Download Niches at the edges: price-value tradeoff, consumer behavior, and

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Viral marketing wikipedia , lookup

Perfect competition wikipedia , lookup

Dumping (pricing policy) wikipedia , lookup

Target audience wikipedia , lookup

Street marketing wikipedia , lookup

Brand loyalty wikipedia , lookup

Digital marketing wikipedia , lookup

Visual merchandising wikipedia , lookup

Marketing research wikipedia , lookup

Food marketing wikipedia , lookup

Service parts pricing wikipedia , lookup

Targeted advertising wikipedia , lookup

Pricing wikipedia , lookup

Marketing strategy wikipedia , lookup

Direct marketing wikipedia , lookup

Multicultural marketing wikipedia , lookup

Marketing wikipedia , lookup

Marketing mix modeling wikipedia , lookup

Customer satisfaction wikipedia , lookup

Integrated marketing communications wikipedia , lookup

Price discrimination wikipedia , lookup

Value proposition wikipedia , lookup

Target market wikipedia , lookup

Pricing strategies wikipedia , lookup

Global marketing wikipedia , lookup

Youth marketing wikipedia , lookup

Product planning wikipedia , lookup

Advertising campaign wikipedia , lookup

Marketing channel wikipedia , lookup

Green marketing wikipedia , lookup

Consumer behaviour wikipedia , lookup

Neuromarketing wikipedia , lookup

Sensory branding wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Niches at the edges: price-value tradeoff, consumer behavior,
and marketing strategy
Author: Syed H. Akhter, Marquette University, Milwaukee, Wisconsin, USA
Abstract:
Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to examine how the perception of price-value tradeoff is
related to overall satisfaction, purchase intention, word-of-mouth advertising, and actual
repurchase behavior.
Design/methodology/approach – Data on subscribers and single ticket buyers of a major
symphony orchestra in the Midwest are used to test the hypotheses.
Findings – The ANOVA results show significant differences across the three levels of pricevalue tradeoff in each of the response variables. Additional analyses of cross-tabulated data
show that some of the bivariate relations conform to, as well as depart from, the rational
consumer behavior model.
Research limitations/implications – Although the hypotheses are supported, bivariate
relations examined in this study can mask or overstate true relations due to the omitted
variables bias. Future research can explore reasons for favorable behaviors of consumers
whose perception is that the value they receive is overpriced, and also for unfavorable
behaviors of consumers whose perception is that the value they received is under-priced.
Practical implications – The different niches at the edges provide opportunities for marketers
to fine-tune segmentation and marketing mix strategies. The use of standardized strategies for
these niches with different perception and behavior linkages will yield suboptimal results.
Originality/value – While previous research has mostly focused on price-quality linkages, this
study extends the body of research by examining the perception of price-value tradeoff and its
relation to overall satisfaction, purchase intention, word-of-mouth advertising, and actual
repurchase behavior. This adds to our understanding of post consumption behavior, showing
how consumers respond to the perception of price-value tradeoff.
Although the theoretical significance of scholarly research in marketing is well
established in the academia, marketing executives sometimes feel that the elaborate conceptual
linkages presented in academic studies fail to provide meaningful guidance in strategic decision
making. Managers also express difficulty in relating findings from these studies to routine
consumer behavior and thus shy away from using them. McCole (2004) points to this situation
1 Akhter
and suggests that there exists a sizable gap between the worldviews of academics and
practitioners. And Brennen (2004, p. 492), in his editorial comments in a special issue that
explored the nature of the academic-practitioner divide, notes that “a consensus seems to be
emerging that much, or perhaps most, of work done by academics is of limited relevance to
practitioners.”
These concerns have spilled into the academia where marketing and management
scholars continue to debate the tradeoffs between rigor and relevance of academic research.
The debate continues, and the issue of how to select research questions as well as how to
answer them is far from resolved. But there has emerged a new orientation in the discipline that
focuses more on discovering parsimonious relations between constructs than on developing
grand models of consumer behavior. Major scholarly journals have also taken the lead in
requiring authors to delineate the strategic implications of the findings of their research.
Changes are also evident in the corporate world, where access to large data bases has led to
