I I I - R criteria because the latter place less emphasis on physiological tolerance and withdrawal than did the former (Nace,
1988). A return to an emphasis on symptoms of tolerance and
withdrawal in D S M - I V may present even further difficulties in
comparing comorbidity rates across studies in the future. In addition, a wide variety of methods for establishing diagnoses have
been used, again making comparisons across studies difficult.
These include a number of structured interviews, such as the
SADS (Endicott & Spitzer, 1978; e.g., Davidson et al., 1985)
and SCID (Spitzer et al., 1987; e.g., Green et al., 1992), and
self-report instruments, such as the MAST (Seizer, 197 l; e.g.,
McFall et al., 1992). The use of more consistent methodology
in terms of establishing alcoholic status is essential.
Moreover, problems are evident in the retrospective case report methodology used in several previous studies. Indeed, not
all patient charts are compiled in the same way, and not all clinicians record the same type of information in charts. Thus, the
results of studies using such methods (e.g., Streimer et al.,
1985 ) may represent conservative estimates of the rates of alcoholism in traumatized samples (Brinson & Treanor, 1989).
Trauma exposure. Similarly, the definitions of traumatic
events, and the methods by which exposure to traumatic events
has been established, have varied markedly across studies. This
issue is particularly evident within the area of abuse. The definitions of abuse used across studies have ranged from childhood
incest (e.g., Kovach, 1986) to broader definitions of abuse including childhood neglect (e.g., Cohen & Densen-Gerber,
1982). Until researchers reach a consensus on the operational
definition of abuse, studies on the relationship between abuse
and alcohol problems must be evaluated in terms of the specific
definition of abuse used within each study (Briere, 1992).
In addition to the definitional confusion, the methods by
which abuse histories have been established have also varied
widely across studies. There is a need for the development of
validated psychometric instruments to assess prior exposure to
abuse. The Trauma Symptom Checklist-40 (TSC-40; Elliott &
Briere, 1992) is an instrument that appears promising in this
regard. In a national survey of approximately 3,000 professional women, the TSC-40 was found to be a reliable and valid
89
measure of childhood sexual victimization (Elliott & Briere,
1992).
There are two potential biases that may operate when participants provide self-reports of their prior exposure to certain
traumatic events. First is the possibility of overreporting. Asking participants to describe their traumatic experience through
the use of leading questions or "probes" may increase the likelihood that they report experiencing a trauma that they did not
experience, leading to an overestimate of the prevalence of traumatic experiences. In fact, the use of probes has been shown to
increase the frequency of reporting of sexual abuse (Fullilove
et al., 1993). This issue appears particularly important given
emerging concerns about the possibility o f a "thlse memory syndrome." This syndrome involves the notion that some individuals, when searching for an explanation for their current psychological problems, may come to recall a traumatic childhood
abuse history that may not have actually occurred. These false
memories might represent either intentional falsifications or
unintentional memory distortions (see Loftus, 1993; Loftus,
Garry, & Feldman, 1994; and L. M. Williams, 1994a, 1994b,
for reviews of the relevant controversy).
Second is the possibility that participants may underreport
their actual levels of exposure to certain traumatic events because of feelings of shame, fear, or discomfort with the subject
matter, lack of trust in the interviewer, reticence to share personal information with a stranger (Fullilove et al., 1993 ), or all
of the above. Researchers have argued that this reporting bias is
characteristic of individuals exposed to abuse (e.g., Briere &
Zaidi, 1989), but it might also be characteristic of individuals
exposed to other forms of trauma. For example, political refugees might be reluctant to report experiences of torture due to
their vulnerable political immigrant status (APA, 1994).
Briere (1992) has reviewed three methods that researchers
use to increase the validity of self-reports on abuse histories: (a)
obtain independent corroboration of abuse reports from other
sources (e.g., family and friends; see also Frankel, 1993); (b)
use only cases validated by the child protection system or justice
system; and (c) attempt to decrease rewards for falsifying abuse
histories (e.g., avoiding the use of differential payment when
recruiting research participants exposed to abuse). Although
useful in allaying concerns about false memory syndrome, the
first two methods have some associated practical problems. Individuals exposed to abuse may be unwilling or unable to provide corroborating evidence of their abuse histories. Limiting
study to only those cases identified by the child protection or
justice systems may introduce sampling biases (e.g., selecting
for the most severe cases). Given these problems, Briere has
suggested that, instead, researchers should include in their studies other measures relevant to reporting bias (e.g., social desirability, tendency toward repression, and attitudes about
disclosure) and that they should more fully investigate the testretest reliability of abuse reports. In addition, he noted that this
research area could benefit from an increased focus on the potential role of interview variables in determining the frequency
and extent of participants' abuse reports. For example, attention should be given to the context of the interview (part of a
clinical vs. research evaluation), the nature and length of the
interview relationship (long-term therapy vs. one-session
evaluation), and the wording and specificity of the questioning
90
STEWART
(how abuse is defined for the participant). These issues are worthy of careful consideration when planning future research to
investigate the overlap between certain forms of traumatic experience (e.g., sexual abuse, rape, or political torture) and subsequent alcohol abuse.
PTSD. Caution should also be noted about the definitions
of PTSD and the methods by which PTSD diagnoses are established across studies. For example, a variety of instruments have
been used in studies using structured interviews. Previous research has shown, for example, that the SCID (Spitzer et al.,
1987) and DIS (Robins et al., 1981 ) elicit markedly different
PTSD prevalence rates. In fact, the sensitivity of the DIS has
been questioned (e.g., Davidson & Fairbank, 1993). The
NVVRS showed that only one in five cases of PTSD diagnosed
by SCID criteria was accurately identified by the DIS (Kulka et
al., 1988). Therefore, in studies that have used DIS criteria to
establish comorbidity (e.g., Winfield et al., 1990), the overlap
between alcohol abuse and PTSD may have been underestimated. The use of both current and lifetime diagnostic assessments is preferable, given the possibility that current PTSD
symptoms might be masked if the individual is abusing alcohol
at the time of assessment (e.g., Brett & Ostroff, 1985 ), resulting
in underestimates of the actual overlap between PTSD and
alcoholism.
Some studies (e.g., McFall et al., 1992) have relied on selfreport scales alone (e.g., M - P T S D scale; Keane, Caddell, et al.,
1988) to assess PTSD. Although such instruments may be useful indicators of the severity of PTSD symptoms, they should
not be used as the sole method of diagnosing PTSD. Lyons,
Gerardi, Wolfe, and Keane (1988), for example, advocate the
importance of multimodal assessment techniques for establishing the diagnosis of PTSD.
Sample Selection Concerns
Studies using psychiatric outpatients as participants may underestimate levels of alcohol abuse. First, such patients may be
reluctant to admit to abusive drinking because of concerns
about potential ineligibility for anxiety-focused or traumafocused treatment. Second, individuals with significant alcohol
problems may seek treatment at an addictions facility, rather
than at a psychiatric outpatient clinic (B. J. Cox et al., 1990;
Otto, Pollack, Sachs, O'Neil, & Rosenbaum, 1992). Alternatively, studies conducted with patients in treatment may be subject to "Berkson's" selection bias (i.e., individuals with more
than one disorder may be more likely to seek treatment than
individuals with only one disorder). This bias could inflate comorbidity rates obtained with clinical samples above rates present in the general population. Studies using nonclinical samples
have overcome these potential selection biases. In fact, the
strong relationship of trauma exposure, PTSD, and alcohol
abuse appears to be evident in both clinical and nonclinical
samples. However, researchers should be aware of these selection bias issues when designing future studies.
The large majority of comorbidity studies have been conducted with circumscribed samples of individuals exposed to
particular forms of traumatic events, rendering it difficult to
make general statements about the relationship of trauma
exposure, PTSD, and alcoholism across traumatic events. The
relations between some forms of traumatic events (e.g., manmade disasters) and alcohol abuse remain relatively understudied. However, the literature from studies conducted within a variety of specific traumatic events, and the emergence of epidemiological studies in the general population that examine
exposure to a wide variety of traumatic experiences, provides
preliminary data that the relationship between PTSD and alcohol abuse can be found across several traumatic experiences
(for reviews, see Davidson & Fairbank, 1993, and Kilpatrick &
Resnick, 1993). I consider the potential impact of different
types of traumatic events on subsequent abusive drinking in a
later section, Nature of the traumatic event.
Control groups. Another limitation in early comorbidity
studies was the general absence of matched control groups. In
these studies, it was unclear whether traumatic histories, PTSD
diagnoses, or both were more common in alcoholic samples
than in nonalcoholic samples matched for important demographic variables, such as age and SES. However, more recently
researchers (e.g., Covington, 1983) have made headway in establishing a relationship between childhood abuse trauma and
later alcoholism through the inclusion of appropriately
matched control groups.
Statistical control of third variables. Even with the use of
control groups, the question remains whether trauma exposure
is causally antecedent to alcohol abuse or whether other variables (such as the impact of other events before or following the
trauma) are responsible for the overlap between trauma
exposure and alcohol abuse. For example, family dysfunction
(broadly defined) and familial alcoholism have been suggested
as potential third variables which may be responsible for the
high degree of overlap between childhood sexual abuse, physical abuse, or both and alcohol problems in adulthood
(American Medical Association, 1993 ). Some researchers have
used statistical methods to control for the possible mediating
effects of third variables on the relationship between trauma
exposure and subsequent alcohol abuse. For example, B. A.
Miller et al. ( 1993 ) showed that the relationship between childhood victimization and alcohol abuse persisted after the effects
of family background (e.g., parental alcohol problems) were
statistically controlled.
However, Briere and Elliott (1993) have questioned the validity of such statistical control procedures, cautioning that such
analyses are often misleading in quasi-experimental research
(i.e., research involving comparisons between nonrandomly assigned participants--traumatized vs. nontraumatized). They
suggested alternatives to statistical control procedures for controlling for third variables, including multivariate tests such as
canonical correlation analysis or causal modeling. Some researchers have begun to use such techniques in the study of the
relationship between trauma and alcohol abuse. The study by
Green et al. (1989) is a fine example of the use of multivariate
techniques to assess the relative contributions of trauma
exposure, pretraumatic factors, and posttraumatic factors to
the prediction of alcohol abuse in combat veterans. Similarly,
Dembo and colleagues (e.g., Dembo et al., 1988, 1992) have
effectively used structural equation modeling techniques to
study the potential causal relationship of abuse histories, other
family background characteristics, and the development of alcohol problems in adolescents. Such research has demonstrated
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
that trauma exposure contributes unique variance to the prediction of alcohol abuse, over and above the effects of other relevant variables.
Specificity of overlap. Although the prevalence of traumatic
histories among people with alcoholism appears significantly
higher than rates in the general population (Hurley, 1991 ), if
anything is to be made etiologically of these statistics, it must be
demonstrated that traumatic histories are specifically associated with alcoholism diagnoses, as opposed to being more generally associated with a wide variety of behavioral pathologies.
This type of discrimination is seldom made and is seldom clear.
Some researchers have attempted to address this issue. For example, Briere (1988) has shown that not only do individuals
exposed to childhood abuse show higher rates of alcoholism
than do other patients, but they also display significantly more
insomnia, sexual dysfunction, dissociation, anger, suicidality,
self-mutilation, and other forms of drug addiction (see also a
review by Herman, 1992). These results suggest that alcohol
abuse may be merely one type of maladaptive response to dealing with traumatic experiences. However, other researchers have
addressed the question of specificity by examining the relative
overlap of PTSD and other diagnoses including alcohol abusedependence. For example, Branchey et al. (1984) found that
alcohol abuse and PTSD are the most prevalent coexisting psychiatric diagnoses in combat veterans. Similarly, in NVVRS
(Kulka et al., 1988), conducted with a nonclinical sample of
Vietnam veterans, alcohol abuse was the most common comorbid disorder among men with PTSD, and the third most common comorbid diagnosis among women with PTSD. Thus, alcohol abuse does appear to show some specificity in terms of its
overlap with PTSD, at least in veteran samples. This issue is
worthy of further study, particularly with respect to other forms
of traumatic events.
Nature of the traumatic event. Some studies suggest that the
rates of PTSD in individuals exposed to trauma may vary as a
function of the nature of the traumatic event. For example,
PTSD rates are higher in individuals who have been exposed
to heavy combat (Kulka et al., 1988) and sexual assault
(Rothbaum & Foa, 1993) than in individuals who have been
involved in serious accidents (Helzer et al., 1987 ). Researchers
have only just begun to examine whether there might be similar
differences in rates of alcohol abuse as a function of the nature
of the traumatic event. For example, the community study by
Cottler et al. (1992) showed heavy alcohol use to be significantly
associated with prior exposure to combat but not to other forms
of traumatic events. Similarly, Green et al. (1992) made a post
hoc comparison of data from two comorbidity studies and
showed that comorbid alcoholism and PTSD diagnoses were
higher in a sample of combat veterans than in a sample of individuals exposed to a natural disaster. However, methodological
limitations preclude interpreting these results as firmly supporting the notion that combat is more likely to lead to alcohol
abuse plus PTSD than is a natural disaster. Differences in the
methodologies used in the two studies, and differences in demographic characteristics of the samples, might explain the observed variability in comorbidity rates (Green et al., 1992).
First, the study with combat veterans (Green et al., 1989) used
the SADS (Endicott & Spitzer, 1978) to establish diagnoses,
whereas the study with individuals exposed to a flood (Green et
91
al., 1992) used the SCID (Spitzer et al., 1987), thus making
direct comparisons across the two studies difficult to interpret
given the slightly different diagnostic criteria used on the SADS
and SCID (Green et al., 1992). Second, the combat veterans
sample was younger than the disaster sample, and the former
was composed only of men, whereas the latter was a mixed gender group. Because younger ages and the male gender have been
associated with increased rates of alcohol abuse-dependence in
the general population (e.g., Robins et al., 1984), such differences are important to consider before any definitive conclusions about the relative degree of overlap of PTSD and alcohol
abuse across various trauma can be made. Age and gender are
considered further in the following section on the potential impact of various individual difference factors.
Individual Differences
Traditionally, researchers have examined a number of individual difference factors to determine whether such variables
influence the development of PTSD or alcoholism (e.g., W. M.
Cox, 1987; McNally & Shin, 1995). However, few researchers
have focused on individual difference factors that might contribute to the development of alcohol problems, or comorbid
PTSD and alcoholism, following exposure to a traumatic event.
Several such possible individual difference factors are reviewed
here.
Age. Age must be considered as a potentially important individual difference variable, both in terms of the age of the participant at the time of study and the age at which the participant
was exposed to the traumatic event. There are well-known
problems inherent in retrospective reporting. Studies with
adults suffer the limitation that participants are often asked to
report on traumatic events that may have occurred a long time
in the past (e.g., childhood abuse). Work with adolescents (e.g.,
C l a r k & Jacob, 1992; Dembo et al., 1988, 1992) may, to some
degree, overcome this limitation by examining the participants'
reports of the traumatic events closer in time to the occurrence.
Recent research suggests that a substantial proportion of
adult individuals exposed to childhood sexual abuse (particularly those with histories of more severe forms of abuse)
report some prior period of amnesia for the abuse (i.e., Bernet,
Deutscher, Ingram, & Litrownik, 1993; Briere & Conte, 1993;
Herman & Schatzow, 1987) and that this amnesia is likely to
have occurred in adolescence (Briere & Conte, 1993). These
findings, however, are directly contradicted by the work of Terr
(1981, 1983) with the children involved in the Chowchilla
school bus kidnapping. In contrast to Briere and Conte's findings of significant amnesia for traumatic events in adolescence,
Terr found that the participants' memories for the traumatic
event remained intact and detailed both immediately following
the kidnapping (when they were children) and at a 4-year follow-up (when they were adolescents). These two types of traumatic events (kidnapping vs. sexual abuse) may differ in important respects, however. Terr (1991) distinguished between the
effects of a single traumatic event (e.g., kidnapping) and those
o f prolonged, repeated trauma (e.g., sexual abuse). According
to Terr ( 1991 ), the latter type of trauma is more likely than the
former to be associated with denial, numbing, and dissociative
types of reactions in children (see also Herman, 1992). Until
92
STEWART
this issue is further clarified, researchers need to carefully consider the possibility of amnesia for certain types of traumatic
events when examining the relationship between self-reported
traumatic histories and alcohol problems in adolescent
samples.
With respect to the age at which the traumatic event occurred, it might be speculated that the earlier the traumatic
exposure, the less likely drinking problems will develop because
adults might be more likely to use alcohol as a coping strategy
than children. However, Swett et al. ( 1991 ) showed that even
early exposure to trauma is associated with high rates of alcohol
abuse in adulthood. Kovach (1986) has postulated a delayedonset PTSD model to explain these findings. She suggested that
the link between childhood abuse and adult alcohol problems
might be mediated by a delayed onset of PTSD symptoms in
adulthood with which the individuals attempt to cope through
alcohol abuse.
Gender. Epidemiological evidence indicates that men are
approximately five to seven times more likely to receive a lifetime diagnosis of alcohol abuse-dependence than are women
(Robins et al., 1984). Thus, in future studies designed to compare the incidence of alcohol abuse in patients with PTSD
across specific trauma, gender differences in alcoholism diagnoses in the general population should be considered. For example, should it be found that PTSD due to combat is more highly
associated with subsequent alcohol abuse than PTSD following
sexual assault, it should be kept in mind that these two types of
trauma tend also to occur disproportionately across gender,
with men more likely to be exposed to combat and women more
likely to experience sexual assault (Norris, 1992). Thus, comparison of rates of alcohol abuse in patients with PTSD must be
made with base rates by gender before any definitive statements
can be made about the relative prevalences of comorbid PTSD
and alcohol abuse in individuals who have been exposed to various types of trauma (Green et al., 1992).
Cultural and racial factors. Rates of alcohol abuse-dependence vary across cultures. These prevalence differences appear
related to each culture's acceptance of alcohol use (APA,
1994). However, few researchers have focused on how these cultural norms might relate to the likelihood of using alcohol to
cope with exposure to traumatic events. Cross-cultural studies
have shown that the basic symptom patterns of PTSD appear
constant across cultures (e.g., Carlson & Rosser-Hogan, 1994).
Similar cross-cultural studies on posttraumatic changes in
drinking would be useful to determine the influence of cultural
variables on the likelihood of developing drinking problems following trauma exposure.
Previous data suggest that racial factors may be related to the
likelihood of developing PTSD following exposure to trauma.
