* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Statistical and Psychological Factors Affecting Clinical Decisions
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
Statistical and Psychological Factors Affecting Clinical Decisions By: Afshan Mirza Mentors: Mr. Jeffrey Madura Dr. Keith Carroll April 2, 2007 -------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------5/20/2007 Hi Mr. Madura, So I went to the Scholars Banquet last night and got a very pleasant surprise and thought I'd share it with you... Everyone who graduated as a Scholar with Distinction was presented with plaques. When it was my turn, Dr. Mikula announced that I had the highest IRP grade ever! EVER!! Wow...it was so amazing! My whole family is so proud of me and I feel a great sense of achievement too. If it wasn't for your help, I would've quit the program a long time ago and would never have discovered this hidden potential. So thank you very much once again. Have a great summer! Afshan Mirza Table of Contents Introduction…………………………………………………………………………... Prevalence of Misdiagnoses………………………………………………………….. Misdiagnosis Due to Inadequate Understanding of Statistics The Problem……………………………………………………………………….. The Solution……………………………………………………………………….. Misdiagnosis Due to Psychological Barriers The Problem……………………………………………………………………….. The Solution……………………………………………………………………….. Conclusion…………………………………………………………………………….. Bibliography………………………………………….……………………………….. 2 Pg. 3 Pg. 4 Pg. 4 Pg. 8 Pg. 12 Pg. 18 Pg. 24 Pg. 26 Medical Misdiagnosis Introduction A person faces choices from the moment of waking to the moment of falling asleep. Sometimes these choices are insignificant, like what color socks to wear; but at other times, they can mean the difference between life and death, like whether to drink and drive or to take a cab instead. All individuals tackle choices in their personal and professional lives. Personal choices often have an impact only on the person who makes them, whereas professional choices usually affect others. Among the most important decisions made by professionals are those made by doctors. Physicians make choices in the testing, diagnosis, and treatment of patients many times a day. These decisions, when made well, can save a patient’s life or significantly reduce suffering. On the other hand, when made poorly they can cause extreme stress, pain, new illnesses, and even death. In order to reduce the possibility of these negative outcomes, doctors need to understand probability and statistics in medical testing and be able to interpret test results correctly. The required knowledge in this area can be provided through continuing education and application of Bayes’ Theorem. The use of computer programs designed to help with diagnosis and interpretation of test results will also assist doctors. In addition to knowledge deficiencies, doctors often must overcome psychological hurdles that stand in the way of good decisions. The psychological pitfalls in decision-making under uncertainty need to be learned and taken into consideration in order to reduce occurrences of misdiagnosis and mistreatment in medicine. Currently, eighty percent of medical errors are the result of predictable mental traps or cognitive errors (Gorman). Being aware of one’s own mindset and ability to handle risky situations is the key to better medical decisions. Medical 3 school curricula should include programs to teach students the risks of cognitive errors. In addition, doctors should use a formal decision-making approach to ensure their personal weaknesses do not get in the way of their professional decisions. Education, awareness, and action are necessary steps towards better medical care and safer treatment for all. Prevalence of Misdiagnoses Misdiagnoses are an unavoidable part of medicine. Any process that involves humans is bound towards imperfection. The problem lies not only in the presence of misdiagnoses, but more so in the prevalence of them. Each misdiagnosis may result in an increase of a patient’s pain, an increase in a patient’s medical bills, or even the patient’s death. According to a study done by the Institute of Medicine, close to 100,000 Americans die annually due to medical errors. Furthermore, at least 1.5 million people per year are harmed by medical errors, at an annual cost of around $3.5 billion (Kalb). Autopsy studies show that doctors seriously misdiagnose fatal illnesses approximately 20 percent of the time. Astonishingly, there has been no improvement in this rate since the 1930’s, according to an article published in the Journal of the American Medical Association (Leonhardt). The problem of misdiagnosis affects millions of people and their families each year. This is an issue that needs to be addressed immediately. But before something can be improved, it is essential to understand the causes of it. Misdiagnosis Due to Inadequate Understanding of Statistics The Problem Doctors almost always rely on medical testing to diagnose a patient’s condition. From strep throat to leukemia, doctors use tests to determine the cause of the patient’s suffering. The cause of misdiagnosis, however, is not testing; it is the incorrect interpretation of the test results 4 by inadequately-trained doctors. Every medical test has two main characteristics that must be understood in order to accurately analyze the test results. The first is the sensitivity of the test and the second is the specificity. Both of these factors need to be taken into account before reaching a diagnosis based on test results. Sensitivity describes how good a test is at correctly identifying people who have the disease. It is calculated by dividing the number of true positives (the number of people who