data mining and data-based marketing strategies and has motivated the search for customer
segments whose needs and wants can be satisfied with differentiated value propositions.
In light of the above considerations, this paper takes a more applied approach to
examining relations between the perception of price-value tradeoff and four aspects of
consumer behavior, overall satisfaction, purchase intention, word-of-mouth advertising, and
actual repurchase behavior. With respect to these bivariate relations, the paper also comments
on the rational consumer behavior model and discusses the strategic implications of the findings.
The underlying premise of this paper is that the delineations of bivariate relations can reveal
meaningful insights that can prove strategically valuable for segmenting markets, especially at
the edges, and developing value enhancing offerings. The proposed relations are tested on data
collected by a symphony orchestra in a large Midwestern city.
Introduction
As one of the marketing mix variables, price plays an important role in influencing
consumers’ perception of products, increasing demand, attracting customers, and promoting
brand loyalty, among other things. Research in marketing shows that the perception of
reference price (price considered reasonable or fair by consumers) can be altered by changing
how price is presented to consumers (Morris and Morris, 1990; Simonson and Tversky, 1992). If,
for example, a higher priced product is added to the top of the product line, buyers’ reference
price increases. Research also shows that consumers respond differently to price discounts. For
2 Akhter
example, when consumers are presented with higher discounts on both name brands and
unknown brands, they are more likely to buy name brands than unknown brands (Moore and
Olshavsky, 1989; Gupta and Cooper, 1992), because in the case of name brands they link
reduced price with getting a better deal for their money, and in the case of unknown brands they
link reduced price with doubtful quality.
Marketers have also found that demand and price are not always inversely related and
that demand can be increased not only by lowering price but also by increasing it. For example,
Stella Artois, a regular beer in Belgium, was successfully priced at a premium level in the US
market to create the impression of luxury and exclusiveness (Beamish and Goerzen, 2000).
Positive relations between price and demand have often been cited as evidence of consumers
not always behaving according to the rational behavior model. Monroe (1973) and Skouras et al.
(2005) point to the diversity of consumer behavior and suggest that consumers in some
situations behave in accordance with the Marshallian assumption of rationality, but in others
they do not.
Although price plays a role in influencing purchase behavior, consumers do not always
remember the price they paid for a product. The difficulty in recalling the price paid, however, is
compensated by the ability to form evaluative judgments. What this suggests is that while
consumers cannot recall the exact price they paid for the product, they can easily indicate
whether the product or service they bought was overpriced, under-priced, or just-priced for the
value received. The perception of the price-value tradeoff and its relation to consumer behavior
has not been adequately explored in the literature. This study attempts to fill this gap by looking
at bivariate relations between the price-value tradeoff and four different aspects of consumer
behavior, such as overall satisfaction, purchase intention, word-of-mouth advertising, and actual
repurchase behavior. Furthermore, this study adds to the marketing literature by examining the
above relations in the context of a service sector (Voss et al., 1998) and by commenting on the
generalizability of the rational consumer behavior model (Skouras et al., 2005).
Price-value tradeoff and consumer behavior
Price and value are closely related constructs. The former measures the monetary
sacrifice consumers make to obtain a service, and the latter reflects a judgment of what they get
from the service received. While the price paid for a service can be determined relatively easily,
value has a personal and idiosyncratic component to it (Zeithaml, 1988). When consumers use
the term value, they might mean different things. For example, they can associate value with
3 Akhter
low price (Bishop, 1984), with the quality of the product (Dodds and Monroe, 1985), and with
what they get for what they give (Sawyer and Dickson, 1984). Zeithaml (1988) integrates these
different views of value in the concept of perceived value, which is an overall evaluation of “what
is received for what is given” (p. 14). The perceived value is represented as the tradeoff
between the price paid and value received, reflecting the give and get component of a
transaction.
