Download A comparison of punishment exchange rates between offenders

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Trial as an adult wikipedia , lookup

Feminist school of criminology wikipedia , lookup

Criminalization wikipedia , lookup

Youth incarceration in the United States wikipedia , lookup

The New Jim Crow wikipedia , lookup

Private probation wikipedia , lookup

Life imprisonment in England and Wales wikipedia , lookup

Juvenile delinquency wikipedia , lookup

Prison–industrial complex wikipedia , lookup

California Proposition 36, 2012 wikipedia , lookup

Recidivism wikipedia , lookup

U.S. Probation and Pretrial Services System wikipedia , lookup

Probation in Pakistan wikipedia , lookup

Prison reform wikipedia , lookup

Criminal justice system of the Netherlands wikipedia , lookup

Alternatives to imprisonment wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39 – 50
A comparison of punishment exchange rates between offenders
under supervision and their supervising officers B
Christopher M. Flory a, David C. May a,*, Kevin I. Minor a, Peter B. Wood b
a
b
Department of Correctional and Juvenile Justice Studies, Eastern Kentucky University, Stratton 105,
521 Lancaster Avenue, Richmond, KY 40475, United States
Department of Sociology, Anthropology and Social Work, Mississippi State University, P. O. Box C, Mississippi State, MS 39762, United States
Abstract
Previous research revealed gender and racial differences in offender preferences for prison compared to alternative sanctions, in
the amount of an alternative sanction offenders would serve to avoid prison, and in the severity ranking of criminal sanctions.
Nevertheless, opinions of officers who supervised those clients have all but been ignored. In this study, this literature was extended
by comparing perceptions of Kentucky probation and parole officers regarding the amount of alternative sanctions that offenders
would serve to avoid one year of imprisonment to perceptions of the offenders whom they supervised regarding the same topic.
Based on analysis of data from an electronic survey of approximately 230 probation and parole officers and approximately 600
probationers and parolees, results suggested that there were significant differences in bexchange ratesQ of officers when compared
to those of offenders. Justifications for these findings and their implications for correctional policy and practice are discussed.
D 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Introduction
Intermediate sanctions have become an integral part
of the U.S. criminal justice system. Intensive supervision probation, electronic monitoring, boot camps, day
fines, and other sanctions designated as bintermediateQ
have proliferated over the last thirty years. Given current rates of crime and prison crowding, such sentences
can be expected to remain very popular in the foreseeable future.
The intermediate sanctions movement is founded on
the belief that courts have traditionally relied too much
B
An earlier version of this article was presented at the Annual
Meetings of the Southern Criminal Justice Association in Raleigh,
North Carolina, in September 2004.
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 859 622 6681; fax: +1 859 622
6650.
E-mail address: [email protected] (D.C. May).
0047-2352/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2005.11.004
on prison and traditional probation sentences, overusing these two extremes and minimizing alternatives
in dealing out punishments to convicted criminal
offenders (Harris, Petersen, & Rapoza, 2001; Morris
& Tonry, 1990). Though controversies remain, a wide
body of literature evolved around the idea that a
calibrated continuum of intermediate sanctions is potentially desirable and potentially more capable than
the probation-prison dichotomy of meeting the needs
of the offender, correctional system, and the community (e.g., McCarthy, 1987; Morris & Tonry, 1990;
Petersilia & Deschenes, 1994a, 1994b; Smykla &
Selke, 1995). Nevertheless, little empirical evidence
exists to show how this continuum is perceived by
either offenders serving community-based intermediate
sanctions or their supervising officers. In this study,
perceptions of these groups were compared to determine if there are significant differences in punishment
exchange rates, a concept operationalized below.
40
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
Three bodies of literature surround this topic, including literature on: (1) public perceptions of intermediate
sanctions, (2) offender perceptions of intermediate sanctions, and (3) correctional official perceptions of intermediate sanctions. The latter two were compared in this
study. While there was a growing literature on offender
perceptions of alternative sanctions (see May, Wood,
Mooney, & Minor, 2005, for an exhaustive review of
this research), there was little research examining the
perceptions of correctional officials. An examination of
the correctional officers’ perceptions of alternative sanctions with the perceptions of offenders serving actual
sanctions will provide a more complete understanding
of the attitudes of various parties involved in the punishment process. This is essential for a better understanding of the effects of intermediate sanctions.
Literature
Public perceptions
There was limited literature regarding public perceptions of alternative sanctions. Roberts and Stalans
(1997) reviewed several studies and cited research contradicting the viewpoint that the public is excessively
punitive. English, Crouch, and Pullen (1989) and
Applegate, Cullen, and Fisher (2002) demonstrated
that the public was more supportive than judges of
utilizing community corrections alternatives for offenders who had not committed serious offenses and who
had limited criminal histories. This finding was replicated by data from: responses to a national opinion
survey (Haghighi & Lopez, 1998), Minnesota residents
(Bae, 1991), Missouri residents (Fichter & Veneziano,
1988), and jurors (Diamond & Stalans, 1989). Roberts
and Stalans also determined from public opinion surveys of Michigan residents that two out of three respondents believed that, for offenders with limited criminal
histories and who had not committed serious offenses,
community-based alternative sanctions should be used
instead of prison. Furthermore, given the choice of
probation, prison, fines, and fine plus probation for
first time burglary offenders, Roberts and Doob
(1989) reported that only about one out of three Canadian respondents chose prison over the other sentences.
Additionally, half of those who chose prison stated that,
even though they selected prison for the scenario at
hand, they favored community restitution in all or most
cases. Finally, Brown and Elrod (1995) found that,
under certain conditions, New York residents supported
the use of electronic monitoring as an alternative to
incarceration.
