* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download shure ec4
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
J Am Acad Audiol 22:663–677 (2011) Teenage Use of Portable Listening Devices: A Hazard to Hearing? DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.22.10.5 Cory D.F. Portnuff* Brian J. Fligor† Kathryn H. Arehart* Abstract Background: Recently, a number of popular media articles have raised some concern that portable listening devices (PLDs) may be increasing the risk for music-induced hearing loss (MIHL). However, literature regarding adolescents’ listening behavior and how their attitudes and beliefs relate to behavior is currently limited. Purpose: The purposes of this study were (1) to investigate the relationship between volume control settings and output levels of PLDs, (2) to examine how adolescents’ listening behavior changes as a function of background noise and noise isolation, (3) to investigate the relationship between self-reported listening levels and laboratory-measured listening levels, and (4) to evaluate the validity of the Listening Habits Questionnaire as a research tool for evaluating how attitudes and beliefs relate to PLD use behavior. Research Design: A descriptive study. Experiment 1 evaluated the output levels of a set of PLDs, and Experiment 2 characterized the listening behavior and attitudes toward PLD use of a group of adolescents. Study Sample: Twenty-nine adolescents aged 13–17 yr, with normal hearing, participated in Experiment 2. Data Collection and Analysis: Experiment 1 evaluated the output levels of a set of PLDs with stock and accessory earphones using an acoustic manikin. Experiment 2 included survey measures of listening behavior and attitudes as well as output levels measured using a probe microphone. Conclusions: The output levels of PLDs are capable of reaching levels that could increase the risk for MIHL, and 14% of teenagers in this study reported behavior that puts them at increased risk for hearing loss. However, measured listening levels in the laboratory settings did not correlate well with self-reported typical listening levels. Further, the Listening Habits Questionnaire described in this study may provide a useful research tool for examining the relationship between attitudes and beliefs and listening behavior. Key Words: adolescents, Health Belief Model, iPods, MP3 players, personal listening devices Abbreviations: CLL 5 chosen listening level; DRC 5 damage-risk criteria; EAECL 5 estimated ambient ear canal noise level; HBM 5 Health Belief Model; LHQ 5 Listening Habits Questionnaire; MIHL 5 music-induced hearing loss; MIRE 5 microphone-in-real-ear; NIOSH 5 National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health; OSHA 5 Occupational Safety and Health Administration; PLD 5 portable listening device; RMS 5 root mean square S everal recent research studies, as well as reports in the popular media, have raised concerns that the use of digital portable listening devices (PLDs) contributes to the development of hearing loss, particularly in young people (Williams, 2009; Keppler et al, 2010; Shargorodsky et al, 2010). However, the *University of Colorado at Boulder; †Children’s Hospital Boston, Harvard Medical School Cory D. F. Portnuff, Au.D., Ph.D., 409 UCB, Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences Department, University of Colorado at Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309-0409; Phone: 303-492-0067; Fax: 303-316-7061; E-mail: [email protected] Support for this work was provided by Children’s Hospital Boston, Department of Otolaryngology and Communication Enhancement, as well as the American Speech-Language Hearing Association’s Students Preparing for Academic and Research Careers Award. Parts of this work were presented at the Noise Induced Hearing Loss in Children Conference, Covington, KY, October 2006, and the National Hearing Conservation Association Conference, Atlanta, GA, February 2009. 663 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 22, Number 10, 2011 evidence linking hearing loss to the use of PLDs is sparse and thus makes it difficult to effectively evaluate the claims made by the popular media. The present study examines how self-reported PLD use of a group of adolescents relates to laboratory measures of listening behavior. These results are also analyzed in regard to hearing loss damage-risk criteria and used to identify factors that contribute to teenagers’ choice of listening levels. BACKGROUND W ith the exception of age-related hearing loss, noise-induced hearing loss (NIHL) is the most common form of acquired hearing impairment (National Institutes of Health, 1990). A significant body of research indicates that adults exposed to noise for extended duration are at a significant risk of hearing loss (Mencher et al, 1997; Ward et al, 2000). Damage-risk criteria (DRC) have been adopted by the National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH, 1998), the Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA, 1981), and the European Parliament and Council of the European Union (EPCEU) (2003) indicating the “acceptable” risk for noise-induced permanent threshold shift in adults exposed to occupational noise. For each DRC, the permissible exposure level or recommended exposure level is different (80, 85, or 90 dBA), and the time–intensity trading ratio is different (3 or 5 dBA). While each organization’s choice of formula for calculating risk has been justified for use on an adult population, it is unknown if these risk estimates are similar in an adolescent population. In each DRC, exposure can be expressed in terms of a daily noise dose (in percent), where 100% noise dose is equivalent to the maximum permissible or maximum recommended exposure; typically, this is the equivalent of 8 hr of exposure at a given criterion level (80, 85, or 90 dBA). Noise dose is a cumulative measure, and exposures from individual activities in a given day are added together to calculate a daily noise dose. Exposure to high levels of music has been of recent interest in popular media and peer-reviewed literature (Williams, 2009; Keppler et al, 2010; Shargorodsky et al, 2010). Listening to music as a leisure activity can also expose adolescents to high sound levels in a variety of environments, including urban music clubs, concert halls, and discotheques (Royster et al, 1991; Gunderson et al, 1997; Serra et al, 2005). Adolescents may also be exposed to high sound levels through the use of PLDs. Several studies have established that older PLD technology, including cassette tape players and compact disc players, are capable of producing high output levels that could increase the risk of music-induced hearing loss (MIHL [Airo et al, 1996; Fligor and Cox, 2004]). The current generation of digital PLDs is also capable of producing output levels that might increase the risk for MIHL if used for extended durations (Keith et al, 2008; Keppler et al, 2010). As with older technology, output levels of digital PLDs vary depending on the type of earphones used. For example, higher output levels can be produced by some aftermarket earphones than by stock earphones (Fligor and Cox, 2004). Keith et al (2008) reported that the maximum level from digital PLDs ranges from 83.4 to 107.3 dBA, depending on the earphone, the maximum PLD output voltage, and the recorded level of the music. If the actual fit of the earphones in the ear is considered (well fit vs. loosely fit), the range of maximum output expands further. Additionally, digital PLDs have the potential