The examination of the price-value tradeoff perception and its relation to consumer
behavior builds on the research on price-quality link. Consumers have been shown to use price
as a cue to judge the quality of a product or service. Quality, in this case, is intrinsic to the
product, and consumers use price, among other factors, to form evaluative judgments about
these intrinsic characteristics. The influence of price in this evaluation, however, becomes
pronounced when other product-related cues are absent (Kardes et al., 2004), a phenomenon
known as price-quality cue utilization. In a recent study, Estelami (2008) found the existence of
price-quality cue for financial services and suggested the importance of understanding the link
for the optimal positioning of the brand. Price-value tradeoff, on the other hand, looks at postconsumption evaluation of a service encounter or product use. The judgment is formed after the
use of the service or product and reflects the value of the service to consumers in terms of the
price they paid. In this evaluation, the quality of the product or service plays a significant role
(Cronin et al., 2000).
As consumers do not easily recall the exact price they paid for a product, memory
distortion is smaller when consumers are asked to evaluate price (expensive or inexpensive)
rather than to remember the exact price (Xia, 2005). Consumers can thus easily indicate
whether the service received was under-priced, just-priced, or overpriced for the value received.
This study examines in the next section how the perception of the price-value tradeoff (underpriced, just-priced, and overpriced for the value received) relates to overall satisfaction,
purchase intention, word-of-mouth advertising, and actual repurchase behavior.
Price-value tradeoff and overall satisfaction
Overall satisfaction is a post-consumption emotive state that is influenced by the
discrepancy between the actual and expected value of the product or service received
(Anderson, 1994). Research shows that when consumers judge the value of a product, they
bring into consideration the price they paid for the product (Zeithaml, 1988). In judging the level
of satisfaction, the rational consumer behavior model suggests that consumers who think that
4 Akhter
the service they received was under-priced for the value received will exhibit higher levels of
satisfaction than consumers who think that the service was just-priced; and the satisfaction of
the just-priced group will be higher than the satisfaction of the overpriced for the value received
group. We, therefore, propose the following:
H1. Overall satisfaction will vary across the under-priced, just-priced, and
overpriced for the value received groups.
Price-value tradeoff and purchase intention
Purchase intention reveals a predisposition to make a purchase in the future. There is a
positive relation between the perception of value and willingness to buy (Dodds et al., 1991).
Consumers who think that the service was underpriced for the value received will show a higher
level of purchase intention. Similarly, intention to purchase will be higher among people who
think that the price was just for the value received, compared to people who thought that the
price was high for the value received. We, therefore, propose the following:
H2. Purchase intention will vary across the under-priced, just-priced, and
overpriced for the value received groups.
Price-value tradeoff and word-of-mouth advertising
Word-of-mouth advertising is an endorsement of a product or service. Consumers who
feel that they got value for their money are more likely to recommend the service to their friends
and family, just as consumers are more likely to complain if they feel that the value they
received was overpriced. The perception of price unfairness may also induce negative emotions
in consumers (Xia et al., 2004). The disposition to recommend something and engage in wordof-mouth advertising will thus be stronger in consumers who think that they got value for the
price paid. In contrast, consumers who think that the value they received was overpriced would
be less likely to engage in word-of-mouth advertising. We, therefore, propose the following:
H3. Word-of-mouth advertising will vary across the under-priced, just-priced, and
overpriced for the value received groups.
Price-value tradeoff and actual repurchase
Businesses succeed by motivating actual repurchase behavior. The rational behavior
model suggests that consumers who perceive that the price they paid was under-priced or justpriced for the value received will be more likely to repurchase. Likewise, consumers will be
reluctant to repurchase if they think that their service experience was overpriced for the value
5 Akhter
received. Consumers’ perception of price-value tradeoff will thus influence actual repurchase
behavior. We, therefore, propose the following:
H4. Actual repurchase will vary across the under-priced, just-priced, and