For example, in the NVVRS (Kulka et al., 1988), rates of
PTSD in Vietnam veterans were highest in Hispanic veterans
and lowest in Caucasian veterans. Whether these effects represent true racial differences, or rather differences in severity of
combat exposure, remains to be determined (Kulka et al.,
1988). Some data similarly suggest that overlapping rates of
PTSD plus alcoholism may vary as a function of racial factors.
For example, Escobar et al. (1983) showed much higher levels
of comorbid PTSD and alcohol abuse-dependence diagnoses
(i.e., 80%) by SCID criteria in a Hispanic sample of Vietnam
combat veterans than those previously established in non-Hispanic or mixed racial samples of veterans (e.g., Sierles et al.,
1983). However, because the Escobar et al. study did not include other racial groups matched with the Hispanic sample for
important demographic variables, such as SES, the conclusion
that Hispanic veterans are at increased risk for comorbid
PTSD-alcoholism remains speculative.
Intelligence and cognitive factors. Recent data suggest an association between intelligence levels and the development of
PTSD in a sample of Vietnam veterans (McNally & Shin,
1995 ). Specifically, researchers found that veterans with lower
IQs were more likely than those with higher IQs to develop
PTSD. Although these data are correlational in nature, one of
the potential explanations for the overlap is that cognitive deficits may be a risk factor for the development of PTSD following
trauma exposure. If cognitive disadvantage leads to impaired
coping abilities, then lower levels of intelligence may complicate
recovery. Alcohol abuse might be viewed as a maladaptive coping strategy for dealing with extremely stressful experiences.
Thus, lower intelligence levels might be one individual difference factor that predisposes to alcohol abuse following trauma
exposure.
Personality factors. Previous research has attempted to determine whether certain personality factors might be related to
the development of alcohol abuse (for a review, see W. M. Cox,
1987). One often-researched factor is trait anxiety, the tendency to react with fear across a wide variety of situations. Although it makes intuitive sense that individual differences in
trait anxiety might be related to whether a given individual
would turn to alcohol following exposure to a traumatic event,
research shows only weak and inconsistent relations between
trait anxiety and alcohol abuse (Sher, 1987).
In contrast, work on "anxiety sensitivity" might potentially
contribute to understanding the relationship between PTSD
and alcohol abuse. Anxiety sensitivity is a cognitive individual
difference variable that is both conceptually and empirically
distinct from the traditional concept of trait anxiety (McNally,
1996). Anxiety sensitivity is characterized by fear of anxiety
symptoms, based on beliefs that these symptoms have harmful
consequences (Reiss, 1987; Reiss & McNally, 1985). An individual with high-anxiety sensitivity, for example, may misinterpret rapid heart rate as a sign of an impending heart attack,
whereas an individual with low-anxiety sensitivity may regard
these sensations as merely unpleasant.
Several recent studies focus on the potentially important relationship between anxiety sensitivity levels and the use or abuse
of alcohol (McNally, 1996; Peterson & Reiss, 1992; Reiss, 1991;
Stewart, 1995a). For example, higher than normative levels of
anxiety sensitivity are characteristic of individuals meeting
DSM-III-R criteria for alcohol dependence (Karp, 1993).
Anxiety sensitivity levels are positively correlated with selfreported, weekly alcohol use both in clinical (B. J. Cox, Swinson, Shulman, Kuch, & Reichman, 1993) and in nonclinical
(Stewart, Peterson, & Pihl, 1995) samples. Levels of anxiety
sensitivity are significantly positively related to the use of alcohol for coping-related motives (i.e., to reduce or avoid negative
emotional states; e.g., Stewart, Karp, Pihl, & Peterson, 1995;
Stewart & Zeitlin, 1995). Karp has also shown that levels of
anxiety sensitivity are positively associated with the belief that
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
alcohol consumption leads to relaxation and tension reduction.
Moreover, anxiety sensitivity levels are strongly positively correlated with the self-reported frequency of drinking in situations
involving unpleasant emotions and conflict with others (i.e.,
potentially negatively reinforcing drinking situations; Stewart,
1995b). Finally, a series of studies by Stewart and colleagues has
shown that anxiety sensitive participants displayed responses to
alcohol administration in the laboratory that could potentially
explain their increased weekly use of alcohol: High-anxiety sensitive participants are particularly sensitive to the subjectiveemotional, physiological, and cognitive tension-reducing effects
of alcohol when compared with low-anxiety sensitive controls
(Stewart & Pihl, 1994, 1995).
Anxiety sensitivity may be one common factor contributing
to the high degree of overlap between PTSD and alcohol abuse.
There exists some initial, albeit indirect, evidence to support
this contention. First, anxiety sensitivity, PTSD, and some
forms of alcohol abuse (i.e., secondary or "neurotic" forms;
W. M. Cox, 1987) are all characterized by avoidance behavior
(e.g., Reiss, Peterson, Gursky, & McNally, 1986). Second, not
all anxiety disorders are associated with elevated anxiety sensitivity. Significant fear of anxiety appears to figure prominently
in individuals with PTSD and panic disorder, whereas individuals with simple phobias, generalized anxiety disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder display more normative levels of
anxiety sensitivity (McNally, 1992; Stewart, Knize, & Pihl,
1992; S. Taylor, Koch, & McNally, 1992). An increased risk for
alcoholism appears associated with the anxiety disorders characterized by high levels of anxiety sensitivity, such as panic disorder (see a review by B. J. Cox et al., 1990) and PTSD. In
contrast, anxiety disorders, such as a simple phobia, generalized
anxiety disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder that are
characterized by lower levels of anxiety sensitivity, do not appear to be associated with an increased risk for alcoholism
(Kushner et al., 1990; Riemann, McNally, & Cox, 1992). The
potential role of anxiety sensitivity in mediating the relationship between PTSD and alcohol abuse requires further
exploration.
93
an etiological factor, maintenance factor, or both for the other.
Then I review studies that have extended traditional epidemiological comorbidity methods to examine the functional associations between these two disorders.
Possible Causal Relationships
Several causal pathways may explain the co-occurrence of
PTSD and alcoholism (Pihl & Stewart, 1991 ). These possibilities are not necessarily mutually exclusive. First, alcohol abuse
might heighten the susceptibility to the development, maintenance, or both of PTSD. Alcohol abuse might increase anxiety
and arousal levels through psychological processes, such as
stressful life events occurring due to heavy drinking or guilt associated with heavy drinking (B. J. Cox, Norton, Dorward, &
Fergusson, 1989; Kushner et al., 1990). Alcohol abuse might
also increase anxiety and arousal levels through physiological
processes, such as prolonged drinking effects, acute ingestion of
very high doses of alcohol, or alcohol withdrawal effects (e.g.,
LaCoursiere, Godfrey, & Ruby, 1980). These effects could serve
to induce a hyperaroused state in which the individual may be
more vulnerable to develop PTSD following a traumatic event.
Alcohol abuse might also serve to maintain PTSD by interfering with important psychological mechanisms of processing or
"working through" the traumatic experience or by preventing
desensitization to traumatic events (Herman, 1992). Second,
PTSD might be involved in the development, maintenance, or
both of alcohol abuse. Some patients with PTSD might begin
or continue abusing alcohol in an attempt to reduce or control
(i.e., self-medicate for) their PTSD symptoms. This self-medication notion of drug abuse proposes that individuals who are
susceptible to certain aversive states, such as anxiety, are at a
high risk of abusing drugs capable of reducing these aversive
states (Khantzian, 1985 ). Third, some other variable (e.g., anxiety sensitivity; McNally, 1996) might be related to the onset,
maintenance, or both of PTSD and alcohol abuse. Thus, similar
etiological mechanisms involved in the two disorders might explain their high degree of overlap, rather than one disorder causing the other.
Summary
Future comorbidity studies should carefully consider various
methodological issues, including the definitions and choice of
instruments for assessing trauma exposure, PTSD, and alcohol
abuse-dependence, and inclusion of appropriate control
groups, to increase the quality of research in this area and thus
allow researchers to draw more firm conclusions. Researchers
should also pay more attention to factors that may potentially
affect comorbidity rates across studies, such as trauma-related
and individual difference variables. Moreover, researchers may
wish to go beyond traditional comorbidity research methods
and begin to probe the functional relationship of trauma
exposure, PTSD, and alcoholism.
Functional Associations
This section begins with a review of several possible causal
relationships that may explain the overlap between PTSD and
alcoholism. I consider ways in which each disorder may serve as
Studies of Functional Connectedness
The data on the comorbidity between PTSD and alcohol
abuse presented in earlier sections of this article are correlational and do not necessarily provide evidence for a functional
interplay between the two disorders. To overcome this limitation, several studies have examined the relative order of onset of
the comorbid diagnoses of PTSD and alcoholism and patients'
perceptions of the relationship between their problems, in attempts to assess the direction of causality.
Order of onset. Although not specifically examining the diagnosis of PTSD, Winfield et al. (1990) provided evidence that
sexual assault tends to precede alcohol abuse in women. In a
large community sample of women who were both sexually assaulted and diagnosed with alcohol abuse-dependence, drinking problems developed following the assault. None of the
women had a drinking problem before the assault.
In a sample of adolescents with alcoholism, Clark and Jacob
(1992) found that anxiety disorders appeared before alcohol
94
STEWART
abuse in 85% of the comorbid cases. Unfortunately, order of
onset data were not reported separately for each anxiety disorder. Based on the statistics provided, however, it can be estimated that in the large majority (i.e., 75-100%) of the PTSD
cases, PTSD developed before the alcohol disorder. However, the
relatively small sample size in this study precludes firm
conclusions.
Davidson et al. (1985) studied a sample of Vietnam veterans
with PTSD. They found that in the cases comorbid for PTSD
and alcohol or drug abuse, PTSD developed 31/2years before the
onset of the substance abuse disorder, on average. Similarly, in a
study of World War 2 ( W W 2 ) and Vietnam veterans with
PTSD (Davidson et al., 1990) in cases comorbid for alcohol
abuse and PTSD, the alcohol abuse did not develop until approximately 7 years after the emergence of PTSD in the WW2
veterans group and tended to emerge around the same time as
PTSD in the Vietnam veterans group. However, ',he studies
above (i.e., Clark & Jacob, 1992; Davidson et al., 1985, 1990)
examined the relative order of onset of diagnoses, not the onset
of subthreshold symptoms. It is possible that some veterans in
the Vietnam group (Davidson et al., 1990) began abusing alcohol to dampen subthreshold PTSD symptoms before the onset
of full-blown PTSD. Davidson et al. (1990) noted that the earlier onset of alcoholism in the Vietnam as opposed to the WW2
veterans group may reflect the greater availability of alcohol at
the time.
In contrast to these results, Cottler et al. (1992) found that
alcohol abuse symptoms generally predated the onset of PTSD
symptoms in nonclinical participants from the St. Louis ECA
study. However, this study used very liberal criteria for defining
alcoholproblems, including individuals who drank heavily once
a wee.k for at least 2 months, with no objective definition of
heavy drinking provided. Because alcohol problems were not
adequately diagnosed, differences in results across studies that
examine the relative order of onset may be attributable to
differences in diagnostic criteria. Moreover, Cottler et al. examined the relative onset of subthreshold symptoms, whereas the
previously cited studies examined the point at which participants met diagnostic criteria for PTSD and alcoholism. Thus,
these two findings are not necessarily mutually exclusive. For
example, an individual could be a heavy drinker on weekends
and experience only subthreshold symptoms of alcohol abuse
or dependence. If this individual were to experience a traumatic
event and develop PTSD symptoms, he or she might be predisposed to use alcohol to cope with or dampen PTSD symptoms
and over time develop an alcohol disorder.
With the exception of Cottler et a l ' s (1992) study, the majority of data support the notion that PTSD symptoms tend to
precede the development of alcohol abuse problems. However,
the mere fact that one disorder tends to precede the other does
not establish a causal link between the two problems. Rachman
(1991) has recommended several strategies for overcoming
problems inherent in traditional comorbidity research (i.e., observed high-comorbidity rates may be merely statistical relationships rather than functional associations). He suggested
that, in addition to the simple determination of the rates of cooccurrence, attention should be given to the psychological connectedness of the two disorders. This can be assessed in a variety
of ways, including having participants estimate the degree to
which they consider their two problems to be associated.
Patients'perceptions ofassociations. Some researchers have
applied Rachman's ( 1991 ) suggestion that patients be queried
as to their perceptions regarding the functional interplay between their PTSD symptoms and their drinking problems. For
example, Clark and Jacob (1992) found that in the majority of
cases comorbid for anxiety and alcohol abuse disorders, participants perceived that the anxiety disorder was causally linked to
the development of the alcohol abuse problem. Although the
data were not presented as a function of the type of anxiety
disorder, PTSD was by far the most common comorbid anxiety
disorder in this sample. Thus, the Clark and Jacob data provided evidence that patients do perceive a causal connection
between PTSD and the later development of their drinking
problems. Therefore, like the data on chronological patterns,
initial results on perceived relationships provide preliminary
evidence that anxiety disorders, particularly PTSD, may contribute to the development of alcohol disorders.
More research on participants' perceptions of the causal links
between their PTSD and their alcohol abuse symptoms is
needed to examine the functional relationship between the two
problems. However, as Rachman (1991 ) cautioned, such selfestimates may bear little relationship to the actual functional
connectedness of the two problems because individuals are not
always aware of the motives for their behaviors (McClelland,
1985; Nisbett & Wilson, 1977 ). Rachman recommended that,
in addition to having participants make general estimates of
connectedness, in the future researchers should use questions
probing participants' predictions of what would happen to one
problem if the other problem were to increase or to decrease.
Such research has yet to be conducted with respect to comorbid
PTSD and alcoholism.
Drinking patterns. Some researchers have examined the
patterns of drinking behavior in patients with PTSD to better
understand the functional relationship between these two problems. Jellinek and Williams (1984) reported that combat veterans exhibit unusually high levels of alcohol consumption and
engage in continuous, sustained drinking patterns, as well as
frequent binge drinking. In contrast, Boscarino ( 1981 ) showed
that Vietnam combat veterans exhibited only substantially
higher rates of binge drinking than did Vietnam-era veterans
after statistically controlling for important demographic differences between the two groups. This is consistent with the belief
among many clinicians that combat veterans abuse alcohol in
an episodic, rather than habitual, manner (Hyer, Leach, Boudewyns, & Davis, 1991 ).
Hyer et al. ( 1991 ) specifically examined the typical drinking
patterns of Vietnam veterans with alcoholism as a function of
the presence or absence of concomitant PTSD. All veterans
were on a chemical dependency treatment unit, and all had a
current DSM-III-R diagnosis of alcohol abuse. The PTSD
group was identified according to cutoff scores on the M M P I PTSD scale (Keane, Malloy, & Fairbank, 1984). All veterans
were given the Alcohol Use Inventory (AUI; Horn, Wanberg, &
Foster, 1987), a measure of alcohol use patterns. The group
with PTSD achieved significantly higher scores on the AUI than
the group without PTSD, reflecting an overall profile of deteriorated binge drinking patterns in veterans with PTSD.
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
Thus, preliminary data suggest that war veterans comorbid
for PTSD and alcoholism tend to be binge drinkers. Hyer et
al. ( 1991 ) have speculated that the episodic heavy drinking of
combat veterans with PTSD may coincide with periods of intrusive PTSD symptoms. Although McFall et al. (1992) demonstrated that levels of problem drinking (i.e., MAST scores)
were significantly positively correlated with the severity of intrusive PTSD symptoms, no researchers have yet examined
whether heavy drinking episodes actually overlap in time with
the occurrence of intrusive PTSD symptoms.
LaCoursiere et al. (1980) provided a series of case studies
containing some preliminary observations regarding the functional relationship between PTSD symptoms and abusive
drinking. They presented three case examples of war veterans
with comorbid diagnoses of PTSD and alcohol abuse. They
noted that these patients tended to consume large amounts of
alcohol just before bed, reportedly to prevent sleep difficulties
and the experience of trauma-related nightmares. More research in this area is warranted and should be extended to examine individuals with PTSD due to other trauma, such as sexual or physical abuse. Both retrospective and prospective selfreport methodologies (e.g., daily diaries of PTSD symptoms
and drinking behaviors) could be used in future research to further evaluate the typical drinking profiles of patients with and
without PTSD, the situations in which heavy drinking occurs,
patients' perceptions of their motivations for abusive drinking,
and the behavioral consequences of heavy drinking. Examination of the relation between anniversary dates (around which
patients' reexperiencing symptoms are often exacerbated; APA,
1994) and alcohol use might provide important information
about the functional relationships between PTSD symptoms
and abusive binge drinking.
Summary
Studies of both order of onset and patients' perceptions of the
causal connection between their PTSD symptoms and alcohol
abuse suggest an important functional relationship. PTSD
symptoms tend to precede the onset of problem drinking, and
patients' perceive that their anxiety symptoms led to the development of their alcohol abuse. Moreover, it appears that comorbid patients with PTSD and alcoholism tend to drink in an episodic (binge drinking) fashion. More research is required to
determine whether the binges are actually triggered by PTSD
symptoms, external reminders of the traumatic event, or both.
M e c h a n i s m s Proposed to A c c o u n t for C o m o r b i d
P T S D - A l c o h o l Abuse
Most explanations offered to date to account for the high degree of overlap between PTSD and alcohol abuse have centered
on the notion that some individuals with PTSD abuse alcohol
tO reduce or control their PTSD symptoms. For the purposes of
this review, the postulated self-medicating effects of alcohol are
divided into four categories that may or may not be mutually
exclusive: physiological, behavioral, affective, and cognitive. Although the effects of alcohol on each of these conceptually distinct categories of PTSD symptoms are examined separately, it
should be recognized that if alcohol proves to exert its effects on
95
more than one of these symptom clusters simultaneously, the
resultant interactive effect of alcohol on PTSD symptoms may
prove to be greater than the sum of its effects on individual
PTSD symptom components.
In this section, I review the relationship between PTSD and
alcohol abuse from within the framework of the many existing
stress-related drinking theories. I discuss the advantages and
disadvantages of each theory in the most relevant section. For
example, given that the tension-reduction theory (TRT; Conger,
1951 ) of alcohol use-abuse historically developed out of the behavioral tradition, it is reviewed in the Behavioral Effects
section.