tested positive for the disease and did in fact have the disease) by the total number of people who were sick (Loong). Consider a population of 100 people, 40 of whom are sick with a certain illness. Example 1: Sick Well Total Positive Test 30 30 60 Negative Test 10 30 40 Total 40 60 100 In this example, the sensitivity would be 30/40, or 75%. Although it may seem like a test with 100% sensitivity would be ideal, this is not the case. A type of test with 100% sensitivity could be a test that is designed to always produce a positive result (Loong). A test like this might be meaningless due to an excessively high number of false positives, and would not help doctors make wise decisions. Medical tests should, however, aim to have a reasonably high sensitivity because it reduces the chances of false negatives. False negatives are a very serious problem. Patients are always hopeful of walking out of the doctor’s office or the hospital with the assurance that they are perfectly healthy. A false 5 negative gives them this artificial consolation and if they are sick, their disease goes undetected for a longer period of time. Depending on the disease, the time between the false negative and actual detection of the disease’s presence could prove to be fatal. Whether it is fatal or not, the growth of the disease during this time is inevitable, making it more difficult and perhaps more painful to treat. False negatives have greater chances of being discovered as such by taking the test again. But most people do not think of the possibility that although the test may have indicated that they are disease-free, they may still have the disease and the test could be wrong. Therefore, many people do not ask to be retested in the case of a false negative. For this reason and many others, medical tests should be designed to have a high sensitivity to decrease the number of false negatives. The second characteristic of a medical test is specificity. This describes how good the test is at correctly identifying people who are well (Loong). It is calculated by dividing the number of true negatives (people who tested negative for a disease and were truly well) from a test by the total number of people tested who were well. In the above example, thirty well people tested negative and the total number of well people was sixty. The specificity of the test is therefore 30/60, or 50%. Tests with higher specificities are better because they reduce the chances of producing a false positive (healthy people who test positive for a disease). False positives, like false negatives, also pose a threat to patient safety. If a patient is inaccurately diagnosed as having the disease, then there is a strong possibility that a wrong treatment will be given to him/her. Improper treatment could pose a danger to the patient’s life, as in the case of a four year old boy from Georgia. This child was diagnosed with leukemia and chemotherapy was scheduled to start for him in a few days. What the doctors did not realize, however, was that the little boy had a rare form of the disease, which chemotherapy did not cure. 6 Furthermore, each round of chemotherapy had a high risk of killing him since he was so weak already (Leonhardt). Although dangerous, false positives are often less of a threat than false negatives. Because no one likes to be diagnosed with a disease, false positives have a greater chance of being identified by means of retesting and second opinions. On the other hand, the rate of their discovery is not high enough to undermine the need of reducing their occurrences. The aim to achieve higher specificity in tests should still be a top priority to decrease the number of false positives. The real goal for those who develop medical tests should be both high sensitivity and high specificity. Sensitivity and specificity are important concepts to understand and properly utilize in order to make wise medical decisions based on testing. Many doctors, however, do not fully understand these ideas. Sixty doctors from Harvard Medical School teaching hospitals were given data about a test and asked to calculate what the chances were that a patient who tested positive did actually have the disease. They were told that the prevalence of the disease was 1 in 1000, the sensitivity of the test was 100%, and that the specificity of the test was 95%. Here is a table in the same format as the previous example to illustrate the data (the large numbers are needed to avoid fractional people): Example 2: Sick Well Total Positive Test 100 4,995 5,095 Negative Test 0 94,905 94,905 Total 100 99,900 100,000 7 Only 18% of the doctors answered correctly that a patient with a positive test result had less than a 2% (100/5,095 = .019627 = 1.96%) chance of having the disease. The low prevalence was the key factor in this scenario. Because the prevalence was so low, there were bound to be more false positives (4,995) than true positives (100) (Pradhan). Eighty-two percent of the doctors from a prestigious medical school known for its high standards of education were unable to analyze the situation correctly. This number would probably be higher if this test was given to students of other schools. Another statistical error that occurs in medical decision making is conjunction fallacy. Conjunction fallacy occurs when a person concludes that the conjunction of two events is more likely than one of the events alone (Elstein). This cannot be true because in probability theory, the probability of the intersection of two events cannot be greater than the probability of one of the events alone. This faulty thinking can lead to a diagnosis that is more threatening than the true condition of the patient therefore putting the