overpriced for the value received groups.
Method
The above hypotheses were tested on data collected through a survey questionnaire. A
committee of business professionals, academics, and orchestra managers collaborated on
developing the questionnaire. Discussions about wording and content led to several iterations of
the questionnaire before the final version was accepted and mailed in two waves to 5,349
subscribers and single ticket buyers of a major symphony orchestra in the Midwest. The total
number of responses received was 3,087.
Demographic data indicated diverse backgrounds of respondents. With respect to age,
the percentage distribution of respondents was as follows: under 25 (0.6 percent), 25-34 (3.9
percent), 35-49 (19 percent), 50-54 (12 percent), 55-64 (27 percent), 65-74 (25 percent), 75-84
(11 percent), and over 85 (2 percent). With respect to education, the percentage distribution
was as follows: high school (3 percent), some college (13 percent), associate degree (3
percent), bachelors (35 percent), masters (33 percent), and MD/PhD (13 percent). With respect
to income in thousands, the percentage distribution was as follows: under $30 (9 percent), $3049 (19 percent), $50-69 (19 percent), $70-89 (16 percent), $90-124 (16 percent), $125-149 (6
percent), $150-199 (7 percent), $200-249 (2 percent), and over $250 (6 percent).
Two types of analyses were conducted, ANOVA to test the hypotheses, and crosstabulations to examine consumer behavior at the edges of the market.
Variables
Data on price-value tradeoff was obtained by asking respondents to indicate whether the
price they paid for the symphony was overpriced, just-priced, or under-priced for the value
received. The coding of these three categories was as follows: overpriced (-1), just-priced (0),
and under-priced (1). Overall satisfaction data was obtained by asking respondents to indicate
their level of overall satisfaction with the symphony on a nine-point Likert scale, which ranged
from very dissatisfied (1) to very satisfied (9). The nine-point scale was reduced to a five-point
scale to minimize the empty cells problem in the cross-tabulations analysis. Purchase intention
data was obtained by asking respondents how likely they were to purchase tickets on a five6 Akhter
point Likert scale, ranging from definitely will not, probably not, might or might not, probably will,
and definitely will. Data on word-of-mouth advertising was obtained by asking respondents to
indicate how likely they were to recommend the symphony to people they know on a five-point
Likert scale, ranging from definitely would not, probably would not, might or might not, probably
would, and definitely would. Actual repurchase was recorded by registering whether or not the
respondents actually repurchased symphony tickets.
Results
We first present the ANOVA results. Following this, we discuss the findings from crosstabulations. For both analyses, a pairwise deletion procedure was followed. The ANOVA results
show significant differences in means in each of the four aspects of consumer behavior – overall
satisfaction, purchase intention, word-of-mouth advertising, and actual repurchase behavior
(see Table I). For overall satisfaction (F = 83:790, df = 2; 2972, p-value = 0:000), for purchase
intention (F = 42.413, df = 2; 2977, p-value = 0:000), for word-of-mouth advertising (F = 30:382,
df = 2; 2967, p-value = 0:000), and for actual repurchase (F = 11:148, df = 2; 2999, p-value =
0:000).
While the results above support the hypotheses presented in the paper, more detailed
information about consumer behavior can be gleaned from cross-tabulated data. In discussing
findings from the cross-tabulations, we first focus on relations at the edges (the under-priced for
the value received group and the over-priced for the value received group) and then on relations
both within and between the three groups to comment on the rational consumer behavior model.
Although the bivariate links presented in this study provide useful insights into one-onone relations between price-value tradeoff and four different aspects of consumer behavior, two
limitations should be noted. First, bivariate relations can mask or overstate true relations due to
omitted variables bias. Second, bivariate relations do not show the linkages that connect the
variables in a nomological network. With these limitations in mind, we discuss the results.
Price-value tradeoff and satisfaction
In the under-priced for the value received group, 87.6 percent were very satisfied versus
53.3 percent in the over-priced for the value received group. However, the unexpected finding
was that a lower percentage of people in the under-priced group, 12.4 percent, were in the
mostly satisfied category versus 31.1 percent in the over-priced group. Overall ߯2 with 8 d.f.
was 188.5; p = 0:000. See Table II for other results.
7 Akhter
Price-value tradeoff and purchase intention