Numerous researchers had determined that the public might be less punitive than most policymakers
believe. Research by John Doble Research Associates,
Inc. (1995) also found a preference for the use of
alternative sanctions over prison for nonviolent offenders in North Carolina. More than 80 percent of the
respondents agreed that day reporting, house arrest,
community service, boot camps, and halfway houses
should be used instead of prison for those offenders not
convicted of a violent offense. Furthermore, studies by
both McCorkle (1993) and Cullen, Skovron, Scott, and
Burton (1990) suggested that members of the public
generally support rehabilitative programs for inmates
that afford them better opportunities for success upon
release from prison. Thus, the punitiveness of the public
(as viewed through the eyes of policymakers) seems to
be exaggerated.
The research reviewed above revealed that, among
members of the lay public, alternative sanctions were
often preferred over prison. Nevertheless, none of the
studies reviewed above dealt explicitly with exchange
rates, or preferences in the amount of an alternative
sanction that respondents would choose given a choice
between that alternative and prison. Exchange rate comparisons had been limited to research with offenders.
Offender perceptions of alternative sanctions
Two approaches can be employed when ranking the
severity of sanctions and establishing bexchange ratesQ
between custodial and non-custodial sentences (Byrne,
1990). One approach is based on public perceptions of
the criminal justice system. According to Morris and
Tonry (1990), use of this approach functions primarily
to distribute justice in a manner that satisfies the public
and allows the public to view such rates as applying a
rough equivalence in punishment for blike-situated
offenders.Q The second approach attempts to establish
exchange rates based on an offender’s perspective, with
emphasis on how offenders perceive and/or experience
a range of custodial and non-custodial sanctions. This
approach is based on the theory that offenders’ perspectives are influenced by the length of custodial sentences, as well as by perceptions of the conditions
accompanying non-custodial sentences and the usual
sanctions imposed for failure to comply with these
conditions (Byrne, 1990; Stylianou, 2003). The second
approach was more appropriate for this study, as
responses from a sample of offenders under community
correction supervision and the officers responsible for
their supervision were used to provide the data under
examination here.
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
Over the past twenty years, evidence had accumulated that indicated offenders’ perceptions of punitiveness or severity of criminal sanctions were more
complex than previously assumed (e.g., McClelland
& Alpert, 1985; Petersilia, 1990; Spelman, 1995; Van
Voorhis, Browning, Simon, & Gordon, 1997; Wood,
May, & Grasmick, 2005). This growing body of evidence had implications for theoretical explanations of
illegal conduct (e.g., deterrence, rational choice, and
social learning perspectives) that operate from the explicit premise that people consider rewards and punishments as a prelude to criminal action.
The main implication of this body of evidence is that
researchers need to consider the issue of exactly how
punitive (or rewarding) a given consequence is perceived to be by a given individual. The significance
of this topic is not restricted to the realm of theory, as
all efforts to control crime are grounded in theoretical
presuppositions about why people break the law. Nevertheless, in contrast to Hawkins and Alpert’s (1989)
notion of the relativity of perceptions of sanction severity, extant policies and practices assume an objectivity of punishment seriousness. Indeed, Morris and
Tonry (1990) conceptualized the variety of correctional
sanctions as forming an objective, calibrated continuum
ranging from least (probation) to most (prison) severe,
with such things as community service, electronic monitoring, and halfway houses filling the intermediate
region.
Previous research had also demonstrated that: (a)
some offenders perceive that prison is a deterrent
while others do not; and (b) some offenders prefer
prison over intermediate sanctions. Van Voorhis et al.
(1997) determined that young, non-White, unemployed inmates were significantly more likely than
their fellow inmates to view prison as a deterrent
and feel that they were b. . .getting something out of
prison. . .Q (p. 161). As such, even among those incarcerated in the same setting, not all individuals experience the same bdegreeQ of punishment. Additionally,
Crouch (1993) and Wood and May (2003) reported
that Blacks were more likely than Whites to prefer
prison over ostensibly more lenient alternative sanctions. Likewise, Wood et al. (2005) found that males
were more likely than females to express preferences
for prison over shock incarceration (boot camp). Wood
and Grasmick (1999) also observed significant gender
differences.
Other studies had reported variations by age, marital status, and offense type (Crouch, 1993; Petersilia
& Deschenes, 1994a, 1994b; Spelman, 1995). There
was also evidence (Apospori & Alpert, 1993; Wood &
41
Grasmick, 1999) that having experienced a given
sanction influences subsequent perceptions of the punitiveness of that sanction. Spelman (1995) found that
previous incarcerations were associated with a preference for prison over alternatives; thus, as McClelland
and Alpert (1985, p. 317) observed, it might be that
arrestees bwith large numbers of previous convictions
tend to see imprisonment as relatively trivial.Q
While a growing body of research on offender perceptions currently existed, the literature contained a gap
in relation to how offenders’ perceptions of intermediate sanctions compared with the perceptions of the
officials who worked with the offender population. It
is important that researchers examine this area because
conflicting perceptions between these groups can have
implications for the success of intermediate sanctions in
controlling recidivism.
Correctional official perceptions of intermediate
sanctions
Most of the research on the perceptions of correctional officials revolved around victimization risk and
role conflict (Gordon, Moriarty, & Grant, 2003; Kifer,
Hemmens, & Stohr, 2003). As such, little was known
about the attitudes and beliefs of correctional practitioners regarding the severity of intermediate sanctions.
Nevertheless, the literature did give some insight.
Role orientation perceptions seemed to be the most
relevant part of the existing literature for the topic of
this study. The finding that probation and parole officers had conflicting views of their roles (e.g., as either
law enforcement officers or social workers) might give
some insight into how they viewed the sanction continuum. Numerous studies indicated that many officers