to be used for longer durations than older technologies, as digital PLDs have expanded music storage and battery life capabilities. Following occupational DRC, potential damage to hearing from PLD use will be dependent on both listeners’ actual chosen listening levels (CLLs) and durations of exposure. Just because PLDs may produce high maximum output levels does not mean that users of PLDs actually listen to music at these high output levels. CLLs are dependent, in part, on the background noise levels. In the presence of background noise, listeners increase their CLL proportionately to the levels of the background noise, seeking a desired signal-to-noise ratio (SNR [Fligor and Ives, 2006]). Earphones that isolate listeners from the ambient noise levels by either passive isolation or active noise cancellation could allow listeners to choose lower listening levels while retaining their desired music-to-noise ratio. Two different techniques can be used in the estimation of CLLs. First, listeners can be surveyed about their typical CLLs, using either a rating scale that is related to a PLD’s volume control or qualitative reports of loudness. Self-reported CLLs are useful for estimating real-world PLD usage, as listeners can report both the level at which they typically listen and the duration of their listening. However, the validity of listeners’ self-reports has not been established, and it is unclear whether self-reported CLLs are similar to actual CLLs. Second, listeners’ CLLs can be measured in the laboratory using either a microphone-in-real-ear (MIRE) technique (ISO 11904-1 [2002]) or a technique employing a manikin with an ear-simulating microphone (ISO 11904-2 [2004]). In either of these measurement techniques, care must be taken to ensure that recorded sound output levels can be compared against DRC. As current DRC assume a diffuse-field noise source, a diffuse-field equivalent transfer function must be applied to any measurements recorded in the ear canal using a diffuse-field inverse filter as described in ISO 11904-2 (2004). While laboratory measurements of CLL can be obtained with high levels of accuracy, it is often unclear how they relate to real-world music exposure. 664 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Teenage MP3 Player Use/Portnuff et al Self-reports of listening behavior have been used by several researchers to establish patterns of behavior. Across surveys of college students, the majority of listeners use PLDs around 2 hr per day, with 12% listening 3 to 5 hr per day and 4 to 5% of listeners listening greater than 5 hr per day (Ahmed et al, 2006; Torre, 2008; Danhauer et al, 2009). Surveys of CLL have asked people about their typical settings on the volume control of their PLDs. For example, Ahmed and colleagues (2006) asked college students to report their preferred setting by a percentage of the volume control. The average setting of this group was 60% of the maximum volume, with 14% of listeners reporting levels greater than 80% of maximum volume. Hoover and Krishnamurti (2010) asked participants to report their preferred volume settings by quartiles of the volume control. About half of the listeners reported using their players above 50% of maximum volume, and 23% of listeners reported listening between 75 and 100% of the maximum volume level. Danhauer et al (2009) used a 1–10 Likert scale to rate preferred volume control setting, finding that 21% listened at a “6” on the scale, 25% listened at a “7,” and 26% listened between “8” and “10.” Across all of these surveys, it is clear that a small but substantial group of PLD users choose high CLLs, and at least some of this group could be increasing their risk for acquiring MIHL. Another set of studies has attempted to evaluate listeners’ CLLs by measurement using objective techniques (e.g., the manikin or the MIRE technique), obtaining some mixed results. Williams (2005) measured the CLLs of adult PLD users passing through noisy public areas by placing participants’ earphones on a manikin and found a mean CLL of 86.1 dBA. When self-reported listening times were taken into account, the mean exposure was 79.8 dB LAeq,8h, and 25% of users exceeded an estimated 85 dB LAeq,8h. A followup study using the same methods found a significantly lower mean CLL in 2008 of 81.3 dBA, with 17% of listeners exceeding 85 dB LAeq,8h (Williams, 2009). Using similar methodology, though using a recording system instead of a manikin, Epstein, Marozeau, and Cleveland (2010) measured the outputs of iPod users in a subway, on a busy street, in a library, and in a student center. The authors found that out of the 64 users evaluated, none chose levels greater than 85 dBA and that the maximum recorded NIOSH noise dose was 10%. Using an MIRE technique, Fligor and Ives (2006) evaluated the CLLs of 100 graduate students ranging in age from 20 to 46 yr (mean: 23.8 yr). In this group, 6% of listeners had CLLs that exceeded 85 dBA in quiet. The researchers also measured CLLs in several levels of background noise, pink noise from 50 to 80 dBA, a restaurant background noise, and an airplane background noise. A linear relationship between CLLs and the level of background noise was identified for each earphone. The authors reported that significantly lower CLLs were chosen when the listeners used isolator earphones compared with earbuds or supra-aural earphones with little background noise attenuation.1 Another group of college-age students was evaluated by Hodgetts, Rieger, and Szarko (2007) using an MIRE technique. In this study, listeners had mean CLLs of 76.0 dBA in quiet, 83.7 dBA in a 70 dBA multitalker babble, and 85.4 dBA in 70–80 dBA street noise. Additionally, listeners chose higher output levels when using earbuds than when using supra-aural-style earphones, and the authors conclude that supra-aural earphones reduce the risk of MIHL when used in noise. However, the authors describe the earphones as a closed style that could provide active noise cancellation; these earphones also provide, by design, some passive attenuation of background noise (whether or not the active noisecanceling circuit is engaged). Some or all of the effect of earphone style may be due to background noise attenuation in the supra-aural style. Further, the authors present CLLs as measured by a probe microphone at the eardrum without applying a diffuse-field equivalent transfer function. Thus, the numbers reported cannot be compared with either occupational DRC or with other studies that report diffuse-field equivalent levels. A follow-up study by the same authors measured CLLs while exercising and in background noise, finding a significant increase in CLL both when exercising and when resting in the presence of background noise (Hodgetts et al, 2009). However, the CLLs were similarly not reported as diffuse-field equivalent levels, precluding a direct comparison with DRC. In order to gain a better understanding of how teenagers use PLDs, the present study consisted of two experiments designed to assess listeners’ self-reported and laboratory-measured CLLs. In the first experiment, a series of measurements of PLD output levels were taken using an acoustic manikin technique. These measures were designed to help understand the effect of various earphones on the output levels of a set of PLDs, providing data to relate a volume control setting to an actual output level in A-weighted, diffuse-field equivalent decibels. In the second experiment, CLLs were measured in the laboratory using an MIRE technique, with measurements in the presence of