Price-value tradeoff and purchase intention showed the following results. In the underpriced group 79.1 percent, versus 37.8 percent in the over-priced group, indicated that they
definitely would repurchase the ticket. Also, as expected, 9.5 percent in the over-priced group,
versus 0.8 percent in the under-priced group, indicated that they would definitely not repurchase
the ticket. However, the unexpected result was that only 16.3 percent of the respondents in the
under-priced group, versus 32.4 percent in the over-priced group, said that they probably would
purchase the ticket. Overall ߯2 with 8 d.f. was 107; p = 0:000. See Table III for other results.
Price-value tradeoff and word-of-mouth advertising
Price-value tradeoff and the desire to engage in word-of-mouth advertising yielded the
following results. In the underpriced group, 92.9 percent indicated that they would definitely
recommend the symphony to their friends, versus 55.9 percent in the overpriced group.
However, the unexpected result was that 5.5 percent in the under-priced group, versus 35.2
percent in the over-priced group, said that they probably would engage in word-of-mouth
advertising. Overall ߯2 with 8 d.f. was 81.006; p = 0:000. See Table IV for other results.
Price-value tradeoff and actual repurchase
Findings show that in the under-priced group 85.3 percent actually repurchased the
ticket, versus 64.7 percent in the over-priced group. Furthermore, as expected, in the underpriced group 14.7 percent did not repurchase the ticket versus 35.3 percent in the over-priced
group. The unexpected result, however, was that within the over-priced group, more people
actually repurchased the ticket than not, 64.7 percent versus 35.3 percent. Overall ߯2 with 2 d.f.
was 22.153; p = 0:000. See Table V for other results.
Managerial implications
Findings support the proposed hypotheses that the perception of price-value tradeoff is
related to overall consumer satisfaction, purchase intention, word-of-mouth advertising, and
actual repurchase behavior. The concept of price-value tradeoff is strategically relevant because
consumers have choices, and understanding how they respond to such tradeoffs will be
valuable in crafting marketing strategies for niches at the edges.
The under-priced for value received group had the highest percentage of customers who
8 Akhter
were very satisfied and definitely intended to repurchase and engage in word-of-mouth
advertising. This group also had the highest percentage of actual repurchase. These consumers,
as expected, behaved according to the rational consumer model. However, for the majority of
consumers in this group, the marketing implications relate to the issue of adjusting the tradeoff.
The managerial question is, should the perception of price-value tradeoff of this niche be
brought more in line with the just-priced group, and, if yes, how? Looking at the option of
increasing price for this group, several comments can be made. Existing research shows that
consumers’ responses to price increase are shaped by what they learn about firms and what
firms say about their motives for raising prices (Campbell, 1999). When the decision to raise the
price is made, marketing communications can be used to manage consumers’ inference of
motive and, thus, their perception of fairness about the price increase (Campbell, 1999). The
other issue for this niche of 4.3 percent of total respondents relates to people who did not
repurchase the tickets, even though their perception of the price-value tradeoff was positive.
From a marketing perspective, what is important is that these non-buyers already hold a
favorable view of the service encounter, which can be leveraged to bring them back.
The just priced for value received niche constitutes the core group of consumers. This
group was the largest of the three with over 90 percent of the total respondents, who supported
the rational behavior model. They were satisfied with the service, expressed favorable intention
to repurchase, showed willingness to engage in word-of-mouth advertising, and actually
repurchased tickets. However, even within this group, 20.5 percent of consumers did not
repurchase tickets. This is a large percentage of consumers for any firm. With respect to
bringing them back, one of the advantages the firm has is that this niche already has a favorable
price-value tradeoff perception, which can be exploited with reminder oriented marketing
communications. Furthermore, managers also need to develop an effective customer
acquisition strategy to compensate for consumers who did not repurchase.
The over-priced for value received group exhibited unexpected behavior and, to some
degree, supported the notion that consumers do not always behave according to the rational
behavior model. Although consumers in this group thought that the value they received was
overpriced, they still showed favorable responses in terms of overall satisfaction, purchase