used a combination of the two roles to perform their
jobs (see Fulton, Stichman, Travis, & Latessa, 1997;
and Studt, 1978, for examples).
Hardyman (1988) found that some officers rotate
between the two roles, depending on the perceived
character and behavior of the offender. Consequently,
the role adopted can influence not only the way a
practitioner conducts his/her daily duties (Clear &
O’Leary, 1983), but also her/his belief about the severity of the sanction being supervised. Clear and O’Leary
suggested that the amount of supervision goals set by
an officer were dependent on attitudes of authority and
assistance held by the officer. In other words, officers
with more control-oriented philosophies are more likely
to enforce a higher number of conditions on a consistent basis. Clear and Latessa (1993), however, suggested that officer attitudes might be secondary to
42
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
organizational philosophy when it comes to attitudes
toward rehabilitation among supervised offenders.
Goals of the correctional organization also play an
important role in the perceptions of correctional officials toward intermediate sanctions. Vaughn (1993)
found that directors of state correctional institutions
felt that citizens view incapacitation as the main goal
of correctional facilities and were disinclined toward
rehabilitation, in relation to other goals. Furthermore,
the state directors themselves perceived the main goal
of their facilities to be deterrence, followed by rehabilitation, incapacitation, and finally retribution. Vaughn
noted that the conflict between what directors think the
public perceives as goals, and what the public actually
perceives as goals, may be problematic for policy
implementation.
In a study of correctional facilities in Bermuda
Burton, Ju, Dunaway, and Wolfe (1991) determined
that offender rehabilitation was the most important
goal among correctional officers. Among the officers
surveyed, 78 percent of them believed that rehabilitation was just as important as punishing the offender,
while 45 percent thought that rehabilitation was an
effective means of reducing recidivism. As Burton et
al. determined that 83 percent of the surveyed officers
rated punishment as the worst way to reduce crime,
Bermuda officers seemed to perceive punishment and
rehabilitations as direct opposites. Beliefs like these
would almost surely help condition the attitudes of
correctional officials toward intermediate sanctions.
While a number of studies had examined perceptions
held by the public and by offenders, the research
reviewed above revealed one glaring limitation: few
(if any) studies addressed the topic of alternative sanctions using a sample of correctional practitioners.
Therefore, in this study, offenders’ and officers’ perceptions of the amount of an alternative sanction convicted
offenders would serve to avoid prison were compared;
in other words, the results presented herein compared
exchange rates between offenders under community
supervision and the probation/parole officers who supervised them.
Method
Samples and procedure
Data for this study were collected from two diverse
groups: (1) a sample of offenders under community
correction supervision (during the fall of 2003 from
seven state probation and parole offices in Kentucky);
and (2) a sample of probation and parole officers in the
state of Kentucky (in the spring of 2004). An overview
of that data collection effort is provided below.
Offender sample
The offender data from offenders under study here
were collected from seven probation/parole offices in
Kentucky that were selected using a purposive sampling method to yield large enough proportions of
Black, female, and urban offenders to make meaningful
comparisons between the groups. The final sample
consisted of 612 respondents. This represented just
over 2 (2.3) percent of the approximately 27,000 offenders on probation and parole in Kentucky at the end of
2003.
Members of the research team collected data from
offenders who reported to probation/parole offices to
attend their scheduled (typically, monthly) meeting with
the officers on the day of the arrival of the research
team. Thus, the number of offenders available to provide data varied based on the assistance of the officers
in sending their clients to complete the instrument.
Respondents were provided a letter of consent that:
(a) explained the purpose of the study; (b) informed
the respondent that their participation was completely
voluntary and that they were free to answer any, all, or
none of the questions; (c) assured them of the confidential and anonymous nature of the study; and (d)
asked for the respondent’s signature giving informed
consent. The researcher then asked respondents whether
they would prefer to complete the letter and survey on
their own or have them read aloud. Approximately 10
percent of the respondents asked that the material be
read. Less than one in five (19.0 percent) of the participants declined consent.
Survey instrument
The instrument used to collect the data was an
eight-page questionnaire adapted from the one used
in several other studies (May et al., 2005; Wood &
Grasmick, 1999; Wood & May, 2003; Wood et al.,
2005). After answering a series of demographic questions (presented below), the offenders were presented
with descriptions of nine alternative sanctions, including county jail, boot camp, electronic monitoring,
regular probation, community service, day reporting,
intermittent incarceration, halfway house placement,
and day fine (included in Appendix A). After reading
(or being read) the description of the alternative,
respondents were asked to consider twelve months of
medium-security imprisonment and to indicate how
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
many months of the alternative they were willing to
serve to avoid twelve months imprisonment. In some
cases, the interviewer was asked to clarify the questions;
interviewers did so following a protocol developed by
the second author. In this manner, respondents registered their own perceptions of the severity of alternatives compared to a year in prison. This method
provided respondents with a means of comparing alternatives to imprisonment and indirectly created a ranking
of the severity of the sanctions.
Officer sample
In early January 2004, the authors contacted the
Department of Corrections (DOC) and asked for permission to administer a questionnaire to officers that
was identical to the one previously administered to
offenders as discussed above. Modifications were
made to the demographic portion of the questionnaire
(e.g., asking how long officers had been employed in
the DOC, asking how many offenders officers supervised) to make the instrument appropriate for officers.
With DOC agreement, a modified version of the
questionnaire used to obtain data from the offenders