Physiological Effects of Alcohol in PTSD
One of the characteristics of PTSD is persistent symptoms of
increased arousal, as indicated by such symptoms as difficulty
falling or staying asleep, exaggerated startle response, and physiological reactivity to exposure to events that symbolize or resemble an aspect of the trauma (APA, 1994). Alcohol abuse
in PTSD may function to dampen this increased physiological
arousal. For example, LaCoursiere et al. (1980) speculated that
alcohol abuse in patients with PTSD may represent an attempt
to prevent sleep disturbances. Keane, Gerardi, et al. (1988)
have also suggested that substance abuse in combat veterans
with PTSD may promote sleep or help them to alleviate physiological arousal states.
These hypothesized mechanisms can be viewed from within
the framework of Sher's (1987) stress response dampening
(SRD) theory of alcohol abuse. According to this theory, individuals drink to dampen their physiological reactivity to stressful events. It could be speculated that patients with PTSD may
be even more susceptible to alcohol's SRD effects than individuals not exposed to trauma and thus may be highly susceptible
to negative reinforcement from drinking.
No researchers have specifically examined the effects of alcohol consumption on physiological reactivity within PTSD samples. However, much research suggests that alcohol reduces
physiological reactivity to stressful stimuli or events (for a review, see Sher, 1987). Researchers have examined the effects of
alcohol on a variety of measures of physiological responses to
stress in humans. Levenson, Sher, Grossman, Newman, and
Newlin (1980), for example, examined the effects of alcohol on
physiological arousal before and during exposure to two stressors (i.e., signalled shock and a self-disclosing speech) in male
college students. The consumption of alcohol, but not of a placebo, markedly attenuated the magnitude of physiological responses to the stressors. A consistent finding across several studies was a reduction in the magnitude of heart rate and skin conductance responses to stress (e.g., Finn & Pihl, 1987, 1988;
Finn, Zeitouni, & Pihl, 1990; Lindman, Alexanderson, &
Kvarnstrom, 1979; Sher & Levenson, 1982, 1983; Stewart,
Finn, & Pihl, 1992; Stewart & Pihl, 1994; Wilson, Abrams, &
Lipscomb, 1980).
Some studies have not shown SRD effects of alcohol on measures of autonomic reactivity to stress (e.g., D. B. Abrams &
Wilson, 1979; Dengerink & Fagan, 1978; Keane & Lisman,
1980; Wilson & Abrams, 1977). These studies have used relatively low doses of alcohol. In fact, the clear dose effects ob-
96
STEWART
tained by Stewart, Finn, et al. (1992) support the conclusion
that relatively high doses of alcohol (i.e., those producing blood
alcohol levels [BALs] of greater than 0.08%) are needed to
achieve significant SRD effects (see also Stewart, Finn, & Pihl,
1995). In summarizing the results of various studies in this
area, Cappell and Greeley (1987) and Sher (1987) have concurred that the majority of evidence suggests that alcohol attenuates autonomic responses to stress at least when moderately
high doses of alcohol are used.
Gray (1982) has described an anxiety system, termed the behavioral inhibition system (BIS), that responds to threat cues
with inhibition of ongoing behavior and increased physiological
arousal. Alcohol is known to increase activity of the neurotransmitter gamma-aminobutyric acid in septal-hippocampal brain
regions (i.e., BIS, Gray, 1982; Pihl & Peterson, 1995 ). In other
words, one of alcohol's principal physiological effects is to enhance the actions of an inhibitory neurotransmitter (i.e., one
that ultimately leads to decreased neural activity) in the regions
of the brain thought to be associated with anxiety production.
This neurochemical mechanism may underlie alcohol's physiological SRD effects. Gray (1985 ) suggested that anxious individuals are high in BIS activity and may be particularly sensitive
to alcohol-induced SRD effects. These physiological effects of
alcohol may reinforce drinking in patients with anxiety disorders such as PTSD.
More research is required to determine the degree to which
these previous findings on alcohol's physiological SRD effects
are actually related to alcohol abuse in individuals with PTSD.
One study provides some preliminary data on this issue. Studying Vietnam combat veterans, McFall et al. (1992) found that
problem drinking levels (i.e., MAST scores) were significantly
positively correlated with self-report measures of the severity of
physiological arousal symptoms. This finding could suggest that
alcohol abuse represents an attempt to dampen physiological
reactivity in PTSD. However, the correlational nature of these
data again begs the question of causation. Moreover, the effects
of alcohol on other arousal-related PTSD symptoms (e.g., sleep
disturbance and exaggerated startle response) remain to be
studied.
The alcohol challenge paradigm (Newlin & Thomson, 1990)
would prove useful in this endeavor. In this paradigm, patients
with PTSD and controls could be administered a moderately
intoxicating dose of alcohol, and the effects of alcohol on physiological reactivity could be monitored to determine whether patients with PTSD exhibit unique responses to alcohol (e.g., increased sensitivity to alcohol's SRD effects), which might begin
to explain their apparently increased risk for alcohol abuse. Although the SRD theory appears to account well for some of the
data (e.g., McFall et al., 1992), any theory that purports to explain the increased abuse of alcohol in patients with PTSD by
way of alcohol's effects on physiological reactivity must account
for the fact that alcohol consumption has repeatedly been
shown to lead to a significant increase in "tonic" levels of
arousal (i.e., baseline levels of physiological arousal taken in the
absence of situational stressors; e.g., Jones, Parsons, & Rundell,
1976; Niatoh, 1972; Stewart, Finn, et al., 1992).
Even if physiological reactivity symptoms in patients with
PTSD are reduced following alcohol consumption, one needs
to demonstrate that such alcohol-induced SRD effects actually
reinforce alcohol consumption in patients with PTSD. The
stress-induced drinking methodology, first used by Higgins and
Marlatt ( 1973, 1975), might prove helpful. In this paradigm,
participants are exposed to a stress-induction manipulation,
and then the experimenters record participants' levels of alcohol consumption in an unobtrusive taste-rating test. Although
the true variable of interest is the amount of alcohol consumed,
participants are led to believe that the experimenters are interested in their ratings of a variety of beverages including alcohol
on a number of dimensions of taste (e.g., Higgins & Marlatt,
1973; Samoluk & Stewart, 1996a; Sweet, Stewart, Samoluk, &
MacDonald, 1995 ). Previous research has shown that anticipatory stress, measured by increased autonomic arousal, is associated with increased alcohol consumption (Strickler, Tomaszewski, Maxwell, & Suib, 1979). It can be hypothesized that
patients with PTSD, but not controls without PTSD, would be
particularly susceptible to drinking in response to manipulations of physiological arousal levels.
A final physiological stress-related drinking theory that provides a useful framework to view the PTSD-alcoholism comorbidity data is Volpicelli's (1987) endorphin-compensation theory. This theory rests on evidence that stressful events lead to
release of endogenous endorphins. However, with chronic stressors (or chronic responses to the initial stressor, such as in
PTSD), stores of endorphins become depleted. Ingestion of alcohol is said to be reinforced following a chronic stressor because the alcohol compensates for the depleted endorphin stores
(Volpicelli, 1987). In fact, Pitman, van der Kolk, and Orr
(1990) have suggested alterations in the regulation of endogenous opioids as a biological correlate of PTSD in combat veterans. Future research is needed to establish the validity ofendorphin compensation as an explanation for abusive drinking in
patients with PTSD.
Behavioral Effects of Alcohol in PTSD
Another of the characteristic symptoms of PTSD is the persistent avoidance of stimuli associated with the trauma, which is
behaviorally evidenced by efforts to avoid activities or situations
that arouse recollections of the trauma (APA, 1994). Thus, the
self-medication behavior of patients with PTSD may involve
attempts to reduce behavioral avoidance. For example, Skorina
and Kovach (1986) have postulated that, in sexually abused
women with alcoholism, alcohol may be used to reduce avoidance of sexual activity.
The behavioral effects of alcohol can be viewed from within
the framework of Conger's ( 1951 ) TRT of alcohol abuse. This
behavioral theory consists of two tenets. First, alcohol leads to
tension reduction; second, tension reduction reinforces alcohol
consumption (i.e., through negative reinforcement). Although
it is recognized that the TRT is not a global explanation for
abusive drinking, researchers have suggested that certain individuals might be at a heightened risk for learning to drink alcohol for its tension-reducing properties (Cappell & Greeley,
1987). Individuals exposed to trauma might prove to be one
such group. In fact, Skorina and Kovach (1986) reported that
women with alcoholism and sexual abuse histories report sexual dysfunction (e.g., avoidance of sexual activity) before abusive drinking more often than women with alcoholism and no
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
histories of sexual abuse, suggesting that alcohol may be used
by those exposed to trauma to reduce behavioral avoidance of
trauma reminders.
The time course of drinking in response to PTSD symptoms
is clearly specified in TRT (Conger, 1951 ): Drinking occurs before entry into feared situations (i.e., those situations that symbolize or resemble the traumatic events) to reduce anticipatory
anxiety, and thereby reduce behavioral avoidance. This stands
in marked contrast to predictions emerging from Volpicelli's
(1987) endorphin-compensation theory, which implies that
drinking should follow exposure to the traumatic event or reminders of the event (see also Fidler & LoLordo, 1996). More
evidence on the time course of drinking in patients with PTSD
is needed to further evaluate the usefulness of each of these theories in explaining alcohol abuse in patients with PTSD.
Although no researchers specifically have examined alcohol's
effects on behavioral avoidance in samples of participants with
PTSD, previous research suggests that alcohol may be an
effective agent in reducing behavioral avoidance in anxietyprovoking situations, in general (for a review, see Cappell &
Greeley, 1987). Early tests of the TRT focused on the effects of
alcohol on avoidance behavior in animals (e.g., Conger, 1951;
Masserman & Yum, 1946). Alcohol has repeatedly been shown
to reduce fear- or conflict-related avoidance behavior in animals
(for reviews, see CappeU & Greeley, 1987; Cappell & Herman,
1972; and Hodgson, Stockwell, & Rankin, 1979). Similar resuits have been found with human participants. Measures of
approach toward a feared object provide a clear behavioral index of the avoidance-reducing effects of alcohol. For example,
Alexanderson and Lindman (1980) examined the effect of alcohol on a naturally occurring fear. Female participants were
asked to transfer a mouse from one jar to another. The behavioral measure of avoidance was the amount of time taken to
complete this transfer. A moderately high dose of alcohol, but
not a placebo beverage, significantly reduced behavioral avoidance (i.e., significantly shorter transfer latencies following alcohol consumption). Thyer and Curtis (1984) examined the
effects of alcohol on performance in a behavioral avoidance test
(BAT; Lang, 1968) in participants diagnosed with a simple phobia. Degree of approach toward the phobic object was assessed
both before and after participants consumed either a moderately intoxicating dose of alcohol or a placebo. The Thyer and
Curtis results are clearly consistent with reduced avoidance behavior following beverage consumption in the group administered alcohol but not the placebo group (Sher, 1987). A similar
study by Rimm, Bridell, Zimmerman, and Caddy (1981) of
undergraduates with a snake phobia did not replicate findings
of reduced behavioral avoidance following alcohol consumption, but they did use a significantly lower dose of alcohol than
the doses used in other studies. Taken together, these results suggest that alcohol, at least at moderately intoxicating doses, does
reduce behavioral avoidance in anxiety-producing situations.
More research is required to determine the degree to which
alcohol's reduction of avoidance behavior is related to the motives for alcohol abuse in individuals with PTSD. One study
provides some preliminary data on this issue. McFall et al.
(1992) found that MAST scores were positively correlated with
scores on the Numbing/Avoidance subscale of the M - P T S D in
a large sample of Vietnam veterans. The degree of correlation
97
between MAST scores and degree of numbing/avoidance was
not as strong as the correlation between MAST scores and degree of physiological arousal (McFall et al., 1992). This finding
suggests that alcohol abuse in patients with PTSD may be motivated by a desire to control behavioral avoidance symptoms
but that this factor may contribute less to posttraumatic alcohol
abuse than the desire to control physiological arousal symptoms. However, the inclusion of both emotional numbing and
avoidance symptoms in this M-PTSD subscale, and the subscale's focus on both behavioral and cognitive avoidance symptoms, precludes drawing firm conclusions on this issue. Moreover, the McFall et al. data are correlational. Experimental studies on alcohol's effects on behavioral avoidance in PTSD with
the alcohol challenge paradigm (Newlin & Thomson, 1990)
and the BAT (Lang, 1968) are required before researchers can
make any definitive statements on causality. Retrospective and
prospective self-report studies examining the patterns of alcohol use in patients with PTSD could help determine whether
alcohol use in the daily lives of patients with PTSD occurs in
response to anticipation of entering feared situations, as would
be predicted by the TRT (Conger, 1951; Fidler & LoLordo,
1996).
Affective Effects of Alcohol in PTSD
Another characteristic symptom of PTSD is a numbing of
general emotional responsiveness, as indicated by efforts to
avoid feelings associated with the trauma, restricted range of
affect, feelings of detachment or estrangement from others, and
diminished interest in significant activities (APA, 1994 ). Other
affective symptoms characteristic of PTSD include emotional
lability (i.e., irritability or outbursts of anger) and associated
complications of feelings of depression, and guilt (APA, 1994).
As has been previously suggested with respect to other anxiety
disorders (e.g., B. J. Cox et al., 1990; Kushner et al., 1990; Otto,
Pollack, Sachs, et al., 1992), alcohol abuse in patients with
PTSD may represent an attempt to reduce the negative affective
symptoms of PTSD. More specifically, LaCoursiere et al.
(1980), Kosten and Krystal (1988), and Schnitt and Nocks
(1984) suggested that alcohol may be used in PTSD to relieve
subjective feelings of anxiety or to self-medicate for the depressonlike dysphoria and guilt associated with PTSD symptoms.
Alternatively, Krystal (1984) hypothesized that combat veterans with PTSD develop a dread of the more expressive elements
of emotion and strive to block these through alcohol abuse.
Similarly, Evans (1988) hypothesized that individuals exposed
to sexual abuse, physical abuse, or both may use alcohol to
dampen the experience of strong feelings.
Given these hypotheses, research on alexithymia might contribute to understanding the motives for abusive drinking in patients with PTSD. The term alexithymia, which literally means
no words for mood, refers to a disturbance in affective and cognitive functions, characterized by difficulties in recognizing and
verbalizing feeling states, a relative lack of fantasy life, descriptions of somatic symptoms as opposed to affective states, and
speech and thought patterns that are concrete and closely tied
to external events (Nemiah & Sifneos, 1970; Sifneos, 1972,
1973; for a review on alexithymia, see G. J. Taylor, 1994).
Researchers have found that people exposed to trauma, such
98
STEWART
as individuals held in concentration camp (Krystal, 1982), and
combat veterans (Hyer, Woods, Summers, Boudewyns, & Harrison, 1990; Shipko, Alvarez, & Noviello, 1983) are often
highly alexithymic. Krystal proposed that alexithymia may develop in response to extreme trauma to reduce the experience
of painful affect. McNally, Litz, Prassas, Shin, and Weathers
(1994) provided experimental data to support this argument.
They showed that Vietnam veterans with PTSD exhibit "overgeneral" memories (i.e., memories lacking in specific detail) in
response to emotional cue words, which may be indicative of
attempts to avoid affect (J. M. G. Williams, 1995 ). Shipko et al.
have suggested that constriction of emotional expression may be
adaptive in some traumatic situations (e.g., combat).
In addition to being characteristic of people exposed to
trauma, high levels of alexithymia have also been observed in
people with currently symptomatic alcoholism (Krystal,
1979 ), newly abstinent alcoholism (Haviland, MacMurray, &
Cummings, 1988), and those with high familial-genetic risk
for alcohol abuse (Finn, Martin, & Pihl, 1987)--the last study
suggesting that alexithymia in people with alcoholism is more
likely a premorbid vulnerability factor than a consequence of
chronic abusive drinking. Like alexithymia, alcohol abuse
might develop as a means of avoiding strong affect in patients
with PTSD. In fact, alcohol consumption, motivated primarily
by desires to cope with (i.e., avoid or reduce) negative emotional states, is a strong predictor of alcohol-related problems.
Frequency of drinking for coping-related motives predicts
drinking problems over and above drinking quantity and frequency information alone (Cooper, Russell, Skinner, & Windle,
1992). Additionally, McFall et al. (1992) found that level of
Numbing/Avoidance symptoms on the M-PTSD was positively correlated with MAST (Seizer, 1971 ) scores, providing
initial empirical support for Krystal's (1984) emotional-numbing hypothesis of alcohol abuse in combat veterans. However,
the correlational nature of these data and the fact that numbing
and avoidance symptoms are included in the same subscale
make interpretation of McFall et al.'s findings problematic.
Although no researchers have directly examined the effects of
alcohol on affect in either people exposed to trauma in general,
or patients with PTSD in particular, a good deal of empirical
research exists that supports the notion that alcohol can, at
times, be effective in reducing negative mood states (Pihl &
Smith, 1983; Russell & Mehrabian, 1975). In general, alcohol
leads to improvements in self-reported mood (i.e., increases in
positive mood and decreases in negative mood; Cappell & Greeley, 1987). The finding that alcohol consumption reduces negative mood (i.e., subjective anxiety or tension) provides support
for the TRT (Conger, 1951 ). However, results in this area have
not been as consistent or robust as the findings of alcoholinduced SRD effects on physiological measures (Sher, 1987).
Moreover, there is significant variability in the methodologies
used across studies, including differences in the doses of alcohol
used and the point at which self-rating measures are completed
relative to the phase of the blood alcohol curve (Pihl & Smith,
1983 ). With regard to dose, the effects of alcohol on subjective
mood appear to be dose dependent, with larger acute doses of
alcohol being associated, in general, with greater reductions in
self-reported negative mood states (e.g., Kalin, McClelland, &
Kahn, 1965; Persson, Sjoberg, & Svensson, 1980; Warren &
Raynes, 1972). Self-reported mood also appears related to the
direction of the change in blood alcohol concentration: Participants tend to report mood enhancement when their BALs are
ascending and mood deterioration during the descending limb
of the blood alcohol curve (e.g., Goldberg, 1966; Jones, 1973 ).
Paradoxically, alcohol at times leads to increases in negative
affect. For example, in the laboratory, prolonged drinking has
been associated with increases in self-reports of anxiety and
other negative affect in participants with alcoholism (e.g.,
McNamee, Mello, & Mendelson, 1968; Mendelson, LaDou, &
Solomon, 1964). Moreover, the ingestion of very high doses of
alcohol has been shown to lead to increased anxiety (Freed,
1978), as has withdrawal from continued drinking (Peyser,
1982). Any theory that purports to explain the overlap between
PTSD and alcohol abuse in terms of attempts to reduce negative emotional states must consider these paradoxical findings.