patient at risk of overtreatment. The Solution The best and simplest way of reducing the number of misdiagnoses due to an inadequate understanding of statistics is by educating doctors in these matters. Providing physicians with the correct information on the significance of different statistical measures, especially sensitivity and specificity, and their correct applications will give them the resources they need to make intelligent decisions. Education on Bayes’ Theorem will be one of the keys to lowering misdiagnosis rates and moving towards safer healthcare. Bayes’ Theorem is an easy way of calculating a conditional probability based on the inverse conditional probability, which is known. For example, it is possible to calculate the probability that the patient has the disease given that the test is positive from knowledge of the 8 probability that the test is positive for a patient who has the disease. In addition to this, two more pieces of information are needed for the calculation: the probability that a person has the disease and the probability of a false positive test result. The equation is as follows: P = the event that the test gives a positive result D = the event that the patient has the disease W = (well) the event that the patient does not have the disease P(W) = 1 – P(D) Then P(D|P) = P(P|D)P(D) . P(P|D)P(D)+P(P|W)P(W) The following is a sample application of the Bayes’ Theorem: Problem: A test is known to detect a disease in a patient correctly 95% of the time (sensitivity = 95%). However, 15% of all disease-free patients who take this test also test positive (specificity = 85%). 10% of the population has the disease. Patient X has taken this test and has tested positive. What is the probability that he does in fact have the disease? Solution: P(P|D) = 0.95 (sensitivity) P(P|W) = 0.15 (1 – specificity) P(D) = 0.10 (prevalence) P(W) = 0.90 (1 – prevalence) Using Bayes’ Theorem, P(D|P) can now be calculated using the probabilities above. P(D|P) = (0.95)(0.10) . (0.95)(0.10)+(0.15)(0.90) P(D|P) = 0.095 0.23 9 P(D|P) = 0.4130 The probability that patient X does in fact have the disease, given that his test was positive, is only 41.30% (Waner). This table reflects the facts above: Example 3: Sick Well Total Positive Test 95 135 230 Negative Test 5 765 770 Total 100 900 1000 Of the 230 patients with positive test results, only 95, or 41.30%, are actually sick. Here is Bayes’ formula using the terms sensitivity, specificity, and prevalence: P (D|P) = (sensitivity)(prevalence) . (sensitivity)(prevalence) + (1 – specificity)(1 – prevalence) Bayes’ Theorem is a very important concept to be familiar with when dealing with any kind of testing, but especially when it comes to medical testing. If doctors were to assume that patient X had the disease without realizing that there was a 58.70% chance that he did not have the disease, then his health could have faced serious risks. Education in school and training in the workplace are the starting points of a better future for healthcare. Another tool, aside from education, that will help in reducing the number of medical misdiagnoses is computer software designed for the purpose of aiding doctors with making wise medical decisions. These computer programs are commonly referred to as clinical decision support systems. They are defined as “active knowledge systems which use two or more items of patient data to generate case-specific advice” (“Clinical Decision Support Systems”). These 10 systems can make a doctor’s decision-making process easier and more reliable in stressful or complicated situations. There are four main functions of clinical decision support systems. The first is administrative. The systems support clinical coding and documentation. In addition, they authorize procedures and referrals. The second function is to manage clinical details and complexity. As a part of this function, systems keep track of referrals, follow-up, and preventive care of patients. The third function is cost control. Here, the systems are used to monitor medication orders and avoid unnecessary or duplicate testing. The fourth function of a clinical decision support system is decision support itself. This is perhaps the most important function of a decision support system. It supports the processes of clinical diagnosis and treatment plans. Furthermore, it promotes the use of “best practices, condition-specific guidelines, and population-based management” (“Clinical Decision Support Systems”). The use of clinical decision support systems should be encouraged by hospitals and demanded by patients. Doctors need these systems to either help them make their decisions or to reconfirm them. Since doctors are expected to memorize and remember so much information, the chances are high that they may overlook essential information at the time when it is needed the most. Statistical analysis formulas, such as Bayes’ Theorem, take time to work out and are sometimes difficult to remember. Computer programs make it more convenient and easier to apply these and other sophisticated case-management tools. A study was done to test the significance of clinical decision support systems. In this study, the human and computer-aided diagnoses of 304 patients suffering from abdominal pain were compared. The most senior member of the clinical team achieved a 79.6% accuracy, while the computing system scored a diagnostic accuracy of 91.8% (“Clinical Decision Support 11 Systems”). Not only is this an impressive percentage, but it is also an enormous improvement from the human performance level. This study successfully proved the value of computer-aided diagnoses in the medical sector. Bayes’ Theorem and computer programs designed to assist