was developed to administer to the officers. The DOC
contact suggested that the best way to collect data
from the officers would be through use of an elecTable 1
Sample descriptive statistics
Offenders
Officers
Male
Female
N = 588
(percent)*
467 (79.4)
119 (20.2)
N = 208
(percent)*
125 (60.1)
82 (39.4)
Ethnicity
White
Black
350 (59.5)
238 (40.5)
195 (93.8)
11 (5.3)
Mean age
33.29
39.56
Currently on probation or parole
Probation
Parole
351 (59.7)
229 (38.9)
–
–
50
159
228
133
14
3
2 (1.0)
151 (72.6)
53 (25.5)
Gender
Highest level completed education
Eighth grade or less
Some high school
High school graduate
Some college
College graduate
Some graduate/professional
studies
(8.5)
(27.0)
(38.8)
(22.6)
(2.4)
(0.5)
* Percent may not equal 100 due to missing data.
43
tronic questionnaire. As such, at the end of January
2004, the authors created a web site where the officers
could complete the questionnaire and then e-mailed the
DOC contact with a link to the web site and an e-mail to
forward to the officers describing the purpose of the
research and asking the officers to participate. The
contact then forwarded that e-mail to all the probation
and parole officers who currently worked in the state.
After two weeks, she sent a second e-mail (with text
developed by the authors) to remind those who had not
yet participated in the data collection of the survey.
Finally, four weeks after the initial e-mail and two
weeks after the second e-mail reminder, a third e-mail
was sent to ask all who had not participated in the
research to do so. The questionnaire was removed
from the web site in mid-March 2004, approximately
six weeks after the first e-mail was sent to all the
officers. Of the 295 officers who supervised clients in
the state of Kentucky in January 2004, 208 officers
responded, for a participation rate of 70.5 percent.
Officer demographics also appear in Table 1.
Results
Offender descriptive statistics
For the offender sample, respondents provided
background information on age, gender, race, education, and whether they were presently under probationary or parole supervision. With the exception of
the variable measuring their supervision status, identical data were collected from the officers. The descriptive statistics for the offender and officer samples
are presented in Table 1.
The offender sample consisted of 588 respondents
who were serving either probation or parole sentences.
Four in five offenders (79.4 percent) were male. As
previous research with offenders had demonstrated,
Blacks were far more likely than Whites to prefer
prison over alternative sanctions (see May et al.,
2005, for review); as such, only Blacks and Whites
were included in the offender sample. Three in five
offenders (59.5 percent) were White.
Three in five (59.7 percent) offenders in the sample
were on probation. The education levels of the offender
sample varied from less than an eighth grade education
to some graduate or professional studies. Almost one in
ten (8.5 percent) offenders had less than an eighth grade
education and slightly over one in four (27.0 percent)
had completed some high school. Only 2.4 percent had
a college degree, while three offenders had some graduate or professional studies. The remaining offenders
44
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
had either a high school diploma (38.8 percent) or some
college (22.6 percent).
Officer descriptive statistics
Of the 208 officers who provided data for this study,
three in five (60.1 percent) were male and almost all
(93.8 percent) were White. The vast majority of the
respondents (72.6 percent) were college graduates,
while an additional 25.5 percent had taken some postgraduate classes. Two officers (1 percent) responded
that they had taken some college classes; these officers
were likely bgrandfatheredQ for the college education
requirement that applies to officers in Kentucky.
Comparison of exchange rates between offenders and
officers
No previous research had examined exchange rates
among officers providing community supervision, thus,
the primary purpose of this research effort was to
determine whether officers differed from offenders in
their perceptions of the amount of alternative sanctions
offenders would take to avoid prison. Offenders were
asked bWhat is the maximum number of months of [a
particular alternative sanction] you would take to avoid
serving twelve months actual time in prison?Q The
officers were asked the same question, only with respect to their perceptions of how much time a
bconvicted offenderQ would serve. The intermediate
sanctions under study were boot camps, jail, electronic
monitoring, probation, community service, day reporting, intermittent incarceration, halfway houses, and day
fines.
The means and standard deviations for each sample
are presented in Table 2. Responses from both groups
were combined into one data file and independent
sample t-tests were then conducted to examine the
mean differences between the two groups. The results
from those analyses are presented in detail below.
Of the nine sanctions under study, there were statistically significant differences between officer and offender exchange rates on six of the alternative
sanctions. With the exception of community service
(discussed in detail below), in sanctions where there
were statistically significant differences between the
samples, the mean exchange rate for offenders was
significantly less than the mean exchange rate for the
officer sample. For example, the exchange rate for
regular probation that the officers felt offenders would
serve to avoid twelve months prison was almost twice
as high as what the offenders themselves stated they
would actually serve (a mean of 44.23 months for
officers and 23.56 months for offenders). While the
disparity between the two samples was not as great
for any of the other sanctions, the consistent pattern
was that the exchange rate that the officers thought
offenders would serve was almost twice as high as
the mean exchange rate for the offenders. Thus, offenders had significantly lower exchange rates than officers for county jail (5.55 months versus 8.12 months),
electronic monitoring (13.95 months versus 17.57
months), day reporting (17.01 months versus 22.62
months), and intermittent incarceration (14.60 months
versus 16.88 months). Nevertheless, officers did have a
sense that offenders did not view prison as the most
stringent punishment; for both officers and offenders,
jail and boot camp were viewed as the most stringent
Table 2
Comparison of officer and offender exchange rates
Sentence
Boot camp
County jail
Electronic monitoring
Regular probation
Community servicea
Day reporting
Intermittent incarceration
Halfway house
Day fine
Officers
Offenders
Mean
S.D.
% None
Max.
Mean
S.D.
% None
Max.
6.05
8.12***
17.57***
44.23***
8.76
22.62***
16.88*
15.62
13.48
4.02
5.79
12.15
25.61
12.67
20.24
13.21
12.96