intention, word-of-mouth advertising, and repurchase behavior. With their repurchase behavior,
they exhibited what can be called the Starbucks factor. Most consumers view Starbucks coffees
as overpriced; but they continue to buy the product, not because there are no other options, but
because they like the store environment, the product, and the service. Starbucks has created an
9 Akhter
image that resonates with people. However, considering the perception of the overpriced for the
value received niche, it can be assumed that these customers will be most susceptible to
switching to competing services. The recommended strategy to retain these customers would
be to increase the value of the service encounter, focusing on non-price elements.
Conclusions
Findings from this study indicate complex relations between price-value tradeoff and
overall satisfaction, purchase intention, word-of-mouth advertising, and actual repurchase.
Although the overall findings support the rational behavior model, findings for the niches at the
edges challenge the assumption commonly held about price-value tradeoff and consumer
behavior. These niches constitute specific segments and challenge the practice of crafting a
standardized strategy for different consumer groups within the same product or service market.
As results from this study show, the one-size-fits-all strategy will result in sub-optimizing returns
on marketing investments.
Marketers have traditionally focused on a core group of consumers, who are satisfied
with the exchange equation, and, for most firms, this core group constitutes the majority of
customers. Focusing on this group pays off because of its size and favorable response to
marketing offerings. However, as shown in this study, there are niches at the edges that not
only see the exchange equation differently but also behave differently. These niches form
specific segments for which firms will need to adapt marketing strategies. The understanding of
what is happening in these niches will provide firms with opportunities to improve their
competitive position.
As competitive pressures on firms increase due to the variety of choices available to
consumers, the significance of understanding the different niches at the edges within a market
has increased. These groups behave differently from the core group, partly because they depart
from the general economic assumption of rationality. Although the percentage of these
customers may vary for different businesses, the real challenge for managers is determining
how to treat these consumers who buy and use the same service but have different price-value
tradeoff perceptions and behavioral dispositions.
Many substantive issues arise from this research; exploring them would add to the body
of research on pricing strategy and consumer behavior. Future research can study why people
repurchase products and services even when they perceive that the value they received was
overpriced. If consumers think that they overpaid and yet repurchase the product, there must be
10 Akhter
cognitive, emotional, and contextual factors that play a role in influencing this behavior. These
factors can be examined in the context of the price-value tradeoff. Another promising area of
research is examining how much of price-value tradeoff can be changed for the under-priced for
the value received consumers before they decide not to buy the service. Specifically, to what
level can the price be raised before the perception of price-value tradeoff affects behavior?
What is the price threshold for consumers before it affects their tradeoff evaluation and
behavior? Another promising area of research is determining the effects of price-value tradeoff
on consumer behavior for different types of products and services, such as necessities versus
luxury versus discretionary goods.
References
Anderson, E.W. (1994), “Cross-category variation in customer satisfaction and retention”,
Marketing Letters, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 19-30.
Beamish, P.W. and Goerzen, A. (2000), The Global Branding of Stella Artois, Ivey Management
Services, London.
Bishop, W.R. Jr (1984),“Competitive intelligence”, Progressive Grocer, Vol. 63, pp. 19-20.
Brennan, R. (2004), “Should we worry about an ‘academic-practitioner divide’ in marketing?”,
Marketing Intelligence & Planning, Vol. 22 No. 5, pp. 492-500.
Campbell, M.C. (1999), “Perceptions of price unfairness: antecedents and consequences”,
Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 36, May, pp. 187-99.
Cronin, J.J., Brady, M.K. and Hult, G.T.M. (2000), “Assessing the effects of quality, value, and
customer satisfaction on consumer behavioral intentions in service environments”,
Journal of Retailing,Vol. 76 No. 2, pp. 193-218.
Dodds, W.B. and Monroe, K.B. (1985), “The effect of brand and price information on subjective