in medical decision making are essential in order to reduce the number of misdiagnoses due to incorrect statistical analysis. Using these two simple methods, doctors can provide their patients with a safer healthcare environment and a more secure feeling towards their medical concerns. Misdiagnosis Due to Psychological Barriers The Problem Incorrect statistical analysis is not the only factor contributing to the high rates of misdiagnosis. Psychological barriers also play a large role in its prevalence. Unlike statistical errors, psychological blunders are more difficult to recognize and avoid. Technical errors are more commonly discussed while thinking errors are largely ignored (Gorman). A few simple steps, however, can ensure the path to a wise decision. But before a solution can be discussed, the problem has to be identified. This is often the most challenging task of the process. Yet, at the same time, it is often the most important. Doctors, and people in general, find it difficult to recognize the flaws in their decisions because they assume that they are using the rational model of decision-making. This, however, is rarely the case (Plochg). Most people do not use this model and by erroneously assuming that they do, they are greatly reducing their chances of exploring the formal methods of decision making. Without any formal methods, doctors are left to their own individualized approaches to decision making. Decisions and diagnoses made under such circumstances have a high risk of being distorted by the doctor’s personal emotions, drives, and preferences (Plochg). These 12 distortions are usually subconscious phenomena; most doctors do not even realize that they are occurring. Some of the psychological impediments to good decision making will be discussed here. Tunnel vision occurs when people have mental blinders that limit them to only consider a narrow range of alternatives (Huitt). In a clinical environment, this could mean a doctor limiting the possibilities of a patient’s diagnosis. For example, if a patient walks into the doctor’s clinic complaining of a fever and muscle aches during January, the doctor, who has been seeing a lot of flu patients lately, may unconsciously start limiting his diagnosis options to a cold or the flu. But in fact, this patient could have a wide range of problems or diseases. The patient’s symptoms could be an indication of anything from meningitis to a reaction to a recently taken tetanus shot (Gorman). The doctor’s limited vision causes him to ignore the less obvious alternatives of diagnosis. Bounded rationality is the idea that people attempt to be rational, but their rationality is “constrained by their own values and experiences and by unconscious reflexes, skills and habits” (Plochg). For example, if a doctor has the habit of using a specific medical test when attempting to diagnose a blocked artery in a patient, then she may not consider other methods of diagnosis for new patients. Other methods may be less invasive or more convenient, but because she is in the habit of using that particular test, she will most likely continue using it for her future patients as well. This could cause a problem for patients who prefer that their medical treatment be personalized according to their needs and preferences, as opposed to the doctor’s. Previous commitments can also interfere with medical decision making and diagnosis. Previous commitments come into play when people feel personally responsible for a poor decision made in the past, their ability to evaluate the possible alternatives in a current situation 13 becomes distorted (Plochg). This is especially relevant with doctors. Since doctors make decisions that can mean the difference between life and death for some patients, an incorrect decision leading to the patient’s death or severe harm can push doctors into harshly blaming themselves for the consequences of their decisions. To some extent, this is a healthy way of committing to do better next time, but some doctors become so engrossed with the feeling of guilt that they are unable to function properly in future decisions. They are so wrapped up in the past decisions, that they find it difficult to “objectively evaluate other alternatives” of the situation at hand (Plochg). This is a pitfall that doctors should avoid as much as possible. Another psychological barrier is anchoring. This is when a person chooses an alternative at the very beginning of the decision-making process and continues to evaluate the other alternatives, but with a bias towards the initial selection. The subsequent alternatives are therefore distorted perceptually according to the first choice (Plochg). Furthermore, since the doctor has become psychologically committed to the initial hypothesis, she will retain it even in light of new and relevant data (Elstein). Anchoring is a behavior found in novices as well as senior clinicians and needs to be guarded against carefully (Sutherland). For example, if a doctor initially feels inclined to utilize a certain method of diagnosing lung cancer in a patient, then she will continue to analyze other methods of diagnosis, but through the lens of the first method. According to her perception, the other alternatives may be too expensive, too time-consuming, or too simplistic compared to her first inclination. It is not necessary that the first picked alternative should be the best, but when the doctor is a victim of anchoring, the first alternative will seem like the best alternative. This, of course, is a substantial concern for patients since they want the best test or the best treatment, rather than the test or treatment that their doctor first thought of. 14 Lack of creativity is also