several different levels and types of background noise, using three different styles of earphones. Additionally, a Listening Habits Questionnaire (LHQ) was developed to allow adolescents to report their typical volume control levels, including what style of earphone they used. Using the data collected in Experiment 1, which provided a conversion factor for estimating the output level of a PLD from the volume control level, the participants’ self-reported volume control levels were converted to output levels in decibels. To gain insight into the psychosocial factors that influence adolescents’ CLLs, the LHQ included questions 665 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 22, Number 10, 2011 based on the Health Belief Model (HBM [Rosenstock, 1960]). The HBM is a widely used conceptual framework designed to model the factors underlying health behaviors. The HBM provides a model with several generic constructs that can be adapted to explain the beliefs behind specific health behaviors. Since its creation in the 1950s, the HBM has been applied to a wide variety of health behaviors (Hochbaum, 1958; Rosenstock, 1960; Janz et al, 2002). The components of the HBM are based on the theory that people will take actions to change health behaviors if they feel susceptible to a condition with consequences they feel are serious and will take actions if the benefits of taking action will outweigh the barriers to taking action (Janz et al, 2002). The constructs measured in the traditional HBM include the following: perceived susceptibility, perceived severity, perceived benefits, and perceived barriers. For behaviors requiring lifestyle changes, a construct of perceived self-efficacy to take action can also be included in the HBM (Rosenstock et al, 1988). The use of the HBM has been validated in several areas of health behaviors, including nutrition education, mediation compliance, and beliefs about hearing loss (Becker et al, 1974; Abood et al, 2003; Rawool and ColligonWayne, 2008). The HBM has been identified as particularly effective in explaining preventative health behaviors (Janz and Becker, 1984). Though they are common in other disciplines, few studies in the field of hearing conservation have used models of health beliefs to evaluate the factors predictive of health behaviors. Several studies have evaluated the correlates of adolescents’ exposure to loud music, though none to date have used a validated health behavior model, such as the HBM, to look specifically at listening behaviors for PLDs (Vogel et al, 2007). Additionally, cognitive theories of health models suggest that adolescents’ risk perception and knowledge impact their choices in health behaviors (Greening et al, 2005; Reyna and Farley, 2006). The action of “choosing moderate listening levels” is, in itself, a preventative health action. As the HBM has been validated for explaining preventative behaviors, it has been suggested as a good model for understanding knowledge, beliefs, and attitudes about PLD use (Sobel and Meikle, 2008). The goal of this study is to examine adolescents’ perceptions of their PLD use compared with laboratory measurements of CLL, as well as to model how adolescents’ beliefs about PLDs and hearing loss relate to actual behavior. Specifically, this study is designed to evaluate the following research questions: 1. What is the relationship between volume control settings and output levels of current-generation PLDs? 2. How do adolescents’ CLLs on digital PLDs change as a function of background noise and earphone style? 3. What is the relationship between self-reported listening levels and CLLs measured in the laboratory? 4. How effective is the use of the LHQ in predicting adolescents’ CLLs? EXPERIMENT 1: OUTPUT LEVEL MEASUREMENTS I nitial research by Keith et al (2008) and Keppler et al (2010) has indicated that PLD output levels could reach a level that increases the risk of MIHL. These studies provide excellent information and analysis showing the maximum output levels for several PLDs but do not provide data on output levels beyond the maximum. In order to assess CLLs by self-report, listeners must be given a metric with which to report their behavior. On many PLDs, a volume control provides a measure that can be represented by a visual analogue scale. However, to be able to convert a volume control setting to a CLL, it is critical to understand the relationship between volume control settings and PLD outputs. The available published data do not provide enough information to convert a volume control level to actual sound output levels. Thus, Experiment 1 examines the output level of PLDs in relation to the volume control for several PLDs and types of earphones. METHOD Digital Music Players and Headphones Five commercially available PLDs were evaluated: an Apple iPod, an Apple iPod Mini, an Apple iPod Nano, a Creative Zen Micro, and a SanDisk Sansa. Each device was purchased new prior to data collection and came with a set of stock (earbud-style) earphones. These players were chosen because they represented a cross section of the available devices on the market at the time of data collection, in late 2005. As of the third quarter of 2005, the Apple corporation held 44.8% of the market share for all digital music players and 71.5% of the hard-disk-based digital music player market, which is reflected in the choice of players for this study (Canalys, 2005). To examine the effect of different styles and brands of earphones, output measurements were taken using the stock earbud-style earphones that were packaged with each player, as well as with a set of Apple In-Ear earphones, Etymotic Research ER-6i isolator earphones, Shure ER4c isolator earphones, and a set of Koss supra-aural earphones. Apparatus Recordings were taken through a KEMAR manikin (G.R.A.S. Sound & Vibration, Denmark). In the KEMAR 666 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Teenage MP3 Player Use/Portnuff et al used, the pinna of the left ear was made of a softer silicone rubber than the right ear, creating a better fit of the earphone in the left ear than in the right ear. The earsimulating microphones were connected through ER-11 preamplifiers to an analog-to-digital converter (Echo Event) to a personal computer. A custom-designed Matlab 7.0 (Mathworksª, Needham, MA) program recorded the input to a 16-bit stereo .WAV file with a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz. The diffuse-field inverse filter of the ER-11 preamplifiers was engaged during recording, applying a transfer function to the signal so that the output was equivalent to diffuse-field recordings based on manufacturer data. at lower volume control levels were calculated using the regressions obtained by the pink noise recordings. Consistent with ISO 11904-2 (2004), A-weighting was then applied to the .WAV file through a digital filter in Matlab, and RMS averages and peak levels were calculated. Additionally, a voltmeter was used to measure the voltage output by each player for a 1000 Hz tone at 10% increments of the volume control. Between each measurement, the earphones were removed and replaced on the KEMAR. To determine the reliability of the measurements, all of the pure tone trials and 39 of the combined 151 pink noise and music trials were repeated three times. A total of 1024 recordings were completed. Output Level Measurement Procedure The top-ranked songs on the iTunes Web site at the time of data collection (www.itunes.com) were chosen from the categories Rock, R&B, Dance, Top 40, and Country and were purchased from a