For example, it could be that patients with PTSD may be less
sensitive to these aversive effects of alcohol consumption than
controls without PTSD. Alternatively, it could be that patients
with PTSD are initially motivated to drink to decrease or avoid
negative emotional states but eventually drink to alleviate aversive alcohol withdrawal symptoms ( Mello, 1981 ).
Moreover, any hypothesis purporting to explain the overlap
between PTSD and alcohol abuse on the basis that alcohol is
used to avoid strong affect (e.g., Krystal, 1984) must account
for findings that alcohol not only reduces negative emotions but
also increases positive emotions. For example, it may be that
the motive for drinking in patients with PTSD is not the avoidance of strong affect, in general ( Krystal, 1984), but rather the
avoidance of only painful and negative affects. Alternatively, it
might be that patients with PTSD experience a dampening of
painful affect following alcohol consumption but, unlike controls, experience no change in levels of positive affect. Finally, if
patients with PTSD only dread strong emotions (Krystal, 1984)
in the sober state but are able to find strong positive emotions
pleasurable and reinforcing when intoxicated, then it could
be that they experience both decreased negative affect (negative reinforcement) and increased positive affect (positive
reinforcement) following alcohol consumption. These various
possibilities are deserving of further study. Methods to assess
the relationship between various aspects of affect and alcohol
consumption in patients with PTSD include retrospective
and prospective self-reports, the alcohol challenge paradigm
(Newlin & Thomson, 1990), and the stress-induced drinking
methodology (e.g., Higgins & Marlatt, 1973 ).
Cognitive Effects of Alcohol in PTSD
The hallmark symptom of PTSD is the persistent reexperiencing of the traumatic event (APA, 1987 ). Such intrusive cognitive symptoms can involve recurrent and distressing recollections of the event, recurrent distressing dreams of the event,
sudden acting or feeling as if the traumatic event were recurring,
and intense psychological distress at exposure to events that
symbolize or resemble an aspect of the trauma (APA, 1994).
Other characteristic cognitive symptoms of PTSD include
difficulty concentrating, hypervigilance, and an effort to avoid
thoughts associated with the trauma (i.e., thought suppression;
APA, 1994). Thus, abusive drinking to self-medicate in patients
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
with PTSD may involve attempts to control one or more of
these cognitive symptoms. For example, both LaCoursiere et
al. (1980) and Krystal (1984) postulated that alcohol abuse in
patients with PTSD may serve to eliminate terrifying nightmares. More specifically, LaCoursiere et al. suggested that patients with PTSD may be using alcohol to suppress rapid eye
movement (REM) sleep and its associated nightmares. They
also postulated that the abuse of alcohol in such patients would
escalate as tolerance to the REM-suppressant effects of alcohol
develops over time. However, traumatic nightmares are not confined to REM sleep (Kramer, Schoen, & Kinney, 1984). Therefore, alcohol may suppress REM sleep, eliminating some, but
not all, nightmares. Not being able to eliminate their dream
disturbances completely may reinforce heavier drinking in patients with PTSD.
LaCoursiere et al. (1980) further postulated that in patients
with PTSD, heavy drinking might block intrusive memories associated with the traumatic event during the waking state. Similarly, Keane, Gerardi, et al. (1988) and Lindy, Grace, and
Green (1984) suggested that substance abuse in combat veterans with PTSD exists to quell intrusive cognitive experiences.
Alcohol abuse in patients with PTSD might also focus attentional resources (i.e., reduce the concentration difficulties characteristic of PTSD; Steele & Josephs, 1988), reduce hypervigilance, or both.
In Wilson's (1988) review of the relation between alcohol
abuse and various anxiety disorders, he noted that there may
be important pathophysiological differences among the various
anxiety disorders that interact differentially with alcohol. Several current theories interpret alcohol's anxiolytic properties in
terms of direct or indirect effects on cognitive processes (e.g.,
threat appraisal, Levenson et al., t980; attention allocation,
Steele & Josephs, 1988). Wilson has speculated that alcohol's
self-medicating effects may be cognitively mediated only in patients with anxiety disorders with a large cognitive component.
With intrusive cognitive symptoms often hailed as the disorder's "hallmark" symptom, PTSD could certainly be viewed
as one disorder meeting criteria for inclusion in this category.
However, are the cognitive symptoms of PTSD influenced by
alcohol consumption, and does this hypothesized reduction in
intrusive cognitive symptoms reinforce heavy drinking in patients with PTSD?
Preliminary results support the notion that alcohol abuse in
patients with PTSD may represent an attempt to dampen intrusive cognitive symptoms. For example, in the study by
McFall et al. (1992) of participants with combat-related PTSD,
degree of cognitive reexperiencing of the trauma was significantly positively correlated with MAST (Seizer, 1971 ) scores.
However, these data are correlational and thus do not establish
a causal connection between cognitive reexperiencing and alcohol abuse. Future studies that monitor daily symptoms in relation to alcohol use would be useful in this regard, as would experimental alcohol challenge (Newlin & Thomson, 1990)
studies.
Researchers have begun to use methods derived from experi
mental cognitive psychology to investigate informationprocessing biases in participants with high levels of anxiety
(e.g., Samoluk & Stewart, 1996b; Stewart & Gignac, 1996).
Previous studies whose researchers used such methods have
99
shown patients with PTSD to be characterized by informationprocessing biases that may be related to intrusive cognitive
symptoms. First, an attentional bias favoring the processing of
trauma-related information has been repeatedly demonstrated
in patients with PTSD but not in controls (e.g., Bryant &
Harvey, 1995; Foa, Feske, Murdock, Kozak, & McCarthy,
1991; Kaspi, McNally, & Amir, 1995; McNally, English, &
Lipke, 1993; McNally et al., 1987; Thrasher, Dalgleish, & Yule,
1994). For example, Bryant and Harvey ( 1995 ) compared the
performance of individuals involved in motor vehicle accidents
with PTSD, simple phobia of driving, or low anxiety on an emotional Stroop color-naming paradigm. This paradigm is commonly used to assess degree of attentional bias for fear-related
information in the emotional disorders (see reviews by Logan
& Goetsch, 1993; Mathews & MacLeod, 1994; Segal, 1996; and
J. M. G. Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Mathews, 1988). Participants were asked to name the color in which a variety of words
were printed, as quickly as possible, while ignoring the meaning
of the words. Words included strong threat words (e.g., smash),
mild threat words (e.g., traffic), positive words (e.g., smile),
and neutral words (e.g., blanket). In contrast to individuals
with a simple phobia of driving and low-anxious controls, those
with PTSD were significantly slower to color name strong threat
words than positive and neutral words. Such findings suggest
that trauma-related information captures and holds the attention of patients with PTSD, thereby interfering with task performance (i.e., color naming). These findings support the notion that patients with PTSD are hypervigilant toward traumarelated information (see reviews by Chemtob, Roitblatt,
Himada, Carlson, & Twentyman, 1989; and Litz & Keane,
1989). These results also support the hypothesis that information about trauma is primed in memory and parallel phenomenological reports of the ease with which traumatic memories
involuntarily "come to mind" for patients with PTSD.
This latter hypothesis has been evaluated in patients with
PTSD using memory paradigms derived fi'om experimental
cognitive psychology. Initial research in this area used cued recall and word-stem completion tests to assess relative explicit
and implicit memory for trauma-relevant versus trauma-irrelevant material in patients with PTSD and controls (see a review
by McNally, 1994). Explicit memory refers to conscious recollection of previous experiences, whereas implicit memory is revealed when performance on a task is facilitated by prior experience without the necessity of conscious intentional recollection of the previous experience (Schacter, 1987). Work with the
cued recall (explicit memory) and word-stem completion
(implicit memory) tests in patients with PTSD and controls
demonstrated enhanced memory for trauma-related words in
patients with PTSD (see a review by McNally, 1994).
However, recent research demonstrates that the word-stem
completion test is sensitive to manipulations of perceptual
( Roediger & Blaxton, 1987 ) but not semantic (Gignac & Stewart, 1995) attributes of stimuli. This task may therefore have
little relevance for the study of implicit memory for emotional
meanings in anxiety disorders (Gignac & Stewart, 1995;
McNally, 1994). Researchers have thus begun to use alternative
implicit memory paradigms adapted from cognitive psychology
to study implicit memory biases for trauma-related material in
PTSD. For example, Amir, McNally, and Wiegartz ( 1995 ) used
100
STEWART
a noise judgement task developed by Jacoby, Allan, Collins, and
Larwill ( 1988 ) to investigate implicit memory bias for combat
material in Vietnam veterans with and without PTSD. Veterans
were first exposed to combat relevant (e.g., "the chopper landed
in a hot LZ" [landing zone receiving small arms fire]) and neutral (e.g., "the shiny apple sat on the table") sentences. Veterans
then rated the volume of white noise accompanying the presentation of these primed sentences as well as novel unprimed sentences. Implicit memory for the meaning of previously heard
sentences is revealed when veterans rate the white noise accompanying unprimed sentences as louder than the noise accompanying the primed sentences (Jacoby et al., 1988 ). Veterans with
PTSD, but not controls, were found to rate the noise accompanying unprimed combat sentences as louder than noise accompanying primed combat sentences, suggesting implicit memory
for the meaning of combat-related material in patients with
PTSD. Such implicit memory biases for trauma-related material may underlie the intrusive cognitive symptoms (e.g.,
flashbacks) characteristic of PTSD.
Researchers have begun to investigate whether alcohol and
related drugs, such as the benzodiazepines (Stewart, Pihl, &
Padjen, 1992), might affect the tendency of anxious participants to selectively process fear-related information (e.g., Golombok, Stavrou, & Bonn, 1991 ). Although no researchers have
specifically examined the effects of alcohol consumption on intrusive cognitive symptoms in patients with PTSD, some preliminary research suggests that alcohol may be an effective agent
in reducing selective attention to threat in participants with
high anxiety. For example, a set of two experiments reviewed by
Stewart and Pihl (1995) examined the effects of alcohol on the
processing of threatening information in an analogue sample
of students with high-anxiety sensitivity and controls with lowanxiety sensitivity. In the first study (Stewart, Achille, DuboisNguyen, & Pihl, 1992), the processing bias favoring threatening
words observed in sober students with high-anxiety sensitivity
was eliminated following the administration of a moderately intoxicating dose of alcohol. These results were replicated and extended in a second study (Stewart, Achille, & Pihl, 1993): The
degree ofattentional bias for threatening information in a group
of students with high-anxiety sensitivity who had received a
moderately intoxicating dose of alcohol was significantly attenuated relative to the degree of bias for threat in a control group
of students with high-anxiety sensitivity who had received a placebo beverage. Thus, some preliminary evidence suggests that
alcohol may be effective in reducing the tendency of participants with anxiety sensitivity to selectively attend to threatening
material. The degree to which such results are applicable to explaining the association between alcohol abuse and PTSD remains to be determined.
No researchers have specifically assessed the effects of alcohol
on memory biases for trauma-related material in patients with
PTSD. However, much basic research attests to the fact that
acute doses of alcohol and the structurally and functionally similar benzodiazepines (Stewart, Pihl, et al., 1992) have significant effects on human memory abilities. Initial research suggested that alcohol (Hastroudi, Parker, DeLisi, Wyatt, & Mutter, 1984) and the benzodiazepines (Danion, Zimmerman,
Willard-Schroeder, Grange, & Singer, 1989; Fang, Hinrichs, &
Ghoneim, 1987; also see a review by Curran, 1991) signifi-
cantly impaired performance on traditional tests of explicit
memory, while leaving performance on implicit memory tasks
intact. However, recent evidence suggests that certain benzodiazepines may not only reduce explicit memory performance
but also impair implicit memory processes when memory is
tested at peak drug concentrations (e.g., Legrand et al., 1995;
Stewart, Rioux, Connolly, Dunphy, & Teehan, 1996). Similar
studies of the time course of the effects of alcohol on implicit
memory have yet to be conducted.
Given evidence that alcohol impairs explicit memory abilities
(Hashtroudi et al., 1984), it might be speculated that patients
with PTSD may be motivated to drink to reduce their conscious
recollection of trauma-relevant material. However, this diminished awareness of traumatic memories might be achieved at a
cost in the form of a general reduction in their overall recall
ability. It has also been speculated (e.g., LaCoursiere et al.,
1980) that patients with PTSD may be motivated to drink to
control the intrusive cognitive symptoms (e.g., involuntary cognitive reexperiencing) presumably supported by implicit memory processes (Amir et al., 1995 ). Although recent data suggest
deletrious effects of benzodiazepines on implicit memory task
performance at peak drug concentrations (e.g., Legrand et al.,
1995; Stewart, Rioux, et al., 1995), additional basic research
on the effects of alcohol on implicit memory is required to test
this latter speculation. In addition, studies addressing the effects
of acute doses of alcohol on memory biases for trauma-related
material (e.g., Amir et al., 1995 ) are clearly needed. Finally, if
future research determines that alcohol administration is indeed effective in reducing and controlling intrusive cognitive
symptoms in patients with PTSD, it must also be determined
that these cognitive effects of alcohol actually reinforce alcohol
consumption. Alcohol consumption in response to cognitive
manipulations (e.g., exposure to primes resembling some aspect of the traumatic event; McNally et al., 1994) could be explored using the unobtrusive taste-rating task (e.g., Higgins &
Marlatt, 1973; MacDonald, Stewart, Samoluk, & Sweet, 1995 ).
Several cognitive theories have been proposed to explain the
high degree ofcomorbidity between the anxiety and alcohol disorders. Each of these theories appears potentially applicable in
explaining the increased abuse of alcohol in patients with
PTSD. For example, Levenson et al. (1980) suggested that alcohol might disrupt the cognitive processes involved in evaluating a situation as stressful or threatening. Thus, alcohol consumption by patients with PTSD might influence the appraisal
of various situations as stressful: Alcohol might reduce the distressing nature of intrusive recollections of the traumatic event
or reduce the distress associated with exposure to events that
symbolize or resemble an aspect of the traumatic event through
its influence on the threat-appraisal process.
Another cognitive theory is the attention allocation model
proposed by Steele and Josephs ( 1988) to account for alcohol's
effects on anxiety (see also Josephs & Steele, 1990). Alcohol is
said to narrow the focus of attention to the most immediate
internal and external cues. When intoxication is accompanied
by a moderately demanding cognitive activity, attention is directed toward that activity, leaving less attentional resources
available for the processing of threatening information, leading
to anxiety reduction. Alcohol abuse in patients with PTSD
might represent an attempt to focus attentional resources to-
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
ward the task at hand and away from trauma-related cues.
Thus, alcohol might serve to reduce hypervigilance toward
threat and difficulty concentrating in the patient with PTSD.
More research is required on this attention allocation model,
particularly with respect to its potential to explain the overlap
between PTSD and alcohol abuse problems.
Marlatt and Rohsenow (1980) have also proposed a cognitive
theory of alcohol abuse. Their expectancy hypothesis places
emphasis on the beliefs about the expected effects of alcohol
consumption. Such "alcohol expectancies" produce significant
effects on both drinking and postdrinking behaviors, which are
often as strong or even stronger than the actual pharmacological
effects of the drug (Hull & Bond, 1986). In particular, individuals with alcoholism are more likely to attribute stress-reducing
properties to alcohol than are controls (Calahan, 1970). Alcohol expectancies in patients with PTSD versus controls without
PTSD remain to be assessed. It would be interesting to determine whether patients with PTSD are more likely than controls
without PTSD to attribute stress-reducing properties to alcohol. It also remains to be determined whether those patients
with PTSD who believe alcohol to be capable of reducing
or controlling PTSD symptoms (e.g., intrusive distressing
thoughts) are more likely to abuse alcohol than those patients with PTSD who report fewer of these specific alcohol
expectancies.
Once alcohol abuse is established, its maintenance in patients
with PTSD might also be viewed through the framework of cognitive urge models of addiction (e.g., Baker, Morse, & Sherman,
1987 ). These models propose that urges are organized at a cognitive level within a propositional network that encodes information on eliciting cues, drug-related responses (e.g., verbal reports of craving and drug-procurement behaviors), and the interpreted meanings of stimuli and responses. Baker et al. drew
contrasts between positive- and negative-affect urges. Patients
with PTSD might be hypothesized to be a group at high risk for
alcohol use following negative-affect urges. Stimulus information encoded within this type of urge network for patients with
PTSD might include internal symptoms (e.g., intrusive cognitive symptoms, physiological arousal or reactivity cues, and
negative affect cues), external stimuli (e.g., events that serve as
reminders of the initial traumatic event), or both. According to
Baker et al.'s urge model, the responses produced by the activation of such a network should promote alcohol-seeking behavior and self-reports of urges or cravings to drink in alcohol abusing patients with PTSD.
Given data that self-reported urges do not always precede relapse in addicts (for a review, see Tiffany, 1990), Tiffany proposed an alternative cognitive theory of addictions that
contends that drug use in the addict is controlled by automatized action schemata. Given a relatively accessible drug like
alcohol and a history of repeated practice of alcohol use in response to certain cues, alcohol procurement and consumption
behaviors may become highly automatized in the alcohol abusing patient with PTSD. Tiffany also proposed that the procedures for alcohol administration are stored in memory in the
form of automatized action schemata, which are activated by
certain internal and external triggers. A modification of Tiffany's theory to explain the maintenance of alcohol abuse in patients with PTSD might assert that PTSD symptoms, reminders
101
of the traumatic event, negative emotions, or all three could
serve as eliciting cues for this automatized action schemata of
alcohol administration behavior. Unlike the traditional urge
models (e.g., Baker et al., 1987), Tiffany's theory asserts that
alcohol administration can occur in the absence of self-reported
urges or cravings for alcohol.
Researchers have begun to test predictions made by Baker et
al.'s ( 1987 ) and Tiffany's (1990) models with respect to several
addictive drugs including alcohol (e.g., Rohsenow et al., 1992).
