Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Movement of the common periwinkle (Littorina littorea) at Woodneck Beach, Falmouth, Massachusetts Kate Buckman, Annette Hynes, and Elizabeth Orchard (for Marine Invertebrates of Cape Cod, Topics Course, Fall Semester 2005) Abstract The intertidal community is an interesting ecosystem to study because of its clear zonation of plants and animals attributed to such factors as physical and competitive pressures. While sessile organisms are forced to maintain a specific location, motile species are capable of changing their position, and therefore are able to maintain a preferred tidal height. This ability is particularly striking with the snail Littorina littorea, whose trails can cover a beach at low tide. In these experiments we studied periwinkle movement with respect to tidal cycle. We also observed winkles on the move to determine velocity and direction of motion of both submerged and above water individuals. We determined that, while periwinkles are capable of “large-scale” movements, they tend to remain stationary during the course of a tidal cycle. Introduction The intertidal zone has long been the setting for classic ecological studies. While the observation of sessile species is straightforward, the distribution of intertidal species along physical gradients is less concrete for mobile species. Adult migration influences patterns of biodiversity and community structure and is significant in determining species ranges, extinction rates and coexistence (Davidson et al., 2004). Invasive species also add complexity to intertidal environments. They affect the ecology and evolution of native species through competition, predation, hybridization, disease, and structural engineering (Grosholz, 2002). For both its motility and invasive history, the periwinkle Littorina littorea is important in shaping the biodiversity of New England intertidal and subtidal habitats. L. littorea (Fig. 1) is known as the common periwinkle, the edible periwinkle, and the wrinkle winkle. Its range extends from the British Isles and northwest Europe to the North American Atlantic coast from Labrador to New Jersey. L. littorea became abundant in New England only after European settlement in America and the first live specimen was collected in 1840 (DFO report, 1998). It has displaced the periwinkle L. palliata and the mud snail Ilyanassa obsoleta in some areas (DFO report, 1998; Brenchley & Carlton, 1983). The average winkle lives three years and grows to a shell height of 20 mm, but the largest recorded winkle grew to 52 mm (Jackson, 2005). Winkles eat microalgae such as benthic diatoms and dinoflagellates as well as macroalgae such as Ulva lactuca and ephemeral and juvenile green, red and brown algae (Jackson, 2005). Winkles aggregate and inhabit the understory of rockweed such as Fucus vesiculosis and Aschophyllum nodosum but can also be found on rocks, in crevices, and on sand (DFO report, 1998). There are about twelve species of the genus Littorina, the most common in New England being L. littorea, L. obtusata, and L. saxatilis. L. littorea can be identified by its striped tentacles and its spire that is neither as sharp as that of L. saxatilis nor as smooth as that of L. obtusata (Crowe, www). These three littorinids also have very different modes of reproduction; L. littorea releases eggs to the sea, hatching into planktotrophic larvae while L. obtusata eggs hatch into crawling juveniles and L. saxatilis has direct development (Crowe, www). Motile intertidal animals need to be able to maintain their position on the shore and to orient themselves if they are dislocated or if the preferred habitat changes (Gendron, 1977). L. littorea moves in the direction of the preferred tidal height when it is dislocated (Gendron, 1977; Petraitis, 1982). On Gansett Point, Woods Hole, MA, the greatest winkle densities are at and below mean low water (MLW) with larger winkles preferring lower depths and having a larger range (Gendron, 1977). Winkles at the upper levels migrate downward in winter. Some investigations have shown that winkles use phototactic and geotactic responses to orient themselves (Petraitis, 1982), but others have shown the primary directional cue to be wave motion (Gendron, 1977). As winkles move, they mark or track mucus trails. Mucus trails are energetically costly, so the winkles must gain some benefit from making them. Mucus trails bind microalgal cells on which the tracker winkles can graze (Davies and Beckwith, 1999). Trail-following may also aid in winkle aggregation for food, shelter or reproduction. The complexity of winkle motion is less than chaotic, as in Brownian motion, so motion is non-random and directional (Erlandsson & Kostylev, 1995). Winkles follow conspecific trails more often than trails of other species, but reports differ on whether the winkles follow conspecific trails or their own trails more often (Davies and Beckwith, 1999; Erlandsson & Kostylev, 1995). Male winkles mount whatever snail they encounter on a trail; however, males may identify females and even trematode-infected females, which can become sterile, by their trails (Erlandsson & Kostylev, 1995). L. littorea engage in fewer male-male copulations than L. saxatilis, indicating a difference in trail-following behavior in these two species (Erlandsson & Kostylev, 1995). In this study, we examined