commercial music sales Web site. A 60 sec recording section was chosen for each song, starting at the beginning of the first chorus, to be used for analysis. The downloaded songs were transferred to the PLDs following the manufacturers’ directions via proprietary software where necessary. The songs are listed in Table 1. Additionally, recordings of pink noise and of a 1 kHz tone were generated at equal root mean square (RMS) levels in Adobe Audition 1.5 (Adobe Systems Inc., San Jose, CA), recorded to a 16-bit stereo .WAV file at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz, and transferred to each of the PLDs. For each recording trial, the chosen earphones were placed on both of KEMAR’s ears, and the recording was started at the chosen time. For recordings of the pure tone and noise, measurements were taken at 10% increments of the volume control 10–100% for each player and each set of earphones. The Apple and Creative products have a visual indicator of volume level, which was measured, divided into the 10% increments, and marked on the player. The SanDisk PLD had a variable volume control with incremental steps achieved by pushing a volume button. The total number of steps was counted and divided into the 10% increments. For music samples, recordings were taken at 100% of the volume control, and output levels Table 1. Music Samples Used to Evaluate Player Output Levels Artist James Blunt Nine Inch Nails Madonna Mary J. Blige Kenny Chesney Song Genre “You’re Beautiful” “Every Day Is Exactly the Same” “Sorry” “Be Without You” “Living in Fast Forward” Top 40 Rock Dance R&B Country RESULTS Output Voltage and Sound Pressure Levels A comparison of earphones across each player indicates that an incremental increase in volume results in a linear increase of output both in voltage and in diffuse-field equivalent A-weighted decibels. This linearity of change was independent of earphones used, and no earphones added nonlinear distortion to the output. Table 2 shows the incremental increase in volume for each player, as measured using pink noise. The average increase in output level for a 10% increase in the volume control for all players was 6.2 dBA, approximately doubling the sound pressure with a 10% increase in volume. Table 2 also shows the maximum voltage output of each player using a 1000 Hz tone. At 100% of the volume control, the peak sound pressure levels for the music studied ranged from 104.6 dBA to 126.9 dBA using the Creative Zen player with the iPod In-Ear earbuds. The highest peak level measured for any stimulus was 126.9 dBA for the Creative Zen player with the iPod In-Ear earphones. For all music samples, the average difference between peak level and RMS was 18 dB. Effect of Earphone Fit on Output Levels Measurements were taken simultaneously through the hard and soft pinnae on the KEMAR manikin. The difference in hardness of the pinna caused a noticeably different fit of the earbud style of earphones. As judged by the experimenters, the soft pinna created a good fit, and the hard pinna created a worse fit for the earbud. The difference in output levels due to earphone fit is shown in Table 3, which reports the average difference between the hard and soft pinnae when playing pink noise signals. The soft pinna, in the opinion of the researchers, was more similar in hardness to human pinnae. Therefore, all further measurements reported reflect the better fit of the soft KEMAR pinna. 667 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 22, Number 10, 2011 Table 2. Incremental Increase in Output Level with 10% Increase in Volume Control Player iPod iPod Mini iPod Nano Creative Zen Micro SanDisk Sansa Output Level Increase with 10% Volume Control Increase (dBA) Voltage at 100%/10% of Volume Control, 1 kHz Tone (mV) Output of 1 kHz Tone/Pink Noise Through Stock Earphones at 100% of Volume Control, “Ideal” Fit (dBA) 5.93 6.10 6.30 5.81 6.61 534/0.9 577/0.93 450/0.75 448/1.0 284/0.35 112.7/98.9 113.1/99.5 111.7/99.1 112.9/101.0 113.4/104.4 Differences Between Players, Stimuli, and Earphones Figure 1 shows the output levels of five models of MP3 players averaged across all music conditions using stock earphones. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) noted no significant differences in maximum outputs among players using all five music signals (F [4,124] 5 0.20, p 5 .94). Additionally, one-way ANOVAs revealed no significant differences among players for pink noise (F[4,24] 5 0.38, p 5 .82) and a 1 kHz tone (F[4,24] 5 1.05, p 5 .39). The average maximum RMS output level across all players, using the music samples, was 100.8 dBA, with a standard deviation of 1.24 dBA. As identified above, the linearity of the volume control setting (in dBA) can be seen in Figure 1. When averaged across all players using stock earphones, no significant differences were found among the five genres of music and the pink noise stimuli (F[4,124] 5 .07, p 5 1.0). All differences in output levels between genres of music were less than 1 dB. A oneway ANOVA found significant differences between the signals (F[5,150] 5 20.5, p , .001). A Scheffe post hoc test indicated that the differences are a result of differences between the 1 kHz tone and each of the music genres, and no differences were noted between the music genres and pink noise. For all players, the 1 kHz tone was 7.2 dB higher than the pink noise stimulus. Table 3 shows the maximum output levels of each earphone averaged across the music stimuli. A oneway ANOVA identified a significant difference among the maximum output levels of the earphones when averaged across all music genres (F[4,124] 5 85.3, p , .001). A Scheffe post hoc test revealed significant differences in output level among all pairs of earphones except between the Shure E4c earphone and the Etymotic Research ER-6i earphone. The isolator-style earphones produced higher output levels than the earbuds and supra-aural earphones. In order to assess the reliability of this method, intraclass correlations for these measurements were obtained using a one-way random effects model. For the betterfitting ear, an intraclass correlation of 0.975 was found, and for the worse-fitting ear, an intraclass correlation of 0.954 was found. These correlations indicate that the measurement procedure obtained stable, reliable measurements. Additionally, the mean range between the highest and lowest level for each trial was 1.1 dB, with a median of 0.5 dB. These small ranges and high intraclass correlations indicate good test–retest reliability. DISCUSSION T able 4 shows the average listening time necessary for each style of earphone to reach 50% daily noise dose, using the NIOSH DRC, which the authors suggest to be the maximum exposure from PLD alone (given a person may be exposed to other intense noise during the day). A general rule of thumb one might give PLD users is that they can listen to their music at 80% of the maximum volume control for 90 min per day, using the earbud earphones that are purchased with the PLD, without increasing their risk for MIHL. An additional concern reported in the literature has been that of acoustic trauma due to transient sounds in music samples, such as cymbal crashes. Though many of the peaks noted in the music samples could be considered to be transient sounds, none exceed the critical level noted by Price (1981) of 132 dB for causing acoustic trauma to tender ears. Thus, though the peak levels add to the overall RMS level of the recording, their presence does not inherently increase the risk of hearing loss. It is interesting to note that