18.83
3.9
1.9
1.5
0.5
1.5
1.5
0.5
0.5
4.9
24
60
60
120
50.0
90
60
75
120
6.07
5.55
13.95
23.56
22.72***
17.01
14.60
14.42
12.22
5.54
4.81
11.82
16.54
21.84
14.96
12.34
11.96
12.80
19.2
17.4
8.6
3.1
5.7
9.3
7.5
7.2
17.6
28
26
48
100
62.5
56
48
48
48
a
Community service was converted to months by dividing the total number of hours of community service by eighty (at a rate of twenty hours
per week on community service).
* p b .05.
** p b .01.
*** p b .001.
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
sanctions, although the two samples differed in order of
precedence.
The two samples’ views of the stringency of community service when compared to prison presented an
interesting anomaly when compared to the exchange
rates for the other sanctions. Officers perceived that
convicted offenders would do an average of 700.5
hours of community service to avoid twelve months
in prison, while offenders were willing to do over
twice as many community service hours (an average
of 1,817.3 hours) to avoid twelve months in prison.
This was the only alternative sanction where the mean
for the offenders group was higher than that of the
officer group. (This finding is discussed in detail
below.)
For each sanction, there were a number of offenders
and a small number of officers who stated that they (or
a bconvicted offenderQ for the officers) would not do
any amount of the sanction in question to avoid twelve
months in prison. One in twenty officers (4.9 percent)
felt that convicted offenders would refuse to do any
amount of day fine, and one in twenty-five (3.9 percent)
felt that convicted offenders would not participate in
any amount of boot camp. For all the other sanctions,
less than one in fifty officers felt that convicted offenders would refuse to engage in that sanction to avoid
twelve months in prison.
Offenders, however, had very different opinions regarding whether or not they would completely avoid
some sanctions to avoid twelve months prison. With the
exception of regular probation (3.3 percent), at least one
in twenty offenders stated that they would not serve any
time on the alternative sanction to avoid twelve months in
prison. In fact, one in five offenders (19.2 percent) stated
that they would not do any time in boot camp to avoid
twelve months in prison, with day fines (17.6 percent)
and county jail (17.4 percent) having almost as high a
percentage of refusals. It appeared, then, that for a core
group of offenders in this sample, prison was preferred
over any duration of an alternative. These results could
show that offenders felt that intermediate incarceration
was more harsh than prison, whereas, officers perceived
that offenders felt that prison was more harsh.
The results presented in Table 3 depict the means for
both the samples to allow an easy visual comparison of
the exchange rates. Observe that prison was scored as
twelve months because all respondents generated exchange rates in comparison to twelve months of imprisonment. As the results depicted, officers felt that
offenders would do far greater amounts of regular
probation to avoid prison (44.23 versus 23.56 months).
Furthermore, the exchange rates for the offenders were
45
Table 3
Comparison of officer and offender exchange rates
Officers
Offenders
Sentence (months)
Sentence (months)
Regular probation (44.23)
Day reporting (22.62)
Electronic monitoring (17.57)
Intermittent incarceration (16.88)
Halfway house (15.62)
Day fine (13.48)
Prison (12.00 months)
Community service (8.76)
County jail (8.12)
Boot camp (6.05)
Regular probation (23.56)
Community service (22.72)
Day reporting (17.01)
Intermittent incarceration (14.60)
Halfway house (14.42)
Electronic monitoring (13.95)
Day fine (12.22)
Prison (12.00 months)
Boot camp (6.07)
County jail (5.55)
also more likely to hover around twelve months (e.g.,
12.22 months for day fine, 13.95 months for electronic
monitoring, 14.42 months for halfway house) than for
the officers. Once again, the results presented in this
table suggested that, compared to officers, offenders
were much more likely to equate twelve months of
most alternative sanctions with twelve months of prison, though offenders clearly perceived boot camp and
county jail to be significantly more severe than prison.
With regard to the severity rankings, two major discrepancies between officers’ and offenders’ rankings
should be noted. While both officers and offenders
identified regular probation as the least severe sanction,
the officers’ rankings indicated a significant gap between probation and the next most severe sanction (day
reporting)—a nearly twenty-two months difference in
exchange rates. Among officers, the magnitude of this
difference was greater than the difference between day
reporting and boot camp—the most punitive sanction in
the officer ranking. Officers ranked probation as significantly less punitive than did offenders, and felt that
offenders would serve roughly twice as much probation
to avoid imprisonment than offenders said they would.
Put another way, offenders appeared to rate probation as
significantly more punitive than did officers.
In addition, note that officers ranked community
service as the third most punitive sanction just behind
county jail, and ahead of imprisonment, while offenders
ranked it as the second least punitive sanction just
above regular probation. Clearly, officers and offenders
differed in their perception of the severity of serving
community service.
Finally, observe that with the exception of officers’
ranking of regular probation, the exchange rate range
46
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
was similar for both officers and offenders, with exchange rates for all sanctions other than officers’ rating
of probation grouping between 5.55 months for the
offender county jail rate and 23.56 months for the
offender regular probation rate.
Discussion and conclusion
Utilizing a sample of probationers, parolees, and
probation/parole officers from Kentucky, this effort
extended the extant body of research on offender exchange rates (e.g., May & Wood, 2005; May et al.,
2005; Wood & Grasmick, 1999; Wood & May, 2003) to
a comparison between officers and offenders under
their supervision. The results from this study suggested
that, with the exception of community service, officers
felt that offenders were likely to do longer amounts of
alternative sanctions to avoid prison than offenders
themselves stated they would do. Furthermore, for
county jail, electronic monitoring, regular probation,
and day reporting, there were statistically significant
differences in the exchange rates among the two
groups. In each of these cases, the exchange rates of
the officers were higher than those of offenders, indicating that officers felt prison was a more stringent
sanction (when compared to these alternative sanctions)
than did the offenders. Consequently, the results from