the site fidelity, distance, and velocity of motion of Littorina littorea at Woodneck Beach, Falmouth, MA. Materials and Methods The three experiments performed are listed in Table 1. All periwinkles participating in the study were marked at low tide, using nail polish (Revlon ‘raven red’, ‘twinkled pink’; Nailslicks ‘electric blue’, neon green). If the shells were wet, it was necessary to dry them with a cloth. On 25 Sept 2005 we marked 100 periwinkles at each of three sites at Woodneck Beach. The sites (Fig. 2) consisted of the seaward face of a large rock located near the low tide line in the middle of the beach (‘raven red’ winkles), three small rocks in a channel connecting the ocean to the marsh behind the beach (green winkles), and a “winkle mat” consisting of numerous periwinkles aggregated on algae near the marsh (‘twinkled pink’ winkles). We placed rocks on the winkle mat to facilitate finding the same location again. The following day at low tide we returned to each site, counted the number of painted winkles that were visible, and measured the approximate distance of winkle movement. Winkles were located by finding the original site of marking and walking systematically in increasingly larger circles around the site. Distance of winkle movement was measured from a central point in the original marking area to the winkle, as it was impossible to determine exactly where the winkle started from. On 02 Oct 2005 we returned to Woodneck and conducted winkle velocity measurements. As winkle trails were only readily visible in sandy areas, all velocity measurements were made on sand both exposed to air, and submerged beneath the water. Three snails were observed on the exposed sand, two were on a mucous trail (Fig. 3a), and one was not following a previously made trail. The snails were observed for 10 minutes and the distance moved during that interval measured. Eight snails were placed in a line a few centimeters above the water level and observed for 10 minutes while being covered by the incoming tide (Fig. 3b). Concurrently, two fully submerged snails were observed and their distance traveled measured. On 12 Oct 2005 100 snails were again marked at low tide at three locations on the beach: an area of cobbles, the same large rock as experiment one, and the same winkle mat as experiment one (Fig. 4). The channel was not used in this experiment due to the difficulty relocating snails as a result of shifting sand in previous experiments. All winkles were marked with the same color nail polish (Nailslicks ‘electric blue’) to eliminate potential effects of selective predation based on winkle color. The cobble and the winkle mat site were marked with stakes. We returned to each site at two hour intervals until high tide, and again looked for marked snails and measured their distance from a central starting point. Table 1 Date Time 25/Sep/05 0630-0800 Tide level 1.0 26/Sep/05 0730-0900 1.2 02/Oct/05 1230-1430 0.3 12/Oct/05 0930 1200 1400 1600 0.7 Habitat(s) Objective Rock, channel, algal mat near marsh Rock, channel, algal mat near marsh Mark and release Recapture, measure distance Velocity experiments Mark, release, recapture Sand bar parallel to cobble beach Cobble beach, Rock, algal mat near marsh 4.5 Results Results for the first set of winkle deployments are summarized in Table 2. The greatest recovery after one full tidal cycle was achieved on the large rock, with 58 winkles recovered in the marking area and 8 winkles either on different rock faces or on the seafloor surrounding the rock. The greatest distance moved was 254 cm, though we were unable to determine whether the winkle had moved itself there independently of wave action or currents. 24 winkles were recovered in the channel, 10 no longer associated with the rocks, with a greatest distance moved of 284.5cm. Recovery in the channel was difficult as the deployment rocks were partially obscured by sand shifting with the current. Currents were strong at the winkle mat as well, as indicated by our marker rocks having been washed away. The mat itself appeared to be in the same position, but only 5 winkles were recovered, even after disturbing the mat to search for winkles that had moved vertically within the mat as opposed to horizontally along the substrate. Table 2 Habitat Rock Channel Mat # Recovered 58 + 8 24 5 Distances (cm) 68.6, 71.1, 78.7, 81.3, 101.6, 254 12.7, 40.6, 76.2, 81.3, 119.4, 142.2, 226.1, 284.5 Uncertain The winkle velocity experiments showed a wide variety of movement velocities (Table 3). In general, winkles on previously established mucous trails moved faster than those not on trails, but sample size was not large enough to determine if there is a significant difference. Winkles that were disturbed and placed in a line did not move at all, and those submerged did not move faster than those on exposed sand. Winkle travel was not constant; the winkles would often stop for a while for no apparent reason before resuming movement. There did not appear to be any relationship between winkle size and distance traveled within the confines of our experimental design. Table 3 Description Exposed Submerged Mucus trail No mucus trail Line race Individual Individual Winkle Size (cm) 1.3 0.8 1.0 Distance (cm) Comments 13.7 15.9 8.3 range 1.3 0 14.0 took a break crossed a trail Found a trail but promptly left it No motion Small snail beside him much faster