the highest peak level (126.9 dBA) identified here is somewhat lower than the highest peak level found for CD players (139 dB SPL [Fligor and Cox, 2004]). Table 3. Average Maximum Outputs in A-Weighted Decibels of Each Earphone, Averaged Across All Players and All Music Signals, with Ranges in Parentheses, and the Difference Between Good and Ideal Fits Using Pink Noise Earphone Average Maximum Output Level (dBA) Stock earbuds iPod In-Ear Shure E4c ER-6i Koss supra-aural 100.8 102.3 105.1 103.2 96.7 (96.3–103.9) (97.8–104.5) (102.3–107.6) (99.2–105.2) (89.5–103.9) 668 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Difference Between “Good” Fit and “Ideal” Fit (dBA) 7.47 1.09 1.18 1.87 1.17 Teenage MP3 Player Use/Portnuff et al relationship between attitudes and beliefs regarding PLD use and listening behaviors measured through self-report and in the laboratory. METHOD Listeners Figure 1. Diffuse-field equivalent output levels of five MP3 players, using stock earphones, as a function of volume control settings. The Grand Average is the mean of all music genres across all players. Error bars represent 61 SD of the Grand Average. The output levels reported in this experiment encompass the highest levels reported by Williams (2005, 2009), though comparisons between the studies are difficult, as Williams did not differentiate by model of MP3 player or earphone. Additionally, the maximum output levels reported in this study are lower than those of Hodgetts and colleagues (2007), a difference that is likely due to the fact that Hodgetts et al (2007) did not correct for the transfer function of the open ear to arrive at a diffuse-field equivalent level. A more direct comparison can be made between the present data and those reported by Keith et al (2008). They identified maximum RMS average levels for stock earphones between 101 and 107 dBA, which are slightly higher than those found in this study, although Keith et al (2008) evaluated many more PLDs and earphones that this study did. Additionally, Keith and colleagues found similar voltage output levels as this study, as well as a similar output level disparity between the better- and worse-fit earphones. The results for earbud-style earphones are very similar to those found by Keppler and colleagues (2010). EXPERIMENT 2: CHOSEN LISTENING LEVEL MEASUREMENTS A s Experiment 1 showed that PLDs are capable of producing output levels that could increase listeners’ risk of MIHL, Experiment 2 evaluated the CLLs of a set of adolescents using both laboratory measures and self-report measures. A new survey tool, the LHQ, was created to assess attitudes and beliefs about PLD use, as well as assessing individual usage of PLDs. Selfreported volume control levels were converted to estimated output levels on a decibel scale using the data from Experiment 1. Experiment 2 also evaluated the Twenty-nine normal-hearing teenagers, comprising 12 males and 17 females between the ages of 13 and 17 yr (mean age: 14.4 yr), were recruited from the Denver and Boulder, Colorado, metropolitan areas. All participants reported using an MP3 player at least 2 hr per week and had hearing thresholds within the normal range (15 dB HL or better) in both ears at octave frequencies from 250 to 8000 Hz, as well as at 3000 and 6000 Hz. All participants provided informed assent to the study, and parental permission was provided for all participants, under a protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Colorado at Boulder. Music Files Twenty-three songs representing the most popular songs in several musical genres were downloaded from the Apple corporation iTunes Web site. Songs were converted to .WAV format, lead-in and lead-out sections comprising silence were removed, and all songs were equalized by RMS voltage levels using a custom Matlab routine. Each .WAV file was then uploaded to an Apple fourth-generation iPod Classic (Apple Inc., San Jose, CA). Procedures for Determining Chosen Listening Level Participants were asked to choose one song from the list of available music files that was representative of the music genre they preferred to listen to. Consistent with ISO 11904-1 (2002), a probe microphone (Etymotic Research ER7c, Elk Grove Village, IL) was placed into the ear canal and secured to the ear with medical tape. Prior to insertion, the probe microphones were marked Table 4. Average Time to 50% Noise Dose (8 hr 85 dB LAeq) Using National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health Damage-Risk Criteria Maximum Listening Time per Day % of Volume Control 10–50 60 70 80 90 100 Earbud No limit No limit 6 hr 90 min 22 min 5 min Isolator Supra-aural No limit 14 hr 3.4 hr 50 min 12 min 3 min No limit No limit 19 hr 4.6 hr 66 min 16 min Note: “Earbud” includes stock earphones and iPod In-Ear earphones. “Isolator” includes Etymotic ER-6i and Shure E4c. 669 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 22, Number 10, 2011 at a length of 20 mm, and otoscopy was performed following insertion to ensure that the silicone tubing was within 5 mm of the eardrum. The investigator monitored insertion of the earphone into or onto the ear to ensure that the probe microphone was not dislodged. Using the Apple iPod, the participant’s chosen song was set on repeat with the volume control set to zero, and the participant was instructed to “turn the volume up to the level you like” once the listening trial started. The display of the iPod was obscured from the view of the participant. During each listening trial, a 30 sec recording was taken using a custom Matlab recording routine, and RMS average levels were calculated with both Aweighting and diffuse-field inverse filters applied digitally. This procedure was repeated for each earphone and in each background noise condition. All measurements were completed in a double-walled sound-attenuating booth with an ambient noise level of 13 dBA, complying with American National Standards Institute (ANSI) S3. 1-1999 standard for maximum permissible ambient noise levels in audiometric test rooms. CLLs were measured for all subjects for three different sets of earphones. These earphones were each representative of a popular style of earphone and included (1) a set of Apple iPod earbuds, (2) a set of supra-aural Sony MDR-110LP earphones (Sony Corporation, New York, NY), and (3) a set of Etymotic Research ER-6i IsolatorTM earphones (Etymotic Research, Inc., Elk Grove Village, IL). Participants were instructed to move the earphone into a position that was comfortable. For the in-ear isolator-style earphone, participants were provided with the instructions that are included with the earphones and were not coached on obtaining an optimal fit. Participants listened to each earphone in a randomized order within seven background noise conditions, including pink noise presented at 50, 60, 70, and 80 dBA; a recording from a bus at 70 dBA; and a recording of an airplane cabin at 75 dBA. The environmental recordings were downloaded from the Free Sound Project (www.freesound. org), an open-source repository for sound recordings. A “Quiet” condition was also included, with no noise stimulus presented (13 dBA ambient noise). One noise condition was repeated at random for each earphone to assess the reliability of measurements. When present, background noise was presented from four loudspeakers in the corners of the sound-attenuated booth, and levels were confirmed via a Brüel & Kjaer Type 2230 sound-level meter prior to data collection. the inward- and outward-facing flanges around the probe tube to attempt to obtain the