this research suggested three important conclusions
with implications for policy.
First, the results from this research revealed that,
despite significant differences in the amount of alternative sanctions officers felt offenders would do to avoid
prison, officers did appear to have a rough understanding that offenders did not view prison as the harshest
punishment (based on rankings of severity fairly close
to matching offenders). From this standpoint, it is important that officers continue to have a voice in sentencing, as they appear to have a good understanding of
the sanctions offenders think are most stringent. Many
jurisdictions currently use pre-sentence investigation
reports written by probation officers in their sentencing
recommendations, and these findings support the utility
of the PSI for helping to calibrate sanction severity with
at least some regard for offenders’ subjective perceptions of sanctions.
Secondly, in comparison to offenders, officers rated
community service as far more difficult. The implications of this finding are threefold. First, this finding
could be based on the officer’s past experience with
offenders whom they supervised bsuccessQ in completing community service hours. As such, offenders may
be basing their perceptions on how much they would
be bwillingQ to serve to avoid prison while officers
may be basing their perceptions on how much community service offenders on their caseload have
bactually completedQ to avoid prison. Secondly, and
perhaps congruently, these findings may also indicate
that officers feel community service is more of a
hassle to offenders than what the offenders think it
would be. As such, given their role orientation (Gordon et al., 2003; Kifer et al., 2003), officers may be
either more likely (punitive role orientation) or less
likely (social worker orientation) to recommend community service as a condition of an offender’s community sanction. Those officers who follow a punitive role
orientation may use community service as a bclubQ to
either force offenders into compliance with community
supervision or design conditions to make it easier to
violate offenders, based on knowledge that they will be
unlikely to complete the sanction given the onerous
nature (from the officer’s perspective) of community
service. Officers with a social work orientation, on the
other hand, may be less likely to use community service
as one of the conditions of probation or parole based on
their sentiment that offenders will not comply with the
requirements of community service and, as such, will
be less likely to complete the sentence. Irrespective of
role orientation, it is apparent that the officers view
community service as more onerous than the offenders
sentenced to community service, and this difference
may have dramatic implications for community service
as a condition of probation or parole. Future research
should explore this topic in greater detail to determine
why community service is perceived as a more severe
sanction by officers than by offenders. Lastly, it is
possible that officers may also incorporate a degree of
shame/embarrassment into doing community service,
and may feel that being forced to do community service
in their home communities would expose them to the
possibility of shame and embarrassment associated with
community disapproval—something that offenders may
not view as a serious concern.
Finally, this research also revealed two other discrepancies between the perceptions of the officers and
those of the offenders. First and foremost, offenders
were far more likely to refuse to do any amount of an
alternative sanction than officers thought to be true.
Thus, as Wood, May, and their colleagues had suggested in other works (see May & Wood, 2005; May
et al., 2005; Wood et al., 2005), there are some offenders who feel that any agreement to participate in
alternative sanctions is only prolonging the inevitability
of recidivism and incarceration. In other words, they
feel the likelihood of being incarcerated for violations
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
of conditions of any form of alternative sanction is so
high that they would rather go to prison immediately,
rather than attempting to meet the requirements of an
alternative sanction, then ending up in prison anyway
following failure.
A primary implication of this finding is that when
community/local sanctions (electronic monitoring, probation, day reporting, and jail) are compared with
imprisonment, probation/parole officers will often
overestimate the extent to which offenders perceive
imprisonment as being punitive. This, in turn, potentially has major implications for the supervision strategies that probation/parole officers have traditionally
employed. Those strategies are based on the assumption that revocation followed by imprisonment is the
ultimate punitive sanction as a means of enforcing
compliance with conditions. The present findings,
nevertheless, contradict that assumption and suggest
that supervision strategies based on this logic may be
misdirected. Indeed, the findings presented here suggest that officers would do as well, or perhaps better,
to rely on threats of electronic monitoring, day reporting, or jail to enforce compliance. According to the
data, revocation and subsequent imprisonment are not
as punitive to offenders as to officers and supervision
strategies that take this into account might aid in
reducing the extent to which probation/parole revocations presently contribute to prison crowding.
At a more fundamental level, the basic options for
encouraging compliance with probation/parole conditions are to: (a) reward or give incentives for compliant
behavior, or (b) punish, or threaten to punish, noncompliance. In general, community supervision strategies
have placed far more emphasis on the latter than the
former. Yet the effectiveness of the latter approach is in
no small way contingent on the assumption that offenders and officers share similar subjective definitions of
the level of punishment associated with various sanctions. The findings challenge this assumption and,
hence, the viability of supervision strategies based on
the assumption.
These data also suggest that officers are more
optimistic and feel that any offender can successfully
complete alternative sanctions if they so desire. While
this disjunction in opinion may be expected given the
diametrically opposed roles in the criminal justice
system, it is enlightening as well. Officers’ confidence that offenders can complete alternative sanctions is likely a function of the bsuccess storiesQ that
they have supervised. Secondly, as the findings in
Tables 2 and 3 reveal, offenders rate probation as
significantly more punitive than do officers. Thus,
47
regardless of the explanation for this finding, offenders and officers have divergent views on the nature
(and perhaps severity) of regular probation as an