For example, if alcohol cues are stored in the memory of the
individual who abuses alcohol along with eliciting cues, as is
suggested by both Baker et al. and Tiffany, then exposure to
eliciting cures should lead to enhanced processing of alcoholrelated information. Samoluk and Stewart (1996b) used the
Stroop paradigm to test this prediction in an analogue sample
at risk for alcohol abuse. Similar cognitive methodologies could
be used to test the hypothesis that alcohol-related information
is stored in memory networks along with eliciting cues (e.g.,
PTSD symptoms, information related to the trauma, or both)
in patients with PTSD who abuse alcohol. Such findings would
be predicted by both traditional urge models (e.g., Baker et al.,
1987 ) and Tiffany's theory. However, the two models appear to
make different predictions regarding the level of awareness of
alcohol urges or cravings in the individual who is alcohol dependent, with Baker et al.'s theory stating that urges are necessary
for alcohol administration to occur in response to triggering
stimuli and Tiffany's theory positing that urges are unnecessary.
These predictions could be evaluated by using cognitive paradigms designed to examine automatic versus nonautomatic information-processing biases (e.g., Mogg, Bradley, Williams, &
Mathews, 1993) and by having participants report on their level
of alcohol urges or cravings (see Rankin, Hodgson, & Stockwell,
1979) in response to hypothesized triggering stimuli.
Alcohol-Induced PTSD Symptom Enhancement
Alternatively or in addition to the self-medicating role of alcohol abuse in individuals with PTSD are notions that alcohol
might increase susceptibility to PTSD or worsen PTSD symptoms following exposure to a traumatic event. For example,
Kolb ( 1985 ) has hypothesized that alcohol, like the structurally
and functionally similar barbiturates, might serve as a "narcosynthetic" agent in the expression of PTSD symptoms (i.e., certain PTSD symptoms, such as flashbacks, are more likely to
be triggered following heavy drinking). Kolb used the sodium
amytal interview (i.e., barbiturate administration) as a clinical
tool for identifying catastrophic stressors that were too terrifying for discussion in the control state of consciousness. As defined by Kolb ( 1985 ), narcosynthesis is a drug-induced recall
of repressed material through an abreactive experience.
Through this procedure, repressed material becomes consciously available for later integration and synthesis by the personality (for reviews, see Friedman, 1990, and Kolb, 1993 ).
Similar to Kolb's (1985) notion of alcohol-induced PTSD
symptom enhancement, Greenstein, Kitchner, and Olsen
(1986) suggested that alcohol-induced disinhibition may contribute to flashbacks and nightmares in patients with PTSD.
Even LaCoursiere et al. (1980), some of the earliest and strongest supporters of the self-medication hypothesis for alcohol
102
STEWART
abuse in patients with PTSD, noted that long-term heavy use of
alcohol may significantly exacerbate the symptoms of PTSD.
For example, the peripheral nerve damage engendered by
chronic heavy drinking may make some patients with PTSD
even more susceptible to startle reactions. Furthermore, heavy
drinking eventually disturbs the natural sleep process, and thus
continued alcohol abuse may exacerbate the sleep disturbances
typically seen in patients with PTSD.
The high degree of overlap between panic disorder and alcoholism has been explained on the basis of the effects of repeated
alcohol withdrawal. More specifically, neurochemical changes
accompanying alcohol withdrawal, such as noradrenergic (NA)
system activation have been proposed to kindle panic attacks
(George, Nutt, Dwyer, & Linnoila, 1990). Southwick et al.
(1993) have proposed that altered sensitivity of the NA system
may underlie PTSD symptoms and thus serve as a biological
marker for the disorder. However, this notion has been challenged by Wiseman (1994) on the basis that Southwick et al.
did not consider the effects of repeated alcohol withdrawal on
the NA system of patients with PTSD. Thus, rather than the
NA system disturbances being a biological marker for PTSD,
the NA changes noted by Southwick et al. may be secondary
to repeated alcohol withdrawal in many patients with PTSD.
LaCoursiere et al. (1980) also commented on the role of the
well-known alcohol withdrawal effects that emerge following
prolonged drinking. They noted that after chronic use of alcohol, attempts to discontinue this self-medication often lead to
an exacerbation of the initial PTSD symptoms. Given the similarities between several alcohol withdrawal and PTSD symptoms (e.g., autonomic overarousal and sleeping difficulties;
APA, 1994), it is possible that patients misinterpret alcohol
withdrawal symptoms as signs of anxiety or that these symptoms serve as reminders of the trauma, thereby further increasing anxiety and arousal levels and motivating continued alcohol
consumption.
In the future, researchers may wish to focus on the degree to
which alcohol abusing patients with PTSD are able to distinguish between alcohol withdrawal and PTSD symptoms. If
withdrawal and anxiety symptoms are shown to be easily confused by patients with PTSD (as they are by patients with comorbid panic disorder-alcoholism; George, Zerby, Noble, &
Nutt, 1988), researchers might increase the understanding of
the particular difficulties patients with alcoholism and PTSD
face during alcohol detoxification. Such knowledge in turn
could lead toward improving interventions for effectively withdrawing such patients from alcohol. For example, a cognitivebehavioral treatment approach to benzodiazepine discontinuation has been developed for patients with panic disorder, based
on the notion that these patients tend to misinterpret withdrawal symptoms as signs of reemerging anxiety symptoms
(i.e., Otto, Pollack, Meltzer-Brody, & Rosenbaum, 1992). This
novel treatment approach has been shown to be more successful
in assisting patients to withdraw from the drug than a traditional slow taper discontinuation procedure (Otto, Pollack,
Meltzer-Brody, et al., 1992). Similar treatments might be beneficial in helping patients with alcoholism and PTSD through the
withdrawal phase.
Summary
In conclusion, it appears that a single unidirectional pathway
to explain the overlap between PTSD and alcohol abuse is un-
likely to be found. Instead, it seems possible that both self-medication and alcohol intoxication or withdrawal-induced intensification of PTSD symptoms contribute to the high degree of
comorbidity between alcohol abuse and PTSD diagnoses. Although preliminary evidence suggests that the initial motivation
for abusive drinking in patients with PTSD is the reliefofPTSD
symptoms, alcohol intoxication, withdrawal effects, or both
could serve to heighten anxiety in the long term, requiring further heavy drinking to dampen emerging PTSD symptoms.
Thus, a vicious cycle may be at play between PTSD and alcohol
abuse, where one disorder sustains the other (Davidson et al.,
1990; LaCoursiere et al., 1980). This cyclical interplay may
render comorbid PTSD and alcohol abuse disorders more
difficult to treat than either disorder alone (see Davidson et al.,
1990).
Clinical I m p l i c a t i o n s
The results of the studies reviewed have several important implications for the assessment and treatment of people exposed
to trauma who abuse alcohol.
Assessment
The consistent finding of an association between trauma
exposure and alcohol abuse suggests that clinicians working
with individuals who present alcohol abuse symptoms should
carefully assess for a history of exposure to trauma and PTSD
symptoms. Information regarding the nature and severity of the
trauma should also be obtained. This includes, for example,
determining the type of trauma to which the individual was exposed (e.g., disaster, assault, or combat), the age at which the
event occurred, the duration of exposure (e.g., acute vs.
chronic), and the individual's sense of control during the
trauma (see Foa, Steketee, & Rothbaum, 1989; and Foa, Zinbarg, & Rothbaum, 1992).
In the assessment of PTSD symptoms, clinicians should,
whenever possible, use a multimodal assessment (Lyons et al.,
1988 ), focusing on physiological, behavioral, affective, and cognitive components of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Some
clinical researchers have also advocated the use of cognitive psychology paradigms, such as the emotional Stroop task, as additional clinical assessment tools (e.g., McNally et al., 1993).
There are several interviews available for diagnosing PTSD (for
a review, see Blake et al., 1995). There are also self-report assessment instruments available for assessing PTSD symptoms
in general, such as the Impact of Events Scale (Horowitz,
Wilner, & Alvarez, 1979), and for assessing PTSD symptoms
associated with specific trauma, such as the TSC-40 (Elliott &
Briere, 1992) and the PTSD Symptom Scale (PSS; Foa, Riggs,
Dancu, & Rothbaum, 1993 ) for sexual abuse or sexual assault
and the M M P I - P T S D (Keane et al., 1984) and M - P T S D
(Keane, Gerardi, et al., 1988) scales for combat trauma.
When inquiring about a history of trauma exposure, clinicians should consider the social stigma (e.g., feelings of shame
and guilt) that are associated with some traumatic experiences
(e.g., sexual assault) and the potential for underreporting such
events due to discomfort with the subject matter. This issue
highlights the importance of developing rapport with a patient
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
before inquiring about highly personal experiences. However,
concerns about underreporting must be balanced with concerns
about overreporting (see Briere & Zaidi, 1989, and Loftus,
1993, respectively). The effects of the use of leading questions
and probes on the accuracy of patients' reports of traumatic
history remain to be determined.
Clinicians working with individuals exposed to trauma
should carefully assess the individual's alcohol use patterns
(Nace, 1988). Information regarding the individual's use of alcohol before and after the trauma should be obtained. The frequency of drinking occasions and quantity of alcohol used per
occasion (e.g., Stewart, Peterson, et al., 1995; Vogel-Sprott,
1983 ), frequency of drinking to intoxication (e.g., Stewart, Peterson, et al., 1995 ), situational triggers for drinking (e.g., Annis, Graham, & Davis, 1987; Carrigan & Stewart, 1995; Stewart, 1995b), and self-perceived drinking motives (e.g., Cooper
et al., 1992; Stewart & Zeitlin, 1995; Stewart, Zeitlin, & Samoluk, 1996) should be assessed, as should any areas of functioning (i.e., social, occupational, and physical) that have been adversely affected through heavy alcohol use (Nace, 1988 ). There
are several interviews available for diagnosing alcohol abusedependence (for a review, see Jacobson, 1989). There are also
several self-report assessment instruments available for assessing problem drinking symptoms, such as the MAST (Seizer,
1971).
Assessment can serve as a useful bridge to treatment or as a
brief intervention in its own right. Most researchers and clinicians today espouse the view that alcoholism is a complex set of
behaviors associated with a wide variety of causes (e.g., Penk et
al., 1981 ). Clinicians should recognize that trauma exposure
and PTSD symptoms may be one possible pathway to the development of alcoholism. It is important to recognize that the motivations for alcohol abuse in this group may be distinct from
other subgroups of people who are alcohol abusers. Focusing on
the individual's motives for alcohol use during the interview can
serve as a first step in the intervention process (W. R. Miller,
1989). Providing information on the relationship between
PTSD symptoms and alcohol abuse, how the two disorders may
be sustaining each other, and advice on breaking this cycle can
be a useful step in increasing clients' motivation for change
(W. R. Miller, 1989).
Druley and Pashko ( 1988 ) highlighted the importance of accurate and early detection and triage of individuals who have
been exposed to a traumatic event, including implementation
of measures to prevent the development of alcohol-related
problems. They described a group of WW2 and Korean War
combat veterans, all over the age of 49, with chronic PTSD and
alcoholism diagnoses. PTSD in most of these veterans had been
undiagnosed for decades. These veterans had chronically
abused alcohol to manage their PTSD symptoms, resulting in
many hospital admissions for alcohol abuse and related medical
problems. Obviously, early detection of such patients, along
with the institution of effective interventions, could help to relieve the long-term maladjustment of many individuals exposed
to trauma and would relieve the burden on the health care system associated with the chronic care of such patients.
Treatment
In patients with comorbid diagnoses of PTSD and alcohol
abuse, treatment needs to address both disorders. Unfortu-
103
nately, many clinicians who specialize in the treatment of people exposed to trauma view alcohol abuse as secondary to
PTSD and thus assume that the drinking will normalize following the alleviation of PTSD symptoms (Brinson & Treanor,
1989; Gottheil, 1988; Hurley, 1991 ). There are several problems inherent in this viewpoint. Independent of etiological factors, once the abusive drinking has begun, the alcohol disorder
may take on a life of its own (Brinson & Treanor, 1989; Hurley,
1991; Nace, 1988; Pihl & Stewart, 1991 ). Thus, discontinuing
abusive drinking, even following PTSD symptom management,
is difficult for these individuals. Similarly, many substance
abuse clinicians focus on the alcohol problem and overlook the
PTSD symptoms (Brinson & Treanor, 1989; Hurley, 1991 ).
However, if the PTSD symptoms are not treated, the patient
may experience a reemergence or intensification of the distressing PTSD symptoms following sobriety (or during detoxification) and may again turn to alcohol for temporary symptom
relief.
Some researchers speculate that successful treatment of one
disorder may be hindered by the presence of a coexisting disorder (e.g., Davidson et al., 1990; Roy, 1984). Kuhne, Nohner,
and Baraga (1986) empirically evaluated this possibility by assessing the efficacy of chemical dependency treatment as a function of combat exposure in Vietnam veterans. In addition to
the chemical dependency treatment, all veterans were offered
concurrent trauma-oriented group therapy. Combat veterans
with varying degrees of combat exposure completed chemical
dependency treatment at equal rates and showed equal rates of
abstinence at a l-year follow-up posttreatment, suggesting no
effects of trauma severity on chemical dependency treatment
efficacy (at least when patients are offered concurrent traumaoriented therapy). However, this study was limited by a small
sample size and did not use rigorous measures of combat
exposure (see critique by Hyer et al., 1991 ). Moreover, given
data that PTSD symptoms, rather than trauma severity, appear
more highly related to posttraumatic alcohol abuse (e.g.,
McFall et al., 1992), the Kuhne et al. study is particularly lacking in its assessment of the effects of PTSD symptoms on the
efficacy of chemical dependency treatment.
Although most experts agree that both problems need to be a
focus of treatment when dealing with patients comorbid for
PTSD and alcohol abuse, there are divergent opinions about
which problem should be dealt with first or if both problems
should be treated simultaneously. Some experts recommend
treating the alcohol disorder first (e.g., Bartucci & Stewart,
1986; Brinson & Treanor, 1989; Nace, 1988; Schnitt & Nocks,
1984; Skorina & Kovach, 1986). Barlow (1988), for example,
suggested that the efficacy of psychological treatments for anxiety may be diminished when patients are concurrently taking
arousal-dampening drugs, such as alcohol. The efficacy of treatment for anxiety in general, and for PTSD in particular, in individuals who continue to abuse alcohol awaits empirical study.
Other experts suggested an integrated simultaneous treatment
approach, where both alcoholism and PTSD are a focus of
treatment from the outset (e.g., Hyer et al., 1991; Kuhne et al.,
1986; Turner & Colao, 1985). Intervention outcome studies in
which these various approaches to the treatment of dual diagnoses are empirically compared are required before statements
104
STEWART
about the superiority of one treatment approach over the other
can be made.
Clinicians should also be aware of issues related to the use of
benzodiazepines in patients comorbid for PTSD and alcoholism. Although benzodiazepines are effective in eliminating
some symptoms for some patients with PTSD, Friedman
(1981) recommended that, if used, they should be prescribed
with caution for patients with PTSD and concurrent alcoholism. In addition to the growing concerns about addiction and
withdrawal problems associated with benzodiazepine use
(Wardle, 1990), the benzodiazepines have dangerous effects
when combined with alcohol (Stewart, Pihl, et al., 1992).
Moreover, cross-tolerance effects between the two drugs may
mean that an alcohol-dependent patient with PTSD may require higher than normal levels of benzodiazepines to achieve
symptom management. Concerns have also been expressed that
the benzodiazepines may actually interfere with the efficacy of
psychological treatments for anxiety disorders (e.g., Wardle,
1990). Finally, Friedman ( 1988 ) noted that pharmacotherapy
is rarely sufficient to provide complete remission of PTSD
symptoms.
Hyer et al. ( 1991 ) recommended that the binge drinking pattern of patients with alcoholism and PTSD be a specific focus
of treatment. They recommended application of Marlatt and
Gordon's (1985) relapse prevention model. More specifically,
knowledge that these patients tend to drink heavily in an episodic fashion allows the clinician to assist them in identifying
high-risk situations that trigger their drinking and to educate
them in the use of alternative coping strategies to deal with these
situations when they occur. Similarly, Schnitt and Nocks
(1984) recommended making such patients aware of the link
between their abusive drinking and their memories of the traumatic event. They also recommended training comorbid patients in coping strategies to deal with urges to abuse alcohol
in these high-risk situations. If future researchers establish that
intrusive PTSD symptoms, negative affect, reminders of the
trauma, or all three serve as cues for eliciting binge drinking
behavior, then it may be useful to teach comorbid patients with
PTSD and alcoholism to attend to their reactions to these cues,
to conduct coping skills training in the presence of these cues,
and to combine exposure to these cues with response- (i.e.,
drinking-) prevention to reduce these cue reactivity effects
( Rohsenow et al., 1992).
Finally, some researchers commented on difficulties arising
in the detoxification phase in patients comorbid for alcoholism
and PTSD (e.g., Bartucci & Stewart, 1986; LaCoursiere et al.,
1980; Schnitt & Nocks, 1984). For example, Schnitt and Nocks
noted that patients with traumatic histories often experience a
sense of uncontrollable anxiety or insomnia during detoxification. In addition, Bartucci and Stewart cited data showing an
excess of REM sleep in people with chronic alcoholism undergoing detoxification (H. L. Williams & Rundell, 1981 ).
They argued that because patients with PTSD may be using
alcohol to suppress REM sleep and its associated nightmares,
this period of REM rebound may be particularly difficult for
them in attempting to discontinue alcohol abuse. A gradual tapering of alcohol intake is recommended, in combination with
preparing patients for these effects using psychoeducational and
cognitive-behavioral techniques (e.g., Otto, Pollack, MeltzerBrody, et al., 1992).
Summary
We require a good deal more knowledge about the nature
of the relationship between PTSD and alcohol abuse to better
structure therapeutic efforts to the special needs of these comorbid patients. Clinicians working with such populations should
be aware of the data currently available, indicating a high degree
of overlap between the two disorders and the characteristic
binge drinking profile of these patients. As is apparent in the
review above of current therapeutic strategies, clinicians are responsive to the emerging data on PTSD and alcoholism comorbidity and appear to be adapting their therapeutic strategies
appropriately.
Directions for Further Research
Throughout this review, a number of suggestions for future
research have been made. Important remaining issues are highlighted in the following section, including some comments on
suggested methodologies for examining these issues.
First, future comorbidity studies should attempt to use the
multimodal assessment method (Lyons et al., 1988) for establishing PTSD whenever possible. As part of this assessment, researchers should use validated measures, such as structured interviews, for establishing PTSD and alcohol abuse-dependence
diagnoses. In addition, more research should be conducted on
comorbid patients' perceptions of the nature of the relationship
between their two problems. Rachman's ( 1991 ) suggestions regarding suitable methodologies for studies on functional connectedness may prove useful.