same sound isolation as the original ER-6i flange. A 60 dBA pink noise stimulus was presented through the loudspeakers, and two 6 sec measurements were taken through the probe microphone first with an open ear canal and then with the earphone in place. The difference between the RMS average levels of the two measurements is reported as the noise-isolation level of the earphone. Listening Habits Questionnaire Participants also completed an LHQ, which assessed both listening behaviors and attitudes and beliefs about listening levels. To assess listening behaviors by a selfreport measure, the LHQ asked participants what type of earphones they typically use and how long they usually listen to their MP3 player during a day. Using a 1–10 scale, with 10 representing maximum volume, participants were asked to rate the volume setting at which they “usually” listened, what volume setting is “comfortable,” and what volume setting is “slightly too loud.” Participants were allowed to look at their own PLD or a laboratory PLD to help them gauge volume settings. Participants were encouraged to ask for clarification on any questionnaire items that they did not understand. To evaluate the use of the constructs of the HBM as predictors of CLL, each of the HBM constructs was incorporated into the LHQ. The questionnaire (Appendix 1) included a total of 26 questions, each designed to represent a part of an HBM construct. The wording of these questions was derived from Bryan et al (1997), who used a survey to assess condom use in undergraduate women. Readability analysis of the LHQ indicates that it has a Flesch-Kinkaid grade level of 7.0 and a SMOG Index of 6.8, indicating that it should be understandable to the study population (aged 13–17). The questions were based on a Likert scale with a range of 1 to 7, with high numbers indicating agreement with the statement. The questions for each construct were averaged into a subscale. The traditional constructs of the HBM were interpreted with regard to MIHL and PLD use and were represented by questions asking about perceived susceptibility to MIHL from PLD use, the perceived severity of MIHL, the perceived benefits of preventing MIHL, the perceived barriers to taking action to prevent MIHL, and the perceived self-efficacy in taking action to prevent MIHL. The specific questions asked can be found in Appendix 1. Noise Isolation Measurements RESULTS A noise isolation measurement was completed for each earphone using the MIRE technique described above. For noise isolation measures of the ER-6i, a hole the size of the probe tube was made in the earphone flanges, and the probe tube was inserted. A drop of glue was placed on Measured Chosen Listening Levels Plotted in Figure 2 are the CLLs measured in the laboratory as a function of the background noise level 670 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Teenage MP3 Player Use/Portnuff et al condition, divided by earphone. In this figure, 70 dBA pink noise was combined with the 70 dBA bus noise. As expected, CLL increases as the background noise increases for all earphones, though the absolute CLLs are higher for both earbuds and supra-aural earphones than for isolator earphones. Interestingly, listeners’ standard deviations for CLL decreased as the background noise level increased. A two-factor ANOVA indicated that significant differences in CLL were present within both earphone style and background noise level, though no significant interaction was identified between the two variables (F[17,689] 5 22.9, p , .01). Scheffe post hoc tests indicated that levels measured with the isolator earphone were significantly lower than those measured with the earbud or supra-auralstyle earphones ( p , .01). No significant differences were found between earbuds and supra-aural earphones. Additionally, Scheffe post hoc tests also indicated that CLLs were significantly different ( p , .05) among all pink noise conditions except Quiet and 50 dBA pink noise. CLLs for 70 dBA bus noise were significantly different from those in Quiet and 50 dBA, 60 dBA, and 80 dBA pink noise conditions but not for the 70 dBA pink noise. CLLs for 75 dBA airplane noise were significantly different from those in Quiet and 50 dBA and 60 dBA pink noise conditions. For each participant, the amount of sound isolation provided by each earphone was measured. As expected, isolator-style earphones provided the most attenuation (mean 5 8.4 dB, range: 2.3–14.5 dB), followed by earbud-style earphones (mean51.9 dB, range: 0–5.2 dB) and supra-aural earphones (mean 5 0.6 dB, range: 0–6.1 dB). A one-way ANOVA indicated that significant differences were present in noise-isolation levels among all earphone styles (F[2,687] 5 793.4, p , .01). These earphone differences might influence a subject’s CLL, as the level of the background noise reaching the ear- drum will be affected by the noise isolation provided by the earphone. Shown in Figure 3 are each subject’s CLLs as a function of individual estimated ambient ear canal noise level (EAECL), providing a view of the group’s performance as a whole. The EAECL was calculated by subtracting the measured earphone isolation from the known background noise level to provide an estimate of the actual background noise level at the eardrum with the earphone in place. The EAECL removes the variability of earphone isolation from this measure, and when CLL is plotted against the EAECL, the impact of background noise on CLL can be seen, regardless of the type of earphone used. A linear regression line fit to these data explained 31% of the variance in the data set (r2 5 0.31, p , .05). The average SNRs were calculated by subtracting the EAECLs from the measured CLL. Table 5 shows that the average SNR decreases as the background noise increases. A oneway ANOVA found significant differences among CLLs in the background noise conditions (F[5,684] 5 66.6, p , .01). A Scheffe post hoc test found significant differences in CLL among the Quiet, 50 dBA, 60 dBA, and 70 dBA background noise conditions ( p , .05). Figure 4 shows the percentage of participants who chose listening levels above 85 dBA. Because the mean levels of isolation provided by earbuds and supra-aural earphones (0.6 dBA and 1.9 dBA, respectively) were not significantly different, these two earphone styles were combined for data analysis. CLLs were higher when participants were using earbuds and supraaural earphones, which together provided an average 1.3 dBA of isolation, than when participants were using isolator earphones, which provided an average 8.4 dBA of isolation. A Pearson’s chi-square test identified significant differences between the two styles of earphones in the 70 dBA and 80 dBA background noise conditions ( p , .05). Figure 2. Diffuse-field equivalent chosen listening levels plotted as a function of the background noise level condition. Figure 3. Diffuse-field equivalent chosen listening levels as a function of the estimated ambient noise level in the ear canal for all trials in background noise, excluding the Quiet condition (r2 5 0.31). 