alternative to prison.
Given this, it might be helpful for probation/parole
agencies to establish a bmentoring programQ for new
offenders under their supervision, with past bsuccess
storiesQ used as the mentors. Exposing an offender who
is serving an alternative sanction for the first time to a
mentor who has successfully completed that same sanction could be helpful in a number of ways. First, it
would give the newly sentenced offender a positive role
model, a person similar to them who successfully completed the alternative sanction in question. Secondly, it
would let the newly sentenced offender know that
successful completion of the sentence is possible,
even though they may consider completion unlikely;
they may begin to develop confidence that, given some
work and sacrifice, they too can successfully complete
the alternative sanction.
The research reviewed here was not without limitations. There was very little variation in the officer
sample by gender and race, two of the strongest predictors of preference for prison over alternative sanctions among offenders. As such, research such as this
should be conducted with larger, more diverse samples
from other regions to insure that the findings presented
here are not an artifact of the sample used. Secondly,
research regarding perceived exchange rates needs to be
extended to judges, legislators, and other criminal justice officials to determine how their exchange rates
compare with those of probation/parole officers and
offenders. To develop a continuum of sanctions that is
truly fair, just, and possibly more effective at controlling recidivism, input from all the aforementioned
groups needs to be considered.
Appendix A
The text used to describe each of the alternative
sanctions is included below.
A.1. County jail
If you are sent to a county jail, you may spend less
time there than you would in prison. Living conditions,
however, are more restrictive in a jail than they generally are in a large prison. Unless assigned to work, you
may spend more time in your housing unit, and there
are not as many opportunities for sports, school, etc.
Jail time is generally viewed as more boring and more
restrictive than prison time.
48
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
A.2. Boot camp
Boot camp is for a shorter time than you would have
been sent to prison. Boot camp can be more unpleasant
in many ways than living in prison. Boot camp is like
basic training in the army. You live with about a
hundred other people in one big room. There is regular
drill instruction like in the military and you are pushed
physically and psychologically to perform beyond your
capabilities. You experience loss of sleep. You are
required to become physically active and fit. You are
constantly supervised by drill instructors who watch
you closely. You are generally required to participate
in an education program. Virtually all your time and
activities are controlled. You are subject to random
urinalysis tests and can be sent back to prison if you
fail to obey the rules.
A.3. Electronic monitoring
On electronic monitoring, you live at home, but your
freedom is greatly reduced. You wear an electronic
device on your ankle. If you get more than two hundred
feet from your telephone, the device sends an alarm to a
computer. Then an officer who is supervising you
knows that you are not where you are supposed to
be. On electronic monitoring, you are being followed
by the computer twenty-four hours a day. There are
strict curfews and rules about when you must stay near
your phone. If you break these rules, you can be sent to
prison. You are subject to random urinalysis tests and
can be sent back to prison if you fail to obey the rules.
A.4. Regular probation
On probation, you do not spend time in prison, but
the amount of time on probation usually lasts much
longer than whatever prison sentence you might have
gotten. You must see your probation officer at least
once a month, but it can be every week if ordered.
You must get permission from that probation officer to
travel or to move. Your probation officer can require
that you stay away from certain people. Your home or
car can be searched at any time without a search
warrant. If you do not follow the rules, you can be
sent to prison. You are also subject to random urinalysis
tests.
work some time without pay to make up for the crime
for which you were convicted. You work for a government agency or some local nonprofit organization, and
you do not have any choice about where or what the job
is. The judge decides the number of days and hours you
must work. If you fail to work the required days and
hours, you can be sent back to prison. You are also
subject to random urinalysis testing.
A.6. Day reporting
If you are sentenced to day reporting, you can stay
home at night, but you must check in at a parole office
every day. During the day you must have a job or you
must go to some center in the community and be
involved in activities all day. These activities might
include working for no pay in the community, looking
for a job, counseling, job training, and education programs. At the end of the day, you get to go home. You
may be required to work, and if you do, you must
check in every day during non-work hours. Failure
to abide by the rules can result in you going back to
prison. You are also subject to random urinalysis
testing.
A.7. Intermittent incarceration
With this punishment, you must spend weekends or
evenings in the county jail, which typically is much
more unpleasant than prison. Since you are not in
prison, you can have a job and be involved with your
family and community when you are not spending time
in jail. Failure to report to jail or failure to pass a
random urinalysis test, however, can result in you
returning to prison.
A.8. Halfway house
A halfway house is a place where several people
convicted of crimes live. There is no strict security like
there is in prison, but there are firm rules that you must
follow. Halfway houses have rehabilitative programs,
and if your behavior improves you are treated better
and given more freedom. Break the rules and you can
be placed back in prison. As always, you are subject to
random urinalysis and searches, and constant observation. You are not allowed to have visitors.
A.5. Community service
A.9. Day fine
When you are sentenced to community service, you
live at home and can have a job. You must, however,
A day fine is based on the amount of money you
make each day. You are allowed to subtract some
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
money for your rent, transportation, food, utilities,
etc., but whatever is left over you have to pay as a
day fine. For example, if you had $20 left each day
after expenses, your day fine would be $20 for every
day the judge says you have to pay. If the judge
gives you a day fine of ninety days, and your day
fine rate is $20, you would have to pay a total of