product evaluations”, in Hirschman, E. and Holbrook, M. (Eds), Advances in Consumer
Research, Vol. 12, Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 85-90.
Dodds, W.B., Monroe, K.B. and Grewal, D. (1991), “Effects of price, brand, and store
information on buyers’ product evaluations”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 28 No.
3, pp. 307-19.
Estelami, H. (2008), “Consumer use of the price-quality cue in financial services”, Journal of
Product & Brand Management, Vol. 17 No. 3, pp. 197-208.
Gupta, S. and Cooper, L.G. (1992), “The discounting of discounts and promotion thresholds”,
Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 19 No. 3, pp. 401-12.
11 Akhter
Kardes, F.R., Cronley, M.L., Kellaris, J. and Posavac, S. (2004), “The role of selective
information processing in price-quality inferences”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol.
31 No. 2, pp. 368-74.
McCole, P. (2004), “Refocusing marketing to reflect practice: the changing role of marketing for
business”, Marketing Intelligence & Planning, Vol. 22 No. 5, pp. 531-9.
Monroe, K.B. (1973), “Buyers’ subjective perception of price”, Journal of Marketing Research,
Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 70-80.
Moore, D.J. and Olshavsky, R.W. (1989), “Brand choice and deep price discounts”, Psychology
and Marketing, Vol. 6 No. 3, pp. 181-96.
Morris, M.H. and Morris, G.P. (1990), Market-Oriented Pricing, Quorum Books, Westport, CT.
Sawyer, A.G. and Dickson, P. (1984), “Psychological perspectives on consumer response to
sales promotion”, pp. 1-21, Research on Sales Promotion: Collected Papers, Marketing
Science Institute, Cambridge, MA.
Simonson, I. and Tversky, A. (1992), “Choice in context: trade-off contrast and extremeness
aversion”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 29, August, pp. 281-95.
Skouras, T., Avlonitis, G.F. and Indounas, K.A. (2005), “Economics and marketing on pricing:
how and why do they differ?”, Journal of Product & Brand Management, Vol. 15 No. 6,
pp. 362-74.
Voss, G.B., Parasuraman, A. and Grewal, D. (1998), “The role of price, performance and
expectations in determining satisfaction in service exchanges”, Journal of Marketing, Vol.
62, October, pp. 46-61.
Xia, L. (2005), “Memory distortion and consumer price knowledge”, Journal of Product & Brand
Management, Vol. 14 No. 5, pp. 338-47.
Xia, L., Monroe, K.B. and Cox, J.L. (2004), “The price is unfair! A conceptual framework of price
fairness perceptions”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 68, October, pp. 1-15.
Zeithaml, V.A. (1988), “Consumer perceptions of price, quality, and value: a means-end model
and synthesis of evidence”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 52, July, pp. 2-22.
Notes
•
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at
www.emeraldinsight.com/1061-0421.htm
•
Corresponding author: Syed H. Akhter can be contacted at: syed.akhter@ marquette.edu
12 Akhter
Appendix
Table 1
ANOVA
Overall satisfaction
Purchase intention
WOM advertising
Repurchase
13 Akhter
F-value
F(2, 2972) = 83.790
F(2, 2977) = 42.413
F(2, 2967) = 30.382
F(2, 2999) = 11.148
Significance
0.000
0.000
0.000
0.000
Table 2
Price-value tradeoff and satisfaction
Overall
Very
satisfaction dissatisfied
Mostly
dissatisfied
Neither
Mostly
satisfied
Very satisfied
Total
14 Akhter
Under-priced
for value
received
%
n
0
0.0
Price-value tradeoff
Just-priced
Over-priced
for value
for value
received
received
%
%
n
n
6
0.2
2
1.4
Total
8
%
0.3
n
0
0.0
11
0.4
12
8.1
23
0.8
0
16
0.0
12.4
30
378
1.1
14.0
9
46
6.1
31.1
39
440
1.3
14.8
113
129
87.6
100.0
2,273
2,698
84.2
100.0
79
148
53.4
100.0
2,465
82.9
2975 100.0
Table 3
Price-value tradeoff and purchase intention
Purchase
intention
15 Akhter
Definitely not
Probably not
Might/might not
Probably
Definitely
Total
Under-priced
for value
received
%
n
1
0.8
2
1.6
3
2.3
21
16.3
102
79.1
129 100.0
Price-value tradeoff
Just-priced
Over-priced
for value
for value
received
received
%
%
n
n
32
1.2
14
9.5
91
3.4
9
6.1
197
7.3
21
14.2
676
25.0
48
32.4
1,707
63.2
56
37.8
2,703 100.0
148 100.0
Total
%
n
47
1.6
102
3.4
221
7.4
745
25.0
1,865
62.6
2,980 100.0
Table IV
Price-value tradeoff and word-of-mouth advertising
WOM
Definitely
advertising not
Probably
not
Might/might
not
Probably
Definitely
Total
16 Akhter
Underpriced for
value
received
n
%
0
0.0
Price-value tradeoff
Just-priced Over-priced
for value
for value
received
received
n
1
%
0.0
n
0
%
0.0
Total
n
1
%
0.0
0
0.0
15
0.6
0
0.0
15
0.5
2
1.6
53
2.0
13
9.0
68
2.3
5.5
463
92.9 2,166
100.0 2,698
17.2
80.3
100.0
51
81
145
35.2
55.9
100.0
521
2,365
2,970
17.5
79.6
100.0
7
118
127
Table V
Price-value tradeoff and actual repurchase
Under-priced for
value received
n
Repurchase No
Yes
Total
17 Akhter
19
110
129
%
14.7
85.3
100.0
Price-value tradeoff
Just-priced for
OverTotal
value received
priced for
value
received
n
%
n
%
n
%
557
20.5
53 35.3
629 21.0
2,166
79.5
97 64.7 2,373 79.0
2,2723
100.0 150 100.0 3,002 100.0