a barrier to effective medical decisions. Creativity is the ability to generate ideas that are both innovative and functional (Plochg). The lack of it limits the potential of doctors and places constraints on the possibilities of medical testing, diagnosis, and treatment options. Doctors need to be creative in order to develop new ways of testing that are less invasive and more accurate. They need to be creative in finding new methods of treatment that minimize side effects and maximize healing. Creativity is an essential characteristic for doctors and is a fundamental factor for advancement in healthcare. Another cognitive error made in medical decision-making is that the frequency of events that can be easily recalled tends to be overestimated, while the frequency of events that are ordinary or difficult to remember is usually underestimated (Elstein and Summerton). Diseases that receive media attention or are supported by famous celebrities are often thought of as occurring more frequently than they do. More mundane conditions, such as throat infections, however, are considered to occur less frequently than they actually do. This psychological mindset causes an overemphasis of rare conditions in the medical setting since unusual cases remain in the memory longer (Elstein). According to Tversky and Kahneman’s Prospect Theory, small probabilities are overweighted while large probabilities are underweighted (Elstein). This provides an explanation for why “the difference between 99% and 100% is psychologically much greater than the difference between, say, 60% and 61%” (Elstein). This distortion of the probability scale could play a very important role in the patient’s decision for treatment. For instance, if a patient is recommended for major surgery and is told that there is a 99% to 100% chance of success, the patient may worry about that uncertain 1% much more than if the chances for success were 60%-61%. 15 Support theory suggests that people assign higher probabilities to events that are described in greater detail than the same events when described with fewer details (Elstein). From a clinical perspective, this predicts that “a longer, more detailed case description will be assigned a higher subjective probability of the index disease than a brief abstract of the same case, even if they contain the same information about that disease” (Elstein). Factors such as description and length of the case should not matter as long as the information is the same. The fact that they do matter, however, provides an insight into the psychological factors that affect medical decisions. The term framing refers to the manner in which information is presented. The way information is framed has a significant impact on the medical decisions of patients in reference to treatment and course of action (Edwards and Cox). For example, framing a situation in terms of loss causes patients to opt for medical screenings more often than when the same situation is framed in positive terms (Edwards). Another example is when the “probability of having a child with Down’s syndrome was framed negatively – as a 20% risk of an affected child – women were more likely to have an amniocentesis than if the risk was framed positively – an 80% risk of no abnormality” (Sedgwick). Furthermore, presenting data in more understandable terms for the patient and providing greater detail is associated with a greater wariness towards participating in trials and taking medication (Edwards). Patients are the final decision makers of what treatment they would like or whether they would even like to be treated or not. Therefore, it is essential for doctors to understand the effects of framing and to be able to communicate risk and other important information properly. A psychological activity leading to misdiagnosis and mistreatment that patients may take part in is called telescoping. This is when patients have the “tendency to combine separate, 16 similar symptoms into a single generic event” (Summerton). Since each of the separate symptoms may have provided essential clues to the correct diagnosis, telescoping does prove to be harmful and counter-productive. The psychological state of patients has an immense impact on how they report symptoms to their doctors. For example, women who are tying to become pregnant are more likely to describe symptoms of morning sickness and amenorrhea than women who do not want to become pregnant (Summerton). The cognitive unwillingness to accept symptoms of a possible unwanted condition leads to underreporting, which may cause the true source of the patient’s physical condition to go undetected. Furthermore, the patient’s psychological state along with his/her sex, age and other such characteristics can lead doctors to false assumptions and incorrect diagnosis (Gorman). This was clearly demonstrated in the case of an eight year old girl who went to the doctor complaining of severe headaches. At the time, the patient’s parents were adjusting to new, high-stress jobs and top neurologists and pediatricians concluded that her symptoms were probably caused by high stress levels at home and perhaps a sinus condition. A few months later, it was discovered that she had a brain tumor. The doctors looked at scans of her brain taken earlier and were shocked to see that there was a shadow of a tumor and they had completely missed seeing it (Gorman). The same type of situation may arise if a homeless man complains of disorientation. The doctors may feel inclined to attribute this symptom to alcoholism although the true cause may be due to diabetes (Gorman). A doctor’s emotional feelings towards the patient can also be the cause of cognitive errors and misdiagnosis. Doctors