$1,800. Failure to pay your fines can result in you
being sent back to prison.
References
Apospori, E., & Alpert, G. (1993). Research note: The role of
differential experience with the criminal justice system in changes
in perception of severity of legal sanctions over time. Crime and
Delinquency, 39, 184 – 194.
Applegate, B. K., Cullen, F. T., & Fisher, B. S. (2002). Public views
toward crime and correctional policies: Is there a gender gap?
Journal of Criminal Justice, 30, 89 – 100.
Bae, I. (1991). A survey on public acceptance of restitution as an
alternative to incarceration for property offenders in Hennepin
County, Minnesota. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University
of Minnesota, Minneapolis.
Brown, M. P., & Elrod, P. (1995). Electronic house arrest: An
examination of citizen attitudes. Crime and Delinquency, 41,
332 – 346.
Burton, V. S., Ju, X., Dunaway, R. G., & Wolfe, N. T. (1991). The
correctional orientation of Bermuda prison guards: An assessment of attitudes towards punishment and rehabilitation. International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice,
15, 71 – 80.
Byrne, J. M. (1990). The future of intensive probation supervision
and the new intermediate sanctions. Crime and Delinquency, 36,
6 – 41.
Clear, T., & Latessa, E. (1993). Probation officers roles in intensive
supervision: Surveillance vs. treatment. Justice Quarterly, 10(3),
441 – 459.
Clear, T. R., & O’Leary, V. O. (1983). Controlling the offender in the
community. Lexington, MA7 Lexington Books.
Crouch, B. M. (1993). Is incarceration really worse? Analysis of
offenders’ preferences for prison over probation. Justice Quarterly,
10, 67 – 88.
Cullen, F. T., Skovron, S. E., Scott, J. E., & Burton, V. S. (1990).
Public support for correctional treatment: The tenacity of rehabilitative ideology. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 17, 6 – 18.
Diamond, S., & Stalans, L. J. (1989). The myth in judicial leniency in
sentencing. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 7, 73 – 89.
English, K., Crouch, J., & Pullen, P. (1989). Attitudes towards crime:
A survey of Colorado citizens and criminal justice officials.
Denver7 Colorado Department of Public Safety, Division of Criminal Justice.
Fichter, M., & Veneziano, C. (1988). Criminal justice attitudes—
Missouri. Jefferson City7 Missouri Department of Corrections.
Fulton, B., Stichman, A., Travis, L., & Latessa, E. (1997). Moderating
probation and parole officer attitudes to achieve desired outcomes.
Prison Journal, 77(3), 295 – 312.
Gordon, J. A., Moriarty, L. J., & Grant, P. H. (2003). Juvenile
correctional officers’ perceived fear and risk of victimization.
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 30(1), 62 – 84.
49
Haghighi, B., & Lopez, A. (1998). Gender and perceptions of prisons
and prisoners. Journal of Criminal Justice, 26(6), 453 – 464.
Hardyman, P. (1988). No frills: A study of probation resources,
activities and outcome. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ.
Harris, P. M., Petersen, R. D., & Rapoza, S. (2001). Between probation and revocation: A study of intermediate sanctions decisionmaking. Journal of Criminal Justice, 29, 307 – 318.
Hawkins, R., & Alpert, G. P. (1989). American prison systems:
Punishment and justice. Englewood Cliffs, NJ7 Prentice-Hall.
John Doble Research Associates, Inc. (1995). Crime and corrections: The views of the people of North Carolina. Retrieved
December 1, 2004, from http://www.doc.state.nc.us/NEWS/1996/
96news/doblerep.htm
Kifer, M., Hemmens, C., & Stohr, M. K. (2003). The goals of
corrections: Perspectives from the lines. Criminal Justice Review,
28(1), 47 – 69.
May, D. C., & Wood, P. B. (2005). What influences offenders’
willingness to serve alternative sanctions. Prison Journal, 85,
145 – 167.
May, D. C., Wood, P. B., Mooney, J. L., & Minor, K. I. (2005). What
influences offenders’ willingness to serve alternative sanctions?
Crime and Delinquency, 51, 373 – 399.
McCarthy, B. R. (Ed.). (1987). Intermediate punishments: Intensive
supervision, home confinement and electronic monitoring. Monsey, NY7 Criminal Justice Press.
McClelland, K. A., & Alpert, G. (1985). Factor analysis applied to
magnitude estimates of punishment seriousness: Patterns of individual differences. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 1,
307 – 318.
McCorkle, R. C. (1993). Research note: Punish and rehabilitate?
Public attitudes towards six common crimes. Crime and Delinquency, 39, 240 – 252.
Morris, N., & Tonry, M. (1990). Between prison and probation:
Intermediate punishments in a rational sentencing system. New
York7 Oxford University Press.
Petersilia, J. (1990). When probation becomes more dreaded than
prison. Federal Probation, 54, 23 – 27.
Petersilia, J., & Deschenes, E. P. (1994a). What punishes? Inmates
rank the severity of prison vs. intermediate sanctions. Federal
Probation, 58(1), 3 – 8.
Petersilia, J., & Deschenes, E. P. (1994b). Perceptions of punishment:
Inmates and staff rank the severity of prison versus intermediate
sanctions. Prison Journal, 74, 306 – 328.
Roberts, J. V., & Doob, A. N. (1989). Sentencing and public opinion:
Taking false shadows for true substances. Osgoode Hall Law
Journal, 27, 491 – 515.
Roberts, J. V., & Stalans, L. J. (1997). Public opinion, crime and
criminal justice. Boulder, CO7 Westview Press, Harper Collins
Publishers.
Smykla, J. O., & Selke, W. L. (Eds.). (1995). Intermediate sanctions:
Sentencing in the 1990s. Cincinnati, OH7 Anderson.
Spelman, W. (1995). The severity of intermediate sanctions. Journal
of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 32, 107 – 135.
Studt, E. (1978). Surveillance and service to parole. Washington, DC7
U.S. Department of Justice, National Institute of Corrections.
Stylianou, S. (2003). Measuring crime seriousness perceptions: What
have we learned and what else do we want to know? Journal of
Criminal Justice, 14(1), 57 – 67.
Van Voorhis, P., Browning, S. L., Simon, M., & Gordon, J. (1997).
The meaning of punishment: Inmates’ orientation to the prison
experience. Prison Journal, 77(2), 135 – 166.
50
C.M. Flory et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 34 (2006) 39–50
Vaughn, M. S. (1993). Listening to the experts: A national study of
correctional administrators’ responses to prison overcrowding.
Criminal Justice Review, 18, 12 – 25.
Wood, P. B., & Grasmick, H. G. (1999). Toward the development of
punishment equivalencies: Male and female inmates rate the
severity of alternative sanctions compared to prison. Justice Quarterly, 16, 19 – 50.
Wood, P. B., & May, D. C. (2003). Race differences in perceptions of
sanction severity: A comparison of prison with alternatives. Justice Quarterly, 20, 605 – 631.
Wood, P. B., May, D. C., & Grasmick, H. G. (2005). Gender differences in the perceived severity of boot camp. Journal of Offender
Rehabilitation, 40(3–4), 145 – 167.