Stopped on top of rock in path 8.9 The third experiment was implemented to determine if the winkles moved while submerged and returned to a particular position at low tide. Results of these observations are summarized in Table 4. In the cobble area 40 winkles were recovered 2 hours after low tide, with a maximum movement of 66cm. Following this time, the stake marking the site washed away and we were unable to located any winkles despite running snorkel transects through the area. At the rock, recovery was greatest 2 hours following deployment with all of the winkles recovered being found on the same face of the rock as they were deployed. At high tide, a similar number of winkles were recovered, but a larger number (18) were found off of the deployment face of the rock than at the earlier times. A number had crawled up the rock and were found at the air/water interface, while others were on the seafloor surrounding the rock. Conditions were poor at the 1400 hour observation time, making an accurate count of winkles difficult (noted with *). The same is true for the winkle mat area. Highest recovery of any site was achieved at the winkle mat 2 hours after deployment. No significant movement was noticed in the mat at anytime. Table 4 Habitat Time # Recovered Cobbles 1200 1400 1600 1200 1400 1600 1200 1400 1600 40 lost lost 57 20* 30 + 18 71 10* 37 Rock Mat Max Distance (cm) 66.0 167 61 same area Discussion The winkles at Woodneck Beach demonstrated an ability to move fairly far distances, but in general did not take advantage of this ability and showed fairly high and stable site fidelity. These results support previous observations that periwinkles have a preferred tidal height to inhabit where they maintain position, and will return to this height if displaced (Gendron, 1977). There did not seem to be a difference in movement whether the periwinkles were submerged or not, and most winkles maintained position both within and over a tidal cycle. In other words, the winkles didn’t move out of place when submerged, and then return to it for the next low tide or vice versa, they just stayed in the same place the whole time. In fact, we were still able to find red periwinkles on the large rock over 20 days after having marked them there. Though this study was able to demonstrate that the periwinkles at Woodneck Beach do not tend to move from their assumed preferred position over a tidal cycle, it also raised many questions. Future studies would benefit from technical and methodical considerations such as finding a faster drying marker than nail polish, and one that won’t wash off over time to allow for longer time series, better winkle position markers to facilitate finding sites again, as well as better methods of quantifying distances and direction of winkle movement. Other factors to be taken into consideration would be the effect of marking winkles on predation, the effect of season and weather on winkle motility, and the difference between subtidal and intertidal winkle movements. Figures: Figure 1: Littorina littorea on a mucous trail. a. b. c. Figure 2: a. Large rock, site of red winkle deployment. b. sandy channel, site of green winkle deployment. b. winkle mat, site of pink winkle deployment. b. a. Figure 3: a. exposed winkles on trails. b. submerged winkles in line after 10 minutes of observation. a. b. c. Figure 4: a. painted winkles on cobbles at low tide. b. large rock during winkle painting. c. winkle mat during painting at low tide. References Brenchley, G. A. and Carlton, J. T. (1983). competitive displacement of native mud snails by introduced periwinkles in the New England intertidal zone. Biological Bulletin 165: 543-558. Crowe, B. Seashore Identification Notes: WINKLES, Family Littorinidae. http://staffweb.itsligo.ie/staff/bcrowe/bill/styles/frames/marbiol/sshorero/litrslon.h tm Davidson, I. C., A. C. Crook, and D. K. A. Barnes (2004). Macrobenthic migration and its influence on the intertidal diversity dynamics of a meso-tidal system. Marine Biology 145: 833-842. Davies, Mark S. and Beckwith, P. (1999). Role of mucus trails and trail-following in the behavior and nutrition of the periwinkle Littorina littorea. Marine Ecology Progress Series 179: 247-257. Department of Fisheries and Oceans (DFO), Government of Canada (1998). Periwinkles (Littorina littorea), DFO Science Stock Status Report C3-46. Erlandsson, J. and Kostylev, V. (1995). Trail following, speed and fractal dimension of movement in a marine prosobranch, Littorina littorea, during a mating and nonmating season. Marine Biology 122: 87-94. Gendron, Robert P. (1977). Habitat Selection and Migratory Behaviour of the Intertidal Gastropod Littorina littorea (L.). Journal of Animal Ecology 46: 79-92. Grosholz, Edwin (2002). Ecological and evolutionary consequences of coastal invasions. TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution 17(1): 22-27. Jackson, A., 2005. Littorina littorea. Common periwinkle. Marine Life Information Network: Biology and Sensitivity Key Information Sub-programme [on-line]. Plymouth: Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom. [cited 25/08/2005]. Available from: http://www.marlin.ac.uk/species/Littorinalittorea.htm Petraitis, Peter S. (1982). Occurrence of random and directional movements in the periwinkle, Littorina littorea (L.). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 59: 207-217.