who love their patients are known to miss diagnoses of lifethreatening cancers in their patients because they do not want it to be true. On the other hand, 17 patients who are disliked by their doctors are equally disadvantaged. Doctors tend to hesitate to go the extra mile for patients they are not particularly fond of (Gorman). Another psychological factor in the proper diagnosis and treatment of patients is a stigma of psychological illness. Doctors and patients alike go to extensive measures to find a physical cause for the patient’s problems rather than accepting that a psychological factor might be at play. In cases like this, there is a “risk that patients receive extensive investigations that are of limited value and potentially damaging both physically and psychologically, irrespective of the additional healthcare costs incurred” (Summerton). The fear of psychological illness and the stigma attached to it leaves the patient to have unnecessary physical testing and an untreated psychological condition. The psychological barriers to wise medical diagnosis need to be taken seriously. There is no doubt that they do exist and do impact medical decisions significantly. All doctors and patients should be made aware of the potential barriers they could face when making medical decisions and they should be encouraged to gain a better understanding of them and to avoid them as much as possible. The Solution The first step doctors can take to reduce medical misdiagnoses due to psychological barriers is to accept and recognize the fact that these factors are present in their work environment. It becomes very difficult to work towards a solution without the acceptance and recognition of the problem. Educational institutions and doctors need to work together to create awareness of these barriers and to encourage action towards eliminating or reducing them. Since framing plays an essential role in medical decision making, it is extremely important that medical students learn how to properly communicate risk to patients. This topic 18 has received very little attention over the past years in undergraduate medical curriculum (Sedgwick). Using quantitative evidence is key in communicating effectively with patients since qualitative expressions can be interpreted in various ways by each individual. Lessons like these need to be taught to students early in their medical education so that they have the opportunity to practice this knowledge throughout their education and in their careers. Additionally, a formal decision-making model should be used in order to minimize subjectivity due to the doctor’s personal beliefs, habits, and values. There are several different decision-making models that a doctor can choose from, but they all share similar steps. The seven most common and the most helpful decision-making steps for the medical field will be discussed here. They are: identifying the decision to be made, self-assessment, identifying options, gathering information and data, evaluating options, selecting an option, and designing a course of action to implement the decision. The first step to making a good medical decision is identifying the decision being made (Roberts). The doctor must clearly understand what he is trying to decide. For example, he should not attempt to decide which treatment option to use for the patient before he decides what ailment the patient has. Being able to properly identify an appropriate decision at the beginning of the decision-making process has numerous benefits. Firstly, it brings out any biases that the doctor may have about the patient’s condition. In the above example, it would make it clear to the doctor that he has already set his mind on a certain diagnosis without going through the proper decision-making process. Secondly, becoming aware of the goals early in the process provides direction for the work to come. Once the objective is set, the means to achieving it can be worked out. Step one is the foundation of the decision-making model. 19 The next step is self-assessment (Roberts). In order to make a good decision, doctors need to be fully aware of their abilities and also their potential biases or favoritisms. Doctors should evaluate their knowledge and understanding of the patient’s symptoms and objectively decide if they are the correct person to handle the case. In addition, they must also examine their psychological standing. They should ask themselves if they have any psychological barriers that could keep them from making the correct decision in the patient’s best interest. Some doctors may see this step as unimportant or inconsequential, but this aspect of self-assessment is crucial because it has the potential of identifying psychological barriers and eliminating them through their awareness. Doctors should be mindful to be fully objective and honest in their analysis while performing self-assessment in order for it to be successful. Step three in the decision-making process is identifying the possible options (Roberts). Before making any decision, it is important to first consider the various alternatives. If a patient complains of certain symptoms, the doctor should think about all the potential diagnoses that could be indicated by these symptoms. Computer programs designed to generate a list of possible diseases based on the symptoms provided by the doctors are a way of making sure that no possibilities are overlooked. By generating this list, doctors can reduce the chances of psychological barriers such as implicit favorites, bounded rationality, and tunnel vision affecting their medical decisions. In addition, it narrows down the list of diseases to only the ones that are relevant to the specific case. Since