671 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 22, Number 10, 2011 To assess the reliability of the laboratory measurement technique, one noise condition was repeated for each earphone used, resulting in three repeated measures for each subject. A total of 86 measures were repeated. These repetitions showed excellent reliability, indicated by a very high correlation between the two measurements (Spearman’s rho 5 0.932). Additionally, a paired-samples t-test comparing the first measurement to the second measurement found no significant differences between the two measures (t 5 –1.0, p 5 .316). Self-Reported CLLs On the LHQ, participants were asked to report the number of hours per day that they usually listened to their MP3 player. Participants’ mean and median listening time was 2 hr per day, with a range of 0.5–5.75 hr per day. Eighty percent of listeners listened 2.5 hr or less per day. Participants also reported, on a scale from 1 to 10, the level at which they “usually” listened and which style of earphones they used (earbuds, supra-aural, or isolator). Participants reported a mean “usual” listening level (on a scale of 1–10) of 5.6 (SD 5 1.72). Listeners also reported the levels that were “comfortable” (mean 5 5.5, SD 5 1.72) and that were “too loud” (mean 5 7.7, SD 5 1.47). All participants rated their usual volume control setting at a level below their “slightly too loud” rating and within one rating step of their individual “comfortable” volume control setting. Using the output level measurements above, each participant’s self-reported volume control setting was converted to an estimated diffuse-field equivalent dBA, considering the participant’s preferred earphone style indicated in the LHQ. This calculation estimated a mean volume control–equivalent CLL of 74.1 dBA (SD510.8, range: 52.3–91.8 dBA). The estimated equivalent CLL based on self-reported volume control setting was greater than 85 dBA in 20 percent of participants. Using participants’ self-reported listening time and self-reported volume control setting, individual noise doses were calculated for each Table 5. Mean Chosen Listening Levels (CLLs) Averaged Across Earphones, Correlations Between Measured CLL and Self-Reported CLLs, and the Average Signal-toNoise Ratio (SNR) at the Eardrum for Each Condition, Accounting for Individual Earphone Isolation Noise Condition Mean (SD) CLL (dBA) Quiet 50 dB 60 dB 70 dB 70 dB 75 dB 80 dB 68.3 70.6 74.6 79.3 79.1 81.3 84.3 pink noise pink noise pink noise bus noise airplane noise pink noise *p , .05; **p , .01. (10.9) (9.2) (7.3) (5.2) (5.3) (4.1) (3.0) Correlation SNR (dB) 0.619** 0.499** 0.440* 0.368* 0.439* 0.393* 0.298 54.7 21.1 14.7 9.2 12.2 9.8 4.13 Figure 4. Percentage of participants whose chosen listening level exceeded 85 dBA in each background noise condition (“PN” signifies pink noise). Significant differences between the two groups are noted by * (p , .05). participant, as reported in Table 6. These calculations reflect the noise doses for OSHA (1981), NIOSH (1998), and the EPCEU (2003) damage-risk criteria. HBM Constructs Mean scores on each of the HBM scale items are reported in Appendix 1. In order to determine how well the HBM constructs predict listening levels, several linear regression models were created. The averages for each HBM question on the LHQ are presented in Appendix 1. Table 7 presents the regression coefficients for each of these models, as well as Pearson’s correlation of regression (r2). Seven models used the HBM constructs measured on the LHQ to predict each of the CLL measurements in various conditions, and two models are presented using the HBM constructs to predict self-reported volume control–equivalent CLLs. When all variables are entered, no model predicts CLLs well for any of the noise conditions measured in the laboratory (r2 # 0.308). However, the full model predicting listeners’ self-reported volume control–equivalent CLLs explains 67.9% of the variance (r2 5 0.679). Though these full models reflect the behavior of only 29 participants, it is notable that only the model predicting the self-reported listening levels included any significant factors. Within that model, all factors were significant with the exception of the self-efficacy factor. To assess the impact of including the self-efficacy construct, a model was constructed with only susceptibility, severity, benefits, and barriers. Removal of the self-efficacy construct from this model leads to only a small change in the variance accounted for (r2 5 0.669). To assess the reliability of the scale variables created to represent each of the HBM constructs, Cronbach’s alpha was calculated for each scale. Cronbach’s alpha is a 672 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Teenage MP3 Player Use/Portnuff et al Table 6. Noise Dose Statistics for Self-Reported Chosen Listening Levels and Duration of Use According to Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA), National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH), and European Parliament and Council of the European Union (EPCEU) Damage-Risk Criteria Variable Average noise dose Noise dose range Subjects exceeding 50% noise dose Subjects exceeding 100% noise dose OSHA NIOSH EPCEU 8.0% 0–74.1% 1 (3.4%) 20.9% 0–241.0% 4 (13.8%) 66.4% 0–765% 7 (24.1%) 0 (0%) 2 (6.9%) 4 (13.8%) coefficient of internal consistency for a scale that measures how well a set of individual variables measures a single construct. For each scale variable, as reported in Appendix 1, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.8 or higher. This indicates that the set of questions asked for each HBM construct measures the computed scale variable well. Relationship Between CLLs and Self-Reported Levels Reported in Table 5 are the Pearson correlations between measured CLLs and self-reported volume control–equivalent CLLs. The strongest correlation between a measured CLL and self-reported CLLs was found in the Quiet condition (r 5 0.619). However, significant correlations ( p , .05) were noted in all conditions except for the 80 dBA pink noise condition. DISCUSSION A s expected, CLLs increased with the addition of background noise to the environment. Consistent with the findings of Fligor and Ives (2006), the presence of CLL differences between the isolator earphones and the other two styles suggests that earphone attenuation impacts CLL substantially. This is demonstrated in Fig- ure 3, where the CLL as a function of estimated ambient ear canal noise level is shown. The effect of earphone isolation on CLL lends credence to the recommendation that using earphones designed to passively reduce background noise will help to reduce the risk of MIHL from PLD use (Vogel et al, 2009). More important for assessing the risk to hearing, though, are the actual levels chosen by participants. While the average CLLs were below 85 dBA for all conditions, a subgroup of participants chose levels that might increase their risk for MIHL, depending on the durations of their listening. The CLLs measured in this study were, in general, consistent with CLLs measured in previous studies. Williams (2005) found an average listening level in a busy public place of 73.2 dBA for a group of adults, which is very similar to the average self-reported CLL of 74.1 dBA for adolescents in this study. Airo, Pekkarinen, and Olkinuora (1996) reported average CLLs for personal cassette players of 69 dBA using supra-aural earphones in quiet. This level is nearly identical to the 68.7 dBA documented in this study using supra-aural earphones. The graduate students in Fligor and Ives (2006) had average CLLs using stock earphones ranging from 62.2 to 80.8 dBA in the quiet through 80 dBA conditions. The present study of adolescents found a range of average CLLs from 70.1 to 86.0 dBA for the same conditions. A comparison of the reported CLLs indicates that the teenagers of this study chose levels, on average, 7.5 dB higher than those of the graduate students of Fligor and Ives (2006). More research is needed to directly compare the CLLs of different age groups