Second, given the primary nature of intrusive cognitive
symptoms in PTSD, more emphasis should be placed on understanding the potential relationship between these symptoms
and the abuse of alcohol. Paradigms derived from experimental
cognitive psychology (such as the emotional Stroop task and
explicit and implicit memory tasks) are useful for studying the
nature of intrusive cognitive symptoms in patients with PTSD;
such paradigms might also prove useful in shedding light on the
nature of the relationship between intrusive cognitive symptoms and alcohol abuse in patients comorbid for PTSD and alcoholism. For example, patients with PTSD may be highly susceptible to abusive drinking because of the reduction in hypervigilance toward trauma-related cues that alcohol affords.
Moreover, given data on alcohol-induced reductions in cognitive biases for threatening information in participants with anxiety sensitivity (Stewart & Pihl, 1995 ), alcohol might similarly
reduce the cognitive biases for trauma-relevant information in
patients with PTSD, thereby controlling the cognitive reexperiencing symptoms of PTSD.
Third, this area could benefit greatly from studies using the
alcohol challenge paradigm (Newlin & Thomson, 1990) to determine whether patients with PTSD show responses to alcohol
that might begin to explain their propensity to develop posttraumatic drinking problems. Given that individuals are
not always aware of the motivations for their behaviors
(McClelland, 1985; Nisbett & Wilson, 1977), alcohol chal-
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
lenges could provide objective data to supplement data obtained through self-reports.
Fourth, even if it is established that alcohol administration
reduces or controls intrusive symptoms in patients with pTSD,
it remains to be established that these effects actually reinforce
their alcohol consumption. The stress-induced drinking methodology, using the alcohol taste-rating task as an unobtrusive
measure of alcohol consumption (see Higgins & Marlatt, 1973,
1975 ), could elucidate whether elicitation of PTSD symptoms
actually leads to increased drinking in the laboratory. Similarly,
this paradigm could be used to determine whether priming of
trauma-related concepts in memory (e.g., McNally et al., 1994)
leads to increased drinking in the laboratory.
Fifth, this area of research could benefit from an increased
focus on comparison of comorbidity rates across a variety of
traumatic events. Data suggest that some forms of trauma are
more likely to lead to PTSD than others (APA, 1994). Similarly, it is possible that some forms of trauma may be more
likely to result in alcohol abuse or comorbid PTSD and alcohol
abuse. The many dimensions of traumatic experience (e.g., duration of traumatic exposure, exposure severity, interpersonal
trauma vs. acts of God, and age at which the exposure took
place) need to be considered in future comorbidity research.
Exposure to atrocities and grotesque death appears to be one
type of traumatic experience that is strongly associated with
chronic PTSD (Yehuda, Southwick, & Giller, 1992). Preliminary data with combat veterans and individuals exposed to a
disaster (e.g., Green et al., 1985, 1989) also suggest a strong link
between exposure to grotesque death and posttraumatic alcohol
abuse. These findings should be followed up with studies of individuals exposed to other traumatic events to determine their
generalizability.
Sixth, this area of research would be enhanced by an increased focus on individual differences, such as gender and personality factors. For example, although overall men appear to
be at greater risk for the development of alcohol abuse problems
than women (Robins et al., 1984), women have been shown to
be at heightened risk for coping-related alcohol use-abuse when
compared with men (Cooper et al., 1992). No researchers have
directly examined gender differences in rates of comorbid
PTSD and alcohol abuse. This may be due to the fact that most
comorbidity studies have focused on either individuals exposed
to war-related combat (the large majority of whom are men) or
individuals exposed to sexual abuse-assault (the large majority
of whom are women). Examination of gender differences in comorbidity of alcohol abuse and PTSD diagnoses following traumas that occur more equally across genders might assist in addressing this issue.
Seventh, although there is a significant overlap between
PTSD and alcohol abuse, not all individuals with PTSD develop
drinking problems. An increased focus on individual differences that might predict which individuals develop posttraumatic drinking problems, given the presence of PTSD, would
be instructive. Investigation of potential protective factors (e.g.,
social support and early disclosure of the traumatic event)
might also be a useful avenue for research pursuit.
Finally, with regard to treatment, comparative efficacy studies on the various interventions designed to treat comorbid
PTSD and alcohol abuse should be conducted. When evaluat-
105
ing outcome, reduction in both symptoms of PTSD and abusive
drinking should be assessed. Furthermore, it remains to be determined whether approaches that focus on only one disorder
might also reduce symptoms of the other disorder (e.g., determining whether a treatment that focuses on only treating PTSD
symptoms might also have concomitant effects in reducing alcohol abuse symptoms; Gottheil, 1988). Moreover, it remains
to be established which individuals might benefit most from
which types of treatment approach.
Conclusions
This article reviews extant studies on the occurrence of alcohol abuse-dependence in individuals exposed to a wide variety
of traumatic events. In general, the data suggest that individuals
exposed to trauma are at greater risk for the development of
alcohol problems than are individuals not exposed. Data indicate that trauma severity is positively associated both with alcohol consumption following the trauma and with severity of alcohol-related problems. Even more compelling is the large body
of evidence supporting a relationship between PTSD and alcohol problems: Individuals with PTSD appear to be at significantly elevated risk for the development of alcohol-related problems when compared with both individuals exposed to trauma
without PTSD and those who have never been exposed to a
traumatic event. The most common notion proposed to account for the significant degree of comorbidity between PTSD
and alcoholism is the idea that these comorbid individuals are
abusing alcohol to self-medicate their PTSD symptoms. Selfmedication in patients with PTSD might be achieved through
alcohol's effects in reducing physiological reactivity, behavioral
avoidance, negative affect, or cognitive symptoms. Alternatively, alcohol abuse-withdrawal might serve to trigger or exacerbate PTSD symptoms. The contrasting notions of self-medication and alcohol-induced intensification of PTSD symptoms
in patients can be reconciled if data can be obtained that both
processes may be operative in a cyclical fashion. This area of
research could be improved through studies using superior
methodologies for assessment of PTSD and alcoholism, a
greater emphasis on comorbid patients' perceptions of the functional connection between their two disorders, emphasis on individual differences, and the use of experimental methods to
assess the effects of alcohol in patients with PTSD, including
the application of experimental cognitive paradigms and the alcohol challenge and stress-induced drinking methodologies.
The large body of data supporting a link between PTSD and
alcoholism has important assessment and treatment implications. Clinicians are cautioned against viewing either diagnosis
as primary in terms of treatment. Emphasis should be placed
on therapy approaches designed to address both problems. As
greater knowledge is gleaned on the nature of the relationship
between PTSD and alcoholism, assessment and treatment techniques can be further improved to better serve the needs of individuals simultaneously afflicted with these two behavioral
disorders.
References
Abrams, D. B., & Wilson, G. T. (1979). Effects of alcohol on social
anxiety in women: Cognitive versus physiological processes. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 161-173.
106
STEWART
Adams, E R., & Adams, G. R. (1984). Mount Saint Helens' ashfaU:
Evidence for a disaster stress reaction. American Psychologist, 39,
252-260.
Alexanderson, G., & Lindman, R. (1980). Of mice and women: II.
Differential effects of dose information and administration of alcohol
on fear. In R. Lindman (Ed.), Anxiety and alcohok Limitations of
tension reduction theory in non-alcoholics (Research Monograph No.
1, pp. 75-82). Abo, Finland: Abo Akademi, Department of
Psychology.
American Medical Association. ( 1993 ). Adolescents as victims of family violence. Journal of the American Medical Association, 270,
1850-1856.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders ( 3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. ( 1987 ). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders ( 3rd ed., rev. ). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders ( 4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Amir, N., McNally, R. J., & Wiegartz, P. (1995). Implicit memory bias
for threat in posttraumatic stress disorde~ Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Annis, H., Graham, J., & Davis, C. ( 1987 ). Inventory of Drinking Situations (IDS) user's guide. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Addiction Research Foundation.
Baker, T. B, Morse, E., & Sherman, J. E. (1987). The motivation to use
drugs: A psychobiological analysis of urges. In P. C. Rivers (Ed.), The
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Alcohol use and abuse (pp.
257-323 ). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Barlow, D. H. ( 1988 ). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic. New York: Guilford Press.
Bartucci, R. J., & Stewart, J. T. (1986). Drs. Bartucci and Stewart reply
[Letter to the editor]. American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 1203.
Beckman, L. J. (1976). Alcoholism problems and women: An overview.
In M. Greenblatt & M. A. Schuckit (Eds.), Alcoholism problems in
women and children (pp. 65-95 ). New York: Grune & Stratton.
Benson, C. S., & Wilsnack, S. C. ( 1983 ). Gender differences in alcoholic
personality characteristics and life experiences. In W. M. Cox (Ed.),
Identifying and measuring alcoholic personality characteristics ( pp.
53-68 ). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Bernet, C. A., Deutseher, R., Ingram, R. E., & Litrownik, A. J. ( 1993,
November). Differential factors in the repression of memories of
childhood sexual abuse. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Atlanta, GA.
Blake, D. D., Weathers, E W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, D. G., Gusman,
E D., Charney, D. S., & Keane, T. M. (1995). The development of a
clinician-administered PTSD scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8.
75-90.
Boman, B. (1986). Combat stress, posttraumatic stress disorder, and
associated psychiatric disturbance. Psychosomatics, 27, 567-573.
Boscarino, J. ( 1981 ). Current excessive drinking among Vietnam veterans: A comparison with other veterans and non-veterans. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 27, 204-212.
Branchey, L., Davis, W., & Lieber, C. S. (1984). Alcoholism in Vietnam
and Korea veterans: A long-term follow-up. Alcoholism. Clinical and
Experimental Research, 8, 572-575.
Breslau, N., & Davis, G. C. (1992). Posttraumatic stress disorder in an
urban population of young adults: Risk factors for chronicity. American Journal of Psychiatry" 149, 671-675.
Breslau, N., Davis, G. C., Andreski, P., & Peterson, E. ( 1991 ). Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban population of young adults. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 216-222.
Brett, E. A., & Ostroff, R. (1985). Imagery and posttraumatic stress
disorder: An overview. American Journal Of Psychiatry, 142, 41742~4.
Briere, J. ( 1988 ). Long-term clinical correlates of childhood sexual victimization. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 528, 327334.
Briere, J. (1992). Methodological issues in the study of sexual abuse
effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 196-203.
Briere, J., & Conte, J. (1993). Self-reported amnesia for abuse in adults
molested as children. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 21-31.
Briere, J., & Elliott, D. M. (1993). Sexual abuse, family environment,
and psychological symptoms: On the validity of statistical control.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 284-288.
Briere, J., & Zaidi, L. Y. ( 1989 ). Sexual abuse histories and sequelae in
female psychiatric emergency room patients. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 146, 1602-1606.
Brinson, T., & Treanor, V. (1989). Alcoholism and posttraumatic stress
disorder among combat Vietnam veterans. Alcoholism Treatment
Quarterly, 5, 65-82.
Bryant, R. A., & Harvey, A. G. (1995). Processing threatening information in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 537-541.
Calahan, D. (1970). Problem drinkers. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Cappell, H., & Greeley, J. (1987). Alcohol and tension reduction: An
update on research and theory. In H. R. Blane & K. E. Leonard
(Eds.), Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism (pp. 1554). New York: Guilford Press.
Cappell, H., & Herman, C. R (1972). Alcohol and tension reduction:
A review. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 33, 33-64.
Carlson, E. B., & Rosser-Hogan, R. ( 1994 ). Cross-cultural response to
trauma: A study of traumatic experiences and posttraumatic symptoms in Cambodian refugees. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 7( 1 ), 4358.
Carrigan, G., & Stewart, S. H. ( 1995, November). Situational antecedents to drinking behavior among university students: A psychometric
evaluation of the Inventory of Drinking Situations (IDS). Paper presented at the International Conference on Social and Health Effects
of Different Drinking Patterns, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Chemtob, L., Roitblatt, H. L., Himada, R. S., Carlson, J. G., & Twentyman, C. T. (1989). A cognitive action theory of posttraumatic
stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 2, 253-275.
Clark, D. B., & Jacob, R. G. (1992). Anxiety disorders in 30 adolescents with alcohol abuse and dependence [Summary]. Alcoholism:
Clinical and Experimental Research, 16, 371.
Cohen, E S., & Densen-Gerber, J. (1982). A study oftbe relationship
between child abuse and drug addiction in 178 patients: Preliminary
results. Child Abuse and Neglect, 6, 383-387.
Conger, J. J. ( 1951 ). The effects of alcohol on conflict behavior in the
albino rat. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 12, 1-29.
Cooper, M. L., Russell, M., Skinner, J. B., & Windle, M. (1992). Development and validation of a three-dimensional measure of drinking
motives. PsychologicalAssessment, 4, 123-132.
Cottler, L. B., Compton, W. M., Mager, D., Spitznagel, E. L., & Janca,
A. ( 1992 ). Posttraumatic stress disorder among substance users from
the general population. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 664670.
Covington, S. S. (1983). Sexual experience, dysfunction and abuse: A
descriptive study of alcoholic and nonalcoholic women [Summary ].
Dissertation Abstracts International, 44, 287B.
Cox, B. J., Norton, G. R., Dorward, J., & Fergusson, P. (1989). The
relationship between panic attacks and chemical dependencies. Addictive Behaviors. 14, 53-60.
Cox, B. J., Norton, G. R., Swinson, R. P., & Endler, N. A. (1990).
Substance abuse and panic-related anxiety: A critical review. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 385-393.
Cox, B. J., Swinson, R. R, Shulman, I. D., Kuch, K., & Reichman, J. T.
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
( 1993 ). Gender effects and alcohol use in panic disorder with agoraphobia. BehaviourResearch and Therapy, 31, 413-416.
Cox, W. M. (1987). Personality theory and research. In H. T. Blane &
K. E. Leonard (Eds.), Psychologicaltheories of drinking and alcoholism (pp. 55-89). New York: Guilford Press.
Curran, H. V. ( 1991 ). Benzodiazepines, memory and mood: A review.
Psychopharmacology, 105, 1-8.
Danion, J. M., Zimmerman, M. A., Willard-Schroeder, D., Grange, C.,
& Singer, L. (1989). Diazepam induces a dissociation between explicit and implicit memory. Psychopharmacology, 99, 238-243.
Davidson, J. R. T., & Fairbank, J. A. (1993). The epidemiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. In J. R. T. Davidson & E. B. Foa (Eds.),
Posttraumatic stress disorder: DSM-IV and beyond (pp. 147-169).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Davidson, J. R. T., Kudler, H. S., Saunders, W. B., & Smith, R. D.
(1990). Symptom and comorbidity patterns in World War II and
Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Comprehensive
Psychiatry, 31, 162-170.
Davidson, J. R. T., Swartz, M., Storck, M., Krishnan, R. R., & Hammett, E. (1985). A diagnostic and family study of posttraumatic
stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 90-93.
Dembo, R., Dertke, M., Borders, S., Washburn, M., & Schmeidler, J.
( 1988 ). The relationship between physical and sexual abuse and tobacco, alcohol, and illicit drug use among youths in a juvenile detention center. International Journal of the Addictions, 23, 351-378.
Dembo, R., Williams, L., Wothke, W., Schmeidler, J., & Brown, C. H.
(1992). The role of family factors, physical abuse and sexual victimization experiences in high-risk youths' alcohol and other drug use
and delinquency: A longitudinal model. Violence and Victims. 7,
245-266.
Dengerink, H. A., & Fagan, N, J. (1978). Effects of alcohol on emotional responses to stress. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 39, 525-539.
Druley, K. A., & Pashko, S. (1988). Posttraumatic stress disorder in
World War lI and Korean combat veterans with alcohol dependency.
In M. Galanter (Ed.), Recent developments in alcoholism (Vol. 6, pp.
89- l 01 ). New York: Plenum Press.
Elliott, D. M., & Briere, J. (1992). Sexual abuse trauma among professional women: Validating the Trauma Symptom Checklist-40 (TSC40 ). Child Abuse and Neglect, 16, 391-398.
Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. ( 1978 ). A diagnostic interview: The Schedule
for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (SADS). Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837-844.
Escobar, J. I., Randolph, E. T., Peunte, G., Spiwak, F., Asamen, J. K.,
Hill, M., & Hough, R. L. (1983). Posttraumatic stress disorder in
Hispanic Vietnam veterans: Clinical phenomenology and sociocultural characteristics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 171,
585-596.
Evans, S. (1988). Shame, boundaries and dissociation in chemically
dependent, abusive and incestuous families. Alcoholism Treatment
Quarterly, 5, 157-179.
Fang, J. C., Hinrichs, J. V., & Ghoneim, M. M. ( 1987 ). Diazepam and
memory: Evidence for spared memory function. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 28, 347-352.
Fidler, T., & LoLordo, V. (1996). Failure to find postshock increases in
alcohol preference. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
20(1), 110-121.
Finkelhor, D. (1979). Sexually victimized children. New York: Free
Press.
Finn, P. R., Martin, J., & Pihl, R. O. (1987). Alexithymia in males at
high genetic risk for alcoholism. Psychotherapy.and Psychosomatics,
47, 18-21.
Finn, P. R., & Pihl, R. O. (1987). Men at high risk for alcoholism: The
effect of alcohol on cardiovascular response to unavoidable shock.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 230-236.
107
Finn, P. R., & Pihl, R. O. (1988). Risk for alcoholism: A comparison
between two different groups of sons of alcoholics on cardiovascular
reactivity and sensitivity to alcohol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 12, 742-747.
Finn, P. R., Zeitouni, N. C., & Pihl, R. O. (1990). Effects of alcohol on
psychophysiological hyperreactivity to nonaversive and aversive stimuli in men at high risk for alcoholism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 79-85.
Flanigan, B. J., Potrykus, P. A., & Marti, D. (1988). Alcohol and marijuana use among female adolescent incest victims. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 5, 231-248.
Foa, E. B., Feske, U., Murdock, T. B., Kozak, M. J., & McCarthy, P. R.
(1991). Processing of threat-related information in rape victims.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, I00, 156-162.
Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Dancu, C. V., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1993). Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic
stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 459-473,
Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1989). Behavioral/
cognitive conceptualizations of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 20, 155-176.
Foa, E, B., Zinbarg, R., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1992). Uncontrollability
and unpredictability in posttraumatic stress disorder: An animal
model. PsychologicalBulletin, 112, 218-238.
Frankel, E H. (1993). Adult reconstruction of childhood events in the
multiple personality literature. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150,
954-958.
Freed, E. X. (1978). Alcohol and mood: An updated review. International Journal of the Addictions, 13, 173-200.