this eliminates the insignificant information, the task at hand becomes more focused and easier to handle. Once the possible options have been identified, it is time to gather information and data (Roberts). It is during this step that all the research, medical tests, and examining will be done. Since the possible options for diagnosis have already been brought to attention, the type of 20 information needed is also automatically narrowed down. If any of the possible diagnoses need medical testing, then data should be gathered on the sensitivity and specificity of the test. Other data on the disease itself should be gathered, such as its prevalence. Furthermore, medical tests need to be given to the patients and their results need to be submitted to the doctors as soon as possible. After the results are submitted, doctors can move on to the fifth step of decision making, which is evaluating the options (Roberts). Evaluating the options is an essential part of the decision-making process. Once all the data has been gathered and the medical reports are back from testing, doctors must use this information to critically assess the validity of each test result. For example, if the patient is given a test for diagnosing leukemia and tests positive for it while the doctor’s research finds that this test has high sensitivity and low specificity, then the doctor should realize that this patient has a high chance of being a false positive. Knowing this, the doctor can order the same test to be given again, or better yet, another more reliable test. This step also narrows down the options for diagnosis. For example, a simple evaluation of a blood test can eliminate possible diagnoses. Additionally, all options should be reexamined in light of the new data gathered in the previous step. No piece of information should be disregarded as insignificant until it has been given proper attention. Evaluating the options is a key factor to making wise medical decisions. The sixth step for decision making is selecting one of the options (Roberts). This is perhaps the most difficult and certainly the most important step in the whole decision-making process. This is the step that all the other steps were designed to work towards. And this is the step that will direct the last phase of the process. After all the information has been evaluated and all the test or retest results have been taken into consideration, the best option has to be picked. The best option should not be chosen in a hurry. Doctors should give the decision a 21 sufficient amount of time to analyze it from all different perspectives. In addition, doctors should discuss the reasons for picking this option with the patient and his/her family. It is important that the patient should not be left in the dark about any side effects or risks of the treatment. This will help avoid lawsuits as well as reduce the patient’s anxiety and make him/her more confident about the doctor’s knowledge and ability. Furthermore, the patient will feel more secure knowing that the doctor has used a logical and objective approach to reaching his medical diagnosis as opposed to a less structured method. The last step of the decision-making process is to “design a course of action to implement the decision” (Roberts). Even after the decision has been made, the plan of implementation is still unclear at times. For example, if the treatment option chosen for a patient is chemotherapy and the facility where this decision was made does not have the proper equipment for it, then issues such as where to send the patient for treatment need to be worked out. Additionally, in the case of this example, other details like what the course of chemotherapy should be, how often it should be administered, and should the patient observe any dietary regulations before a chemotherapy session also need to be discussed and planned. Once all of these specifics have been decided, the decision-making process is complete. The only remaining task is to put the decisions in action. Aside from using a formal decision-making technique, different educational programs can also help reduce the problem of misdiagnosis due to psychological errors. Problem-based learning is a way of improving medical education in this area. This method can be understood as “an effort to introduce the formulation and testing of clinical hypotheses into the preclinical curriculum” (Elstein). Experienced physicians already use this approach when they are faced 22 with difficult cases. It would therefore be logical to begin training students in this field since even a mediocre case may seem difficult to them at the beginning of their careers (Elstein). Evidence-based medicine should also be taught to students aspiring to become doctors. Evidence-based medicine is the most recent and the most successful effort yet to apply statistical decision theory in medicine. It teaches students Bayes’s theorem and shows them how to interpret diagnostic results and compute probabilities while understanding what they mean (Elstein). Combined with problem based learning, evidence based medicine has the potential to drastically reduce the number of misdiagnosis and mistreatment cases due to statistical errors and psychological barriers, and increase the awareness of the essential role statistics play in medicine. HIT (health information technology) is one of the major proposed solutions to medical errors. It is “the intersection of information science, medicine, and health care. It deals with the resources, devices, and methods required to optimize the acquisition, storage, retrieval, and use of information in health and medicine. Health informatics tools include computers and information technology as