and to understand the underlying factors that contribute to the age differences. The ultimate goal of identifying the output levels of PLDs is to determine the risk for hearing loss from using the devices. To that end, the output levels must be compared with DRC. OSHA (1981) accepts that roughly 25% of persons exposed at the permissible exposure level will have a material hearing impairment after a 40 yr working lifetime. NIOSH (1998) accepts that 8% of persons exposed at the recommended exposure level will have Table 7. Coefficients from the Linear Regression Models Predicting Both Measured Chosen Listening Levels (CLLs) in Each of the Seven Noise Conditions and the Self-Reported Volume Control Equivalent CLL Regression Coefficient Scale Quiet PN 50 PN 60 PN 70 Bus 70 Airplane 75 PN 80 Self-Report Self-Report Susceptibility Severity Benefits Barriers Self-efficacy 1.51 –0.721 –1.99 2.60 0.401 1.15 –0.693 –2.23 2.14 0.566 1.02 –0.594 –1.69 1.65 0.932 0.667 –0.222 –0.094 1.05 0.400 0.636 0.031 –0.848 1.16 0.516 0.349 –0.049 –0.517 1.01 0.166 0.245 0.553 –0.870 0.553 0.251 0.315* 0.699** –0.550* 0.556** –0.186 0.327* 0.718** –0.607** 0.635** 0.305 0.308 0.274 0.210 0.182 0.203 0.172 0.679 0.669 r2 Note: Each model includes each of the Health Belief Model scales measured on the Listening Habits Questionnaire, except for the last column, which reflects all but the Self-efficacy scale. PN 5 pink noise. ** p , .01; *p , .05. 673 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 22, Number 10, 2011 a material hearing impairment after a 40 yr working lifetime. The EPCEU (2003) directive is most conservative, serving to prevent any MIHL. As such, the EPCEU (2003) directive may be restrictive and difficult for PLD users to follow, particularly when using a PLD in background noise. Currently, no DRC exist specifically for music, nor is there consensus in the hearing conservation community about which DRC is most appropriate for evaluating music exposure. Table 6 presents the percentage of participants who exceed both 50 and 100% of a daily noise dose if they use their PLDs as reported on the LHQ. When considering an acceptable noise dose due to PLD use alone, other daily noise exposure, such as occupational or other recreational exposure, should be taken into account. As adolescents could obtain a portion of their noise dose from other activities in a given day, recommending limits that reach 100% of a daily noise dose from a PLD alone is not appropriate. A lower cutoff for exposure due to PLD use, such as a 50% noise dose, may be a better recommendation. Certainly, more research is needed to determine what cutoff level is the most appropriate for a recommended limit for PLD use in adolescents. Using the NIOSH criteria with a 50% noise dose limit, 14% of teenage listeners are potentially at risk for MIHL. While 14% of listeners is a relatively small percentage of the total population, as of October 2011 over 320 million iPods have been sold (Apple, Inc., 2011). Because the ownership rates of PLDs are high, a small percentage would translate to a very large number of individuals with hearing loss attributable, in part, to PLD use. While Shargorodsky et al (2010) did not find noise exposure to be a significant predictor of poorer high-frequency hearing thresholds in adolescents, they indicated that noise exposure could not be ruled out as a causal factor in the 30% increase in the number of teenagers with hearing thresholds outside the normal range from 1988 to 2006. Differences in Laboratory and Self-Report Measures This experiment found significant correlations between the participants’ self-reported volume control settings and laboratory-measured CLLs in several background noise conditions, as reported in Table 5. Interestingly, the strongest correlation of self-reported CLLs to a laboratory background noise condition was in the Quiet condition, though significant correlations were noted in most noise conditions. Several underlying factors may explain why these correlations were seen in multiple laboratory conditions. First, it is possible that the two measurements may be evaluating separate constructs. A laboratory measure captures behavior over the course of the experiment, but a self-report measure must examine past behavior. In this experiment, listeners provided a one-dimensional assessment of their typical behavior, reporting only their typical volume control setting. However, persons listening to PLDs may adjust the volume control to compensate for their environment. This reported typical setting may not truly reflect the listener’s behavior but, instead, could report an average volume control setting. A multidimensional self-report, where listeners report their usage time for various volume control levels, may provide a stronger association to laboratory measures. Second, it is possible that listeners are not able to provide an accurate report of their chosen volume control settings because they do not pay attention to a volume dial. Additional research is needed to assess the reliability of PLD users’ self-reports of listening behavior. Relationship of Attitudes and Beliefs to Behavior This study also shows that the constructs of the HBM may be useful in determining what factors influence an individual’s CLL. Interestingly, the HBM constructs were poor predictors for all of the CLLs measured in the laboratory, regardless of noise condition. However, the HBM constructs were a strong predictor of selfreported CLLs. To put these conditions into context, the self-reported CLLs could also be considered to represent a perceived CLL. Considering that all of the constructs of the HBM are dependent on the listener’s self-perceptions (i.e., Perceived Susceptibility to MIHL), it is possible that perceived beliefs might be more strongly related to perceived behaviors, rather than actually measured behaviors. Future research is needed to validate the efficacy of self-reported measures of listening behavior. Within the HBM that predicted self-reported CLLs, all individual constructs were significant predictors, with the exception of self-efficacy. Though the selfefficacy factor had a mean score of 5.44, which was similar to the other factors, 31% of participants marked the highest value (7) on the scale. A score of 7 on this scale indicates that the participant feels strongly that he or she is able to monitor listening levels in quiet and noisy environments and to appropriately limit his or her volume. The skew of responses and lack of significance of self-efficacy indicate that it may not be a construct necessary for understanding the relationship between beliefs and CLLs. The other constructs, though, were significant predictors of self-reported behavior. Examination of the regression model shows several trends. First, as might be expected, when perceived barriers to preventing MIHL increase, CLL increases. Similarly, when the perceived benefits of preventing MIHL increase, CLL decreases. Curiously, however, when perceived susceptibility to MIHL and perceived severity of MIHL increase, CLL increases. These regression results suggest that effective educational intervention might focus on promoting the benefits of and reducing the barriers to prevention, such as teaching what 674 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Teenage MP3 Player Use/Portnuff et al volume control setting is a safe level for the desired listening duration and the benefits of using soundisolating earphones when in noisy listening environments. While this study validates the use of the HBM constructs