Friedman, M. J. (1981). Post-Vietnam syndrome: Recognition and
management. Psychosomatics, 22, 931-943.
Friedman, M. J. (1988). Toward rational pharmacotherapy for posttraumatic stress disorder: An interim report. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 145, 281-285.
Friedman, M. J. (1990). Biological approaches to the diagnosis and
treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic
Stress, 4, 67-91.
Fullilove, M. T., Fullilove, R. E., I11, Smith, M., Winkler, K., Michael,
C., Panzer, P. G., & Wallace, R. (1993). Violence, trauma, and posttraumatic stress disorder among women drug u~rs. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 6, 533-543.
Gagnon, J. (1965). Female child victims of sex offenses. Social Problems. 13, 176-192.
George, D. T., Nutt, D. J., Dwyer, B. Z., & Linnoila, M. (1990). Alcoholism and panic disorder: Is the comorbidity more than coincidence? Acta Psychiatrica Scandanavia, 81, 97-107.
George, D. T., Zerby, A., Noble, S., & Nutt, D. J. (1988). Panic attacks
and alcohol withdrawal: Can subjects differentiate the symptoms? Biological Psychiatry, 24, 240-243.
Gignac, M., & Stewart, S. H. (1995). Effects of emotional valence of
words on explicit versus implicit memory performance [Summary].
Canadian Psychology, 36 (2a), 75.
Gleser, G. C., Green, B. L., & Winget, C. ( 1981 ). Prolongedpsychosocial effects of disaster: A study of Buffalo Creek. New York: Academic
Press.
Goldberg, L. (1966). Behavioral and physiological effects of alcohol on
man. PsychosomaticMedicine, 28, 590-595.
Golombok, S., Stavrou, A., & Bonn, J. ( 1991 ). The effects ofdiazepam
on anxiety-related cognition. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15,
459-467.
Gottheil, E. (1988). Overview. In M. Galanter (Ed.), Recent developments in alcoholism (Vol. 6, pp. 3-7 ). New York: Plenum Press.
Gray, J. A. ( 1982 ). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An inquiry into the
fimctions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford, England: Oxford
University Press.
108
STEWART
Gray, J. A. (1985). Issues in the neuropsychology of anxiety, In A. H.
Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp.
5-25 ). Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum.
Green, B. L., Grace, M. C., & Gleser, G. ( 1985 ). Identifying survivors
at risk: Long-term impairment following the Beverly Hills Supper
Club fire. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 672678.
Green, B. L., Grace, M. C., Lindy, J. D., Gleser, G. C., & Leonard,
A. (1990). Risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder and other
diagnoses in a general sample of Vietnam veterans. American Journal
of P~ychiatry, 147, 729-733.
Green, B. L., Lindy, J. D., Grace, M. C., & Gleser, G. C. (1989).
Multiple diagnoses in posttraumatic stress disorder: The role of war
stressors. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 177, 329-335.
Green, B. L., Lindy, J. D., Grace, M. C., & Leonard, A. C. (1992).
Chronic posttraumatic stress disorder mad diagnostic comorbidity in
a disaster sample. Journal of Nervous andMental Disease. 180, 760766.
Greenstein, R. A., Kitchner, I., & Olsen, K. (1986). Posttraumatic
stress disorder, partial complex seizures, and alcoholism. American
Journal ofPsychiatry, 143, 1203.
Harrison, P. A., Hoffman, N. G., & Edwall, G. E. (1989). Differential
drug use patterns among sexually abused adolescent girls in treatment for chemical dependency. International Journal of the Addictions, 24, 499-514.
Hashtroudi, S., Parker, E. S., DeLisi, L. E., Wyatt, R. J., & Mutter, S. A.
(1984). Intact retention in acute alcohol amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology.Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 156-163.
Haviland, M. G., MacMurray, J. P., & Cummings, M. A. (1988). The
relationship between alexithymia and depressive symptoms in a sample of newly abstinent alcoholic inpatients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 49, 37-40.
Helzer, J. E., Robins, L. N., & McEvoy, L. (1987). Posttraumatic stress
disorder in the general population: Findings of the Epidemiologic
Catchment Area survey. New England Journal of Medicine, 317,
1630-1634.
Hendin, H., & Haas, A. P. (1984). Wounds of war: The psychological
aftermath of combat in Vietnam. New York: Basic Books.
Herman, J. L. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: HarperCollins.
Herman, J. L., & Schatzow, E. (1987). Recovery and verification of
memories of childhood sexual trauma. PsychoanalyticPsychology, 4,
1-14.
Hernandez, J. T. ( 1992 ). Substance abuse among sexually abused adolescents and their families. Journal of Adolescent Health, 13, 658662.
Hernandez, J. T., Lodico, M., & DiClemente, R. J. (1993). The effects
of child abuse and race on risk-taking in male adolescents. Journal of
the National Medical Association, 85, 593-597.
Higgins, R. L., & Marlatt, G. A. (1973). Effects of anxiety arousal on
the consumption of alcohol by alcoholics and social drinkers. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 41,426-433.
Higgins, R. L., & Marlatt, G. A. (1975). Fear of interpersonal evaluation as a determinant of alcohol consumption in male social drinkers.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 644-651.
Hodgson, R., Stockwell, T. R., & Rankin, H. J. (1979). Can alcohol
reduce tension? BehaviourResearch and Therapy, 17, 459-466.
Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The Social Readjustment Rating
Scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213-218.
Horn, J., Wanberg, K., & Foster, E (1987). Guide to theAlcohol Use
Inventory Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems.
Horowitz, M., Wilner, N., & Alvarez, W. (1979). Impact of Event Scale:
A measure of subjective distress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 209218.
Hull, J. G., & Bond, C. E (1986). Social and behavioral consequences
of alcohol consumption and expectancy: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 347-360.
Hurley, D. L. ( 1991 ). Women, alcohol and incest: An analytical review.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 52, 253-268.
Hyer, L., Leach, P., Boudewyns, P. A., & Davis, H. (1991). Hidden
posttraumatic stress disorder in substance abuse inpatients among
Vietnam veterans. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 8, 213219.
Hyer, L., Woods, M. G., Summers, M. N., Boudewyns, P., & Harrison,
W. R. (1990). Alexithymia among Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 51, 243-247.
Jacobson, G. R. (1989). A comprehensive approach to pretreatment
evaluation: I. Detection, assessment, and diagnosis of alcoholism. In
R. K. Hester & W. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of alcoholism treatment approaches: Effective alternatives (pp. 17-66). New York: Pergamon Press.
Jacoby, L. L., Allan, L. G., Collins, J. C., & Larwill, L. K. (1988).
Memory influences subjective experience: Noise judgments. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14,
240-247.
Jellinek, J. M., & Williams, T. (1984). Posttraumatic stress disorder
and substance abuse in Vietnam combat veterans: Treatment problems, strategies and recommendations. Journal of Substance Abuse
Treatment, 1, 87-97.
Jones, B. M. (1973). Memory impairment on the ascending and descending limbs of the blood alcohol curve. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 82, 24-32.
Jones, B., Parsons, O., & Rundell, O. ( 1976 ). Psychophysiological correlates of alcoholism. In R. E. Tarter & A. Sugarman (Eds.), Alcoholism: Interdisciplinary approaches to an enduring problem (pp. 435477). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Josephs, R. A., & Steele, C. M. (1990). The two faces of alcohol myopia: Attentional mediation of psychological stress. Journal of Abnorreal Psychology, 99, 115-126.
Kalin, R., McClelland, D. C., & Kahn, M. ( 1965 ). The effects of male
social drinking on fantasy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 1,441-452.
Karp, J. (1993). The interaction of alcohol expectancies, personality,
and psychopathology among inpatient alcoholics [Summary]. Dissertation Abstracts International, 53, 4375B.
Kaspi, S. P., McNally, R. J., & Amir, N. (1995). Cognitive processing
of emotional information in posttraumatic stress disorder. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 19, 433-444.
Keane, T. M., CaddeU, J. M., & Martin, B. (1983). Substance abuse
among Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorders. Bulletin of Social Psychologyand Addictive Behavior, 2, 117-122.
Keane, T. M., Caddell, J. M., & Taylor, K. L. (1988). Mississippi Scale
for Combat-Related Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Three studies in
reliability and validity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 56, 85-90.
Keane, T. M., Gerardi, R. J., Lyons, J. A., & Wolfe, J. (1988). The interrelationship of substance abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder:
Epidemiological and clinical considerations. In M. Galanter (Ed.),
Recent developments in alcoholism (Vol. 6, pp. 27-48 ). New York:
Plenum.
Keane, T. M., & Lisman, S. (1980). Alcohol and social anxiety in males:
Behavioral, cognitive, and physiological effects. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 89, 213-223.
Keane, T. M., Malloy, P. E, & Fairbank, J. A. (1984). Empirical development of an MMPI subscale for the assessment of combat-related
posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 888-891.
Khantzian, E. J. (1985). The self-medication hypothesis of addictive
disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 1259-1264.
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
Kilpatrick, D. J., & Resnick, H. S. ( 1993 ). Posttraumatic stress disorder associated with exposure to criminal victimization in clinical and
community populations. In J. R. T. Davidson & E. B, Foa (Eds.),
Posttraumatic stress disorder: DSM-I V and beyond (pp. 113-143 ).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Klassen, A. D., & Wilsnack, S. C. (1986). Sexual experiences and
drinking among women in a U.S. national survey. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 15. 363-392.
Kolb, L. C. (1985). The place of narcosynthesis in the treatment of
chronic and delayed stress reactions of war. In S. M. Sonnenberg, A. S.
Blank, & J. A. Talbott (Eds.), The trauma of war: Stress' and recovery
in Vietnam veterans. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Kolb, L. C. ( 1993 ). The psychobiology ofposttraumatic stress disorder:
Perspectives and reflections on the past, present, and future. Journal
of Traumatic Stress, 6, 293-304.
Kosten, T. R., & Krystal, J. (1988). Biological mechanisms in posttraumatic stress disorder: Relevance for substance abuse. In M. Galanter
(Ed.), Recent developments in alcoholism (Vol. 6, pp. 49-68 ). New
York: Plenum Press.
Kovach, J. A. (1986). Incest as a treatment issue for alcoholic women.
Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 3, 1-15.
Kramer, M., Schoen, L. S., & Kinney, L. (1984). The dream experience
in dream-disturbed Vietnam veterans. In B. A. van der Kolk (Ed.),
Posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological and biological sequelae
(pp. 82-95 ). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Krystal, H. ( 1979 ). Alexithymia and psychotherapy. American Journal
of Psychotherapy, 33, 17-31.
Krystal, H. (1982). Alexithymia and the effectiveness of psychoanalytic
treatment. International Journal of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 9,
353-378.
Krystal, H. (1984). Psychoanalytic views on human emotional damages. In B. A. van der Kolk (Ed.), Posttraumatic stress disorder: Psychological and biological sequelae (pp. 1-28). Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Press.
Kuhne, A., Nohner, W., & Baraga, E. (1986). Efficacy of chemical dependency treatment as a function of combat in Vietnam. Journal of
Substance Abuse Treatment. 3, 191-194.
Kulka, R. A., Schlenger, W. E., Fairbank, J. A., Hough, R. L., Jordan,
B. K., Marmar, C. R., & Weiss, D. S. (1990). Trauma andthe Vietnam War generation: Report of findings from the National Vietnam
Veterans Readjustment Study, New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Kushner, M. G., Sher, K. J., & Beitman, B. D. (1990). The relation
between alcohol problems and the anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 685-695.
LaCoursiere, R. B., & Coyne, L. (1984). Comment on Roy's "Alcohol
misuse and posttraumatic stress disorder (delayed): An alternative
interpretation of the data" Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 45, 283284.
LaCoursiere, R. B., Godfrey, K. E., & Ruby, L. M. (1980). Traumatic
neurosis in the etiology of alcoholism: Vietnam and other trauma.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 966-968.
Lang, P. J. ( 1968 ). Fear reduction and fear behavior: Problems in treating a construct. In J. M. Schlien (Ed.), Research in psychotherapy
(Vol. 3, pp. 90-102). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Laufer, R. S., Yager, T., Frey-Wouters, E., & Donnellan, J. ( 1981 ). Postwar trauma: Social and psychological problems of Vietnam veterans
in the aftermath of the Vietnam War. In A. Egendorf, C. Kadushin,
R. S. Laufer, G. Rothbart, & L. Sloan (Eds.), Legacies of Vietnam:
Vol. 3. The final report of the Veterans Administration (pp. 50-53).
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Legrand, R., Vidailher, P., Danion, J.-M., Grange, D., Giersch, A., Van
Der Linden, M., & Imbs, J.-L. ( 1995 ). Time course of the effects of
109
diazepam and lorazepam on perceptual priming and explicit memory. Psychopharmacology, 118, 475-479.
Levenson, R. W., Sher, K. J., Grossman, L. M., Newman, J., & Newlin,
D. B. (1980). Alcohol and stress-response dampening: Pharmacological effects, expectancy, and tension reduction. Journal of Abnorreal Psychology, 89, 528-538.
Lindman, R., Alexanderson, G., & Kvarnstrom, B. (1979). Skin conductance as a function of alcohol during motor approach towards a
noxious goal. Scandanavian Journal of Psychology, 20, 113-117.
Lindy, J. D., Grace, M. C., & Green, B. L. (1984). Building a conceptual bridge between civilian trauma and war trauma: Preliminary
psychological findings from a clinical sample of Vietnam veterans. In
B. A. van der Kolk (Ed.), Posttraumatic stress disorder." Psychological
and biological sequelae (pp. 43-58). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Press.
Litz, B. T., & Keane, T. M. (1989). Information processing in anxiety
disorders: Application to the understanding of posttraumatic stress
disorder. Clinical Psychology Review,, 9, 243-257.
Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48, 518-537.
Loftus, E. E, Garry, M., & Feldman, J. (1994). Forgetting sexual
trauma: What does it mean when 38% forget? Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1177-1181.
Logan, A. C., & Goetsch, V. L. ( 1993 ). Attention to external threat cues
in anxiety states. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 541-559.
Lyons, J. A., Gerardi, R. J., Wolfe, J., & Keane, T. M. (1988). Multidimensional assessment of combat-related PTSD: Phenomenological,
psychometric and psychophysiological considerations. Journal of
Traumatic Stress, 1,373-394.
MacDonald, A., Stewart, S. H., Samoluk, S. B., & Sweet, S. (1995).
Gender differences in consistency of alcohol consumption measures
[Summary]. Canadian Psychology, 36(2a), 65.
Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (1985). Relapse prevention: Maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive behaviors. New York:
Guilford Press.
Madatt, G. A., & Rohsenow, D. (1980). Cognitive processes in alcohol
use: Expectancy and the balanced placebo design. In N. K. MeUo
(Ed.), Advances in substance abuse: Behavioral and biological research. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Masserman, J. H., & Yum, K. S. (1946). An analysis of the influence of
alcohol on experimental neurosis in cats. Psychosomatic Medicine, 8,
36-52.
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion
and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25-50.
McClelland, D. C. (1985). How motives, skills, and values determine
what people do. American Psychologist, 40, 812-825.
McFall, M. E., Mackay, P. W., & Donovan, D. M. (1992). Combatrelated posttraumatic stress disorder and severity of substance abuse
in Vietnam veterans. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 53, 357-363.
McNally, R. J. (1992). Anxiety sensitivity distinguishes panic disorder
from generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 180, 737-738.
McNally, R. J. (1994). Panic disorder: A critical analysis. New York:
Guilford Press.
McNally, R. J. (1996). Anxiety sensitivity is distinguishable from trait
anxiety. In R. M. Rapee (Ed.), Current controversies in the anxiety
disorders (pp. 214-227 ). New York: Guilford Press.
McNally, R. J., English, G. E., & Lipke, H. J. (1993). Assessment of
intrusive cognition in posttraumatic stress disorder: Use of the modified Stroop paradigm. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 33-41.
McNally, R. J., Litz, B. T., Prassas, A., Shin, L. M., & Weathers, E W.
(1994). Emotional priming of autobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 351-367.
McNally, R. J., Luedke, D. L., Besyner, J. K., Peterson, R. A., Bohm,
1 10
STEWART
K., & Lips, O. J. ( 1987 ). Sensitivity to stress-relevant stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 1, 105-116.
McNally, R. J., & Shin, L. M. ( 1995 ). Association of intelligence with
severity of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in Vietnam combat veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 936-938.
McNamee, H., Mello, N,, & Mendelson, J. ( 1968 ). Experimental analysis of drinking patterns of alcoholics: Concurrent psychiatric observations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 124, 1079-1081.
Mello, M. K. ( 1981 ). The role ofaversive consequences in the control
of alcohol and drug self-administration. In R. E. Meyer, B. C. Gulek,
J. E. O'Brien, & T. E Babor (Eds.), Evaluation of the alcoholic. Implications for research, theory and treatment (pp, 207-228).
Bethesda, MD: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.
Mendelson, J., LaDou, J., & Solomon, P. (1964). Experimentally induced chronic intoxication and withdrawal in alcoholics: III. Psychiatric findings. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol (Suppl. 2 ),
40-52.
Mieselman, D. (1978). Incest." A psychological study of causes and
effects with treatment recommendations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Miller, B. A., Downs, W. R., & Testa, M. (1993). Inter-relationships
between victimization experiences and women's alcohol use. Journal
of Studies on Alcohol (Suppl. 11 ), 109-117.
Miller, W. R. (1989). Increasing motivation for change. In R. K. Hester
& W. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of alcoholism treatment approaches: Effective alternatives (pp. 67-80). New York: Pergamon
Press.
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304-311.
Murdoch, D., Pihl, R. O., & Ross, D. (1990). Alcohol and crimes of
violence: Present issues. International Journal of the Addictions, 25,
1065-1081.
Nace, E. P. ( 1988 ). Posttraumatic stress disorder and substance abuse:
Clinical issues. In M. Galanter (Ed.), Recent developments in alcoholism (Vol. 6, pp. 9-26). New York: Plenum Press.
Nemiah, J. C., & Sifneos, P. E. (1970). Affect and fantasy in patients
with psychosomatic disorder. In O. W. Hill (Ed.), Modern trends in
psychosomatic medicine (Vol. 2, pp. 26-34). London: Butterworths.
Newlin, D. B., & Thomson, J. B. (1990). Alcohol challenge with sons
of alcoholics: A critical review and analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
108, 383-402.