well as clinical guidelines, medical research, science, and engineering. A major focus is the support of information systems for reasoning, decision-making, and learning in a clinical setting” (Grabowski). It has the “potential to make a highly significant contribution to the advancement of medicine and the improvement of healthcare quality and reduction of medical errors” (Zhang). Many medical mistakes are caused by human errors that occur mainly because of “inadequate information processing in cognitive tasks” (Zhang). HIT comes as a solution to this problem. If 23 the health information technology is designed specifically around the environment in which it is meant to be used, then it can simplify things and be a great help to doctors and a blessing for their patients. Several colleges and universities are now taking the initiative to offer programs designed around HIT. University of Illinois at Chicago (UIC), for example, is offering a Masters of Science degree in Health Informatics and Northwestern University is doing the same in Medical Informatics. Benedictine University is currently working on a proposal to start a Master of Science degree in Health Informatics as well. Conclusion Misdiagnosis and mistreatment are critical problems in the healthcare industry. They need to be addressed immediately and measures have to be taken to reduce their high prevalence. Understanding what is causing the misdiagnosis and mistreatment rates to be so high is the beginning of the solution. The two main factors allowing this crisis to go on are doctors’ inability to analyze statistics accurately and the presence of psychological barriers in the minds of doctors and patients. There are simple solutions to both these obstacles. Educational emphasis and the use of Bayes’ Theorem will increase doctors’ ability to accurately analyze statistics while a formal decision-making model, educational programs, and the use of HIT will greatly reduce the chances of their psychological biases distorting proper diagnosis and treatment decisions. The solutions are present and their implementations are not demanding. Perhaps the only remaining hurdle standing in the way of safer healthcare is the lack of awareness about the severity of this problem. It is the responsibility of all patients and doctors to educate themselves about the risks of misdiagnosis and mistreatment. It is the responsibility of every hospital to encourage risk-reducing measures and to educate its staff on this topic. Soaring rates of 24 misdiagnoses and mistreatment are not problems without solutions. But the solution lies in the willingness to take action and to demand safer healthcare for all. 25 Bibliography "Clinical Decision Support Systems." Open Clinical. 2001. 1 November 2006. <http://www.openclinical.org/dss.html>. Cox, Ken. "Daniel's Message for Doctors." Medical Journal of Australia 178 (2003): 510-511. Edwards, Adrian, Judith Covey, Elaine Matthews, and Rolsin Pill. "Presenting Risk Information: a Review of the Effects of Framing and Other Manipulations on Patient Outcomes." Journal of Health Communication 6.1 (2001): 61-82. Elstein, Arthur S., and Alan Schwarz. "Clinical Problem Solving and Diagnostic Decision Making: Selective Review of the Cognitive Literature." BMJ 324 (2002): 729-732. Gorman, Christine. "Where Doctors Go Wrong." Time 15 Mar. 2007. 21 Mar. 2007 <http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,1599718,00.html>. Grabowski, B. T. Proposal for Master of Science Degree in Health Informatics. Benedictine University. Lisle, 2007. Huitt, W. (1992). Problem solving and decision making: Consideration of individual differences using the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Journal of Psychological Type, 24, 33-44. Kalb, Claudia. “Fixing America’s Hospitals.” Newsweek 148.16 (2006): 44-46. Academic Search Premier. 1 November 2006. <http://search.ebscohost.com>. Leonhardt, David. “Why Doctors So Often Get It Wrong.” New York Times 155.53498 (2006): C1-C4. Academic Search Premier. 1 November 2006. <http://search.ebscohost.com>. Loong, Tze-Wey. "Understanding Sensitivity and Specificity with the Right Side of the Brain." British Medical Journal 327 (2003). 1 November 2006. <http://www.bmj.com/cgi/content/full/327/7417/716>. 26 McNutt, Robert A. "Shared Medical Decision Making." Journal of the American Medical Association (2004). 11 November 2006. <http://jama.ama-assn.org/cgi/content/full/292/20/2516>. Plochg, Ivo. "Psychological Barriers to Effective Decision Making." Business Psychology Consulting. 1 November 2006. <http://www.businesspsychologistconsulting.com/article%20psychological%20barriers% 20to%20effective%20decision%20making.html>. Pradhan, Malcolm. "Analyzing the Art of Medicine." Australian Health Informatics (1993). Roberts, Rick. "7-Step Decision-Making Model." 1 Nov. 2006. <http://www.unf.edu/dept/cdc/services/decisio.htm>. Sedgwick, Philip, and Angela Hall. "Teaching Medical Students and Doctors How to Communicate Risk." BMJ 327 (2003): 694-695. Summerton, Nick. "Making a Diagnosis in Primary Care: Symptoms and Context." British Journal of General Practice 54 (2004): 570-571. Sutherland, D. C. "Improving Medical Diagnoses by Understanding How They are Made." Internal Medicine Journal 32 (2002): 277. Waner, Stefan, and Steven R. Costenoble, comps. Bayes' Theorem. April 1997. 1 November 2006. <http://people.hofstra.edu/faculty/stefan_waner/realworld/tutorialsf3/unit6_6.html>. Zhang, Jiajie, Vimla L. Patel, James P. Turley, and Todd R. Johnson. Health Informatics and Medical Errors. School of Health Information Sciences, University of Texas Health Science Center At Houston, and Department of Biomedical Informatics, Columbia University. 2005. 2-5. 27