Niatoh, P. (1972). The effect of alcohol on the autonomic nervous system of humans: Psychophysiological approach. In B. Kissin & H.
Begleiter (Eds.), The biology of alcoholism: Vol. 2. Physiology and
behavior ( pp. 367-433 ). New York: Plenum Press.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know:
Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review,, 84, 231259.
Norris, F. H. ( 1992 ). Epidemiology of trauma: Frequency and impact
of different potentially traumatic events on different demographic
groups. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 409-418.
Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., Meltzer-Brody, S., & Rosenbaum, J. E
( 1992 ). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for benzodiazepine discontinuation in panic disorder patients. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 28,
123-130.
Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., Sachs, G. S., O'Neil, C. A., & Rosenbaum,
J. E ( 1992 ). Alcohol dependence in panic disorder patients. Journal
of Psychiatric Research, 26, 29-38.
Penk, W. E., Robinowitz, R., Roberts, W. R., Patterson, E. T., Dolan,
M. P., & Atkins, H. G. ( 1981 ). Adjustment differences among male
substance abusers varying in degree of combat experience in Vietnam. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 426-437.
Persson, L. O., Sjoberg, L., & Svensson, E. (1980). Mood effects of alcohol. Psychopharmacology, 68, 295-299.
Peterson, R. A., & Reiss, S. (1992). Anxiety Sensitivity Index manual
(2nd ed.). Worthington, OH: International Diagnostic Systems.
Peyser, H. S. (1982). Stress and alcohol. In L. Goldberg & S. Breznotz
(Eds.), Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects ( pp. 585598 ). New York: Free Press.
Pihl, R. O., & Peterson, J. B. (1995). Alcoholism: The role of different
motivational systems. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 20,
372-396.
Pihl, R. O., Peterson, J. B., & Lau, M. A. ( 1993, September). A biosocial model of the alcohol-aggression relationship. Journal of Studies
on Alcohol ( Suppl. 11 ), 128-139.
Pihl, R. O., & Smith, S. (1983). Of affect and alcohol. In L. A. Pohorecky & J. Brick (Eds.), Stress and alcohol use (pp. 203-228). New
York: Elsevier.
Pihl, R. O., & Stewart, S. H. ( 1991 ). Substance abuse and behavioral
pathology: A commentary. Annual Review of Addictions Research
and Treatment, 1, 153-156.
Pitman, R. K., van der Kolk, B. A., & Orr, S. P. (1990). Nalaxonereversible analgesic response to combat-related stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: A pilot study. Archives of General Psychiatry,
47, 541-547.
Pribor, E. E, & Dinwiddie, S. H. ( 1992 ). Psychiatric correlates of incest
in childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 52-56.
Rachman, S. J. ( 1991 ). A psychological approach to the study of comorbidity. Clinical Psychology Review,, 11, 461-464.
Rankin, H., Hodgson, R., & Stockwell, T. (1979). The concept of craving and its measurement. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 17, 389396.
Reiss, S. ( 1987 ). Theoretical perspectives on the fear of anxiety. Clinical
Psychology Review,, 7, 585-596.
Reiss, S. ( 1991 ). Expectancy model of fear, anxiety and panic. Clinical
Psychology Review, 11, 141 - 153.
Reiss, S., & McNally, R. J. ( 1985 ). Expectancy model of fear. In S. Reiss
& R. R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical issues in behavior therapy (pp.
107-121 ). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Reiss, S., Peterson, R. A., Gursky, D. M., & McNally, R. J. (1986).
Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 1-8.
Riemann, B. C., McNaUy, R. J., & Cox, W. M. ( 1992 ). The comorbidity
of obsessive-compulsive disorder and alcoholism. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 6, 105-110.
Rimm, D., Bridell, D., Zimmerman, M., & Caddy, G. (1981). The
effects of alcohol and the expectancy of alcohol on snake fear. Addictive Behaviors, 6, 47-51.
Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Croughan, J., & Ratcliff, K. S. ( 1981 ).
National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule:
Its history, characteristics, and validity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 381-389.
Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Weissman, M. M., Orvaschel, H., Gruenberg, E., Burke, J. D., & Regier, D. A. (1984). Lifetime prevalence of
specific psychiatric disorders in three sites. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41,949-958,
Roediger, H. L., & Blaxton, T. A. ( 1987 ). Effects of varying modality,
surface features, and retention interval on priming in word fragment
completion. Memory and Cognition, 15, 379-388.
Rohsenow, D. J., Monti, P. M., Abrams, D. B., Rubonis, A. V., Niaura,
R. S., Sirota, A. D., & Colby, S. M. (1992). Cue-elicited urge to drink
and salivation in alcoholics: Relationships to individual differences.
Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14, 195-210.
Rothbaum, B. O., & Foa, E. B. (1993). Subtypes ofposttraumatic stress
disorder and duration of symptoms. In J. R. T. Davidson & E. B. Foa
(Eds.), Posttraumatic stress disorder. DSM-1V and beyond (pp. 2335 ). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Roy, R. E. (1983). Alcohol misuse and posttraumatic stress disorder
TRAUMA AND ALCOHOL ABUSE
(delayed): An alternative interpretation of the data. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 44, 198-202.
Roy, R. E. (1984). Alcohol misuse and posttraumatic stress disorder: A
response to LaCoursiere and Coyne. Journal of Studies on Alcohol
45, 285-287.
Russell, J., & Mehrabian, A. ( 1975 ). The mediating role of emotions in
alcohol use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 36, 1508-1536.
Samoluk, S. B., & Stewart, S. H. (1996a). Anxiety sensitivity and anticipation of self-disclosing speech as determinants of alcohol consumption. Psychologyof Addictive Behavior, 10, 45-54.
Samoluk, S. B., & Stewart, S. H. (1996b). Attentional bias for alcohol
cues as a function of food deprivation and anxiety sensitivity
[ Summary ]. Convention Proceedingsfor the 29th Annual Meeting of
the Associationfor Advancement of Behavior Therapy, p. 246.
Schacter, D. L. ( 1987 ). Implicit memory: History and current status.
Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501-518.
Schaefer, M. R., Sobieraj, K., & Hollyfield, R. L. (1988). Prevalence of
childhood physical abuse in adult male veteran alcoholics. Child
Abuse and Neglect, 12, 141-149.
Schaefer, S., Evans, S., & Sterne, M. (1985). Incest among women in
recovery from alcoholism and drug dependency: Correlation and implication for treatment [Summary]. Proceedings of the International
Congress on Alcoholism and Drug Dependence, 34, 368-369.
Schnitt, J. M., & Nocks, J. J. (1984). Alcoholism treatment of Vietnam
veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Substance
Abuse Treatment, 1, 179-189.
Segal, Z. V. (1996). Cognitive interference in depressive and anxietybased disorders. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason,
(Eds.), Cognitive interference. Theories, methods, and findings (pp.
325-346 ). Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum.
Seizer, M. ( 1971 ). The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test: The quest
for a new diagnostic instrument. American Journal of Psychiatry,
127, 1653-1658.
Sheldon, H. (1988). Childhood sexual abuse in adult female psychotherapy referrals. British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 107-111.
Sher, K. J. (1987). Stress response dampening. In H. T. Blanc & K. E.
Leonard (Eds.), Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism
(pp. 227-271 ). New York: Guilford Press.
Sher, K. J., & Levenson, R. W. ( 1982 ). Risk for alcoholism and individual differences in the stress-response-dampening effect of alcohol: A
dose-response study. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 95, 350-367.
Sher, K. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1983). Alcohol and tension reduction:
The importance of individual differences. In L. A. Pohorecky & J.
Brick (Eds.), Stress and a~cohol use (pp. 121-134). Amsterdam:
Elsevier.
Shipko, S., Alvarez, W. A., & Noviello, N. (1983). Towards a teleologic
model of alexithymia: Alexithymia and posttraumatic stress disorder.
Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 39, 122-126.
Sierles, E S., Chen, J. J., McFarland, R. E., & Taylor, M. A. (1983).
Posttraumatic stress disorder and concurrent psychiatric illness: A
preliminary report. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 11771179.
Sifneos, E E. (1972). Short-term psychotherapy and emotional crisis.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Sifneos, E E. ( 1973 ). The prevalence of "alexithymic" characteristics
in psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 22,
255-262.
Singer, M. l., Petchers, M. K., & Hussey, D. (1989). The relationship
between sexual abuse and substance abuse among psychiatrically hospitalized adolescents. Child Abuse and Neglect, 13, 319-325.
Skorina, J. K., & Kovach, J. A. (1986). Treatment techniques for incest-related issues in alcoholic women. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 3, 17-30.
111
Solomon, D. A., & Malloy, P. E (1992). Alcohol, head injury, and neuropsychological function. Neuropsychology Review, 3, 249-280.
Southwick, S. M., Krystal, J. H., Morgan, C. A., Johnson, D., Nagy,
L. M., Nicolaou, A., Heninger, G. R., & Charney, D. S. (1993). Abnormal noradrenergic function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 266-274.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Gibbon, M. (1987). Structured
Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID). New York: New York
State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research Department.
Steele, C. M., & Josephs, R. A. (1988). Drinking your troubles away:
II. An attention-allocation model of alcohol's effect on psychological
stress. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 97, 196-205.
Stewart, S. H. (1995a). Anxiety sensitivity and risk for alcohol abuse in
young adult females [Summary]. Dissertation Abstracts International, 55, 4615B.
Stewart, S. H. (1995b). Anxiety sensitivity and situation-specific drinking [ Summary ]. ConventionProceedingsfor the 29th Annual Meeting
of the Associationfor Advancement of Behavior Therapy, p. 247.
Stewart, S. H., Achille, M., Dubois-Nguyen, I., & Pihi, R. O. (1992).
Alcohol effects on selective attention for threat in anxiety sensitive
females [ Summary ]. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 38,
309.
Stewart, S. H., Achille, M., & Pihl, R. O. (1993). Alcohol dampening of
the attentional bias for threat in anxiety sensitivity: Pharmacology or
expectancy? [Summary]. Programme and Abstracts of the Annual
Meeting of the Canadian Society for Brain, Behavior, and Cognitive
Science, 3, 55.
Stewart, S. H., Finn, P. R., & Pihl, R. O. (1992). The effects of alcohol
on the cardiovascular stress response in men at high risk for alcoholism: A dose response study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 53, 499506.
Stewart, S. H., Finn, P. R., & Pihl, R. O. (1995). A dose-response study
of the effects of alcohol on the perceptions of pain and discomfort due
to electric shock in men at high familial-genetic risk for alcoholism.
Psychopharmacology, 119, 261-267.
Stewart, S. H., & Gignac, M. (1996). Gender differences in the content
of selective processing biases in anxiety sensitivity, Manuscript submitted for publication.
Stewart, S. H., Karp, J., Pihl, R. O., & Peterson, R. A. (1995). Anxiety
sensitivity and self-reported reasonsfor drug use. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Stewart, S. H., Knize, K., & Pihl, R. O. (1992). Anxiety sensitivity and
dependency in clinical and non-clinical panickers and controls.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 6, 119-131.
Stewart, S. H., Peterson, J, B., & Pihl, R. O. (1995). Anxiety sensitivity
and self-reported rates of alcohol consumption in university women.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 9, 283-292.
Stewart, S. H., & Pihl, R. O. (1994). Effects of alcohol on subjectiveemotional and physiological reactivity to threat in anxiety sensitive
women and controls. Psychologyof Addictive Behavior, 8, 14-27.
Stewart, S. H,, & Pihl, R. O. ( 1995 ). Effects of alcohol on attention and
memory for threat in anxiety sensitive women [Summary]. In S. H.
Stewart (Chair), Applications of Cognitive Psychology to the Study
of Addictive Behaviour (Symposium). Abstracts and Proceedings of
the WorldCongress of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies, 1, 35.
Stewart, S. H., Pihl, R. O., & Padjen, A. L. (1992). Chronic use of
alcohol and/or benzodiazepines may account for evidence of altered
benzodiazepine receptor sensitivity in panic disorder. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 49, 329-330.
Stewart, S. H., Rioux, G., Connolly, J. E, Dunphy, S. C., & Teehan,
M. D. (1996). Time course of the effects of oxazepam and lorazepam
on implicit and explicit memory, Manuscript submitted for
publication.
1 12
STEWART
Stewart, S. H., & Zeitlin, S. B. (1995). Anxiety sensitivity and alcohol
use motives. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 9, 229-240.
Stewart, S. H., Zeitlin, S. B., & Samoluk, S. B. (1996). Examination of
a three-dimensional drinking motives questionnaire in a young adult
population. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 61-71.
Straussner, S. L. ( 1985 ). Alcoholism in women: Current knowledge and
implications for treatment. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 2, 6177.
Streimer, J. H., Cosstick, J., & Tennant, C. (1985). The psychosocial
adjustment of Australian Vietnam veterans. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 142, 616-618.
Strickler, D. P., Tomaszewski, R., Maxwell, W. A., & Suib, M. R.
(1979). The effects of relaxation instructions on drinking behavior
in the presence of stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 17, 4551.
Sutker, P. B., Uddo, M., Brailey, K., Allain, A. N., & Errera, P. (1994).
Psychological symptoms and psychiatric diagnoses in Operation Desert Storm troops serving graves registration duty. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 1, 159-171.
Sutker, P. B., Uddo, M., Brailey, K., Vasterling, J. J., & Errera, P.
(1994). Psychopathology in war-zone deployed and nondeployed Operation Desert Storm troops assigned graves registration duties.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 383-390.
Sweet, S., Stewart, S. H., Samoluk, S. B., & MacDonald, A. (1995).
Anxiety sensitivity and social affiliative drinking [Summary ]. Canadian Psychology, 36 (2a), 65.
Swett, C., Jr., Cohen, C., Surrey, J., Compaine, A., & Chavez, R.
( 1991 ). High rates of alcohol use and history of physical and sexual
abuse among women outpatients. American Journal of Drug and AIcohol Abuse. 17, 49-60.
Taylor, G. J. (1994). The alexithymia construct: Conceptualization,
validation, and relationship with basic dimensions of personality.
New Trends in Experimental and Clinical Psychiatry, 10, 61-74.
Taylor, S., Koch, W. J., & McNally, R. J. (1992). How does anxiety
sensitivity vary across the anxiety disorders? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 6, 249-259.
Terr, L. C. ( 1981 ). Psychic trauma in children: Observations following
the Chowchilla school-bus kidnapping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 14-19.
Terr, L. C. ( 1983 ). Chowchilla revisited: The effects of psychic trauma
four years after a school-bus kidnapping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 1543-1550.
Terr, L. C. ( 1991 ). Childhood traumas: An outline and overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 10-20.
Thrasher, S, M., Dalgleish, T., & Yule, W. ( 1994 ). Information processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapk; 32, 247-254.
Thyer, B. A., & Curtis, G. C. (1984). The effects of ethanol intoxication
on phobic anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22, 599-610.
Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use
behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. PsychologicalReview,, 97, 147-168.
Turner, S., & Colao, F. ( 1985 ). Alcoholism and sexual assault: A treatment approach for women exploring both issues. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 2, 91-103.
Vogel-Sprott, M. (1983). Response measures of social drinking: Research implications and applications. Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
44, 817-836.
Volpicelli, J. R. (1987). Uncontrollable events and alcohol drinking.
British Journal of Addiction, 82, 381-392.
Wardle, J. (1990). Behaviour therapy and benzodiazepines: Allies or
antagonists? British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 163-168.
Warren, G., & Raynes, A. (1972). Mood changes during three conditions of alcohol intake. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 33,
979-988.
Warshaw, M. G., Fierman, E., Pratt, L., Hunt, M., Yonkers, K. A., Massion, A. O., & Keller, M. B. (1993). Quality of life in anxiety disorder
patients with histories of trauma or posttraumatic stress disorder.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1512-1516.
Watts, W. D., & Ellis, A. M. (1993). Sexual abuse and drinking and
drug use: Implications for prevention. Journal of Drug Education,
23, 183-200.
Williams, H. L., & Rundell, O. H. (1981). Altered sleep physiology
in chronic alcoholics: Reversal with abstinence. Alcoholism, 5, 318325.
Williams, J. M. G. (1995). Depression and the specificity of autobiographical memory. In D. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering ourpast: Studies in autobiographical memory (pp. 271-296). Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Williams, J. M. G., Watts, E N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988).
Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Chichester, England:
Wiley.
Williams, L. M. (1994a). Recall of childhood trauma: A prospective
study of women's memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consuiting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1167-1176.
Williams, L. M. (1994b). What does it mean to forget child sexual
abuse? A reply to Loftus, Garry, and Feldman (1994). Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1182-1186.
Wilsnack, S. C. (1982). Alcohol abuse and alcoholism in women. In
E. M. Pattison & E. Kaufman (Eds.), Encyclopedic handbook of alcoholism (pp. 718-735 ). New York: Gardner Press.
Wilsnack, S. C. (1984). Drinking, sexuality and sexual dysfunction in
women. In S. C. Wilsnack & L. J. Beckman (Eds.), Alcoholproblems
in women: Antecedents, consequences and intervention (pp. 189227 ). New York: Guilford Press.
Wilsnack, S. C. (1991). Sexuality and women's drinking: Findings
from a U.S. national study. Alcohol Health and Research World, 15,
147-150.
Wilson, G. T. (1988). Alcohol and anxiety. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 26, 369-381.
Wilson, G. T., & Abrams, D., (1977). Effects of alcohol on social anxiety and physiological arousal: Cognitive versus physiological processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1, 195-210.
Wilson, G. T., Abrams, D., & Lipscomb, T. (1980). Effects of increasing
levels of intoxication and drinking pattern on social anxiety. Journal
of Studies on Alcohol, 41, 250-264.
Winfield, I., George, L. K., Swartz, M., & Blazer, D. G. (1990). Sexual
assault and psychiatric disorders among a community sample of
women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 335-341.
Wiseman, E. J. (1994). Alcohol dependence may be the missing link
between posttraumatic stress disorder and panic. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 51, 429-430.
Yager, T., Laufer, R., & Gallops, M. (1984), Some problems associated
with war experiences in men of the Vietnam generation. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 41,327-333.
Yeary, J. ( 1982 ). Incest and chemical dependency. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 14, 133-135.
Yehuda, R., Southwick, S. M., & Giller, E. L. (1992). Exposure to
atrocities and severity of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder in
Vietnam combat veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 333336.
Received March 15, 1994
Revision received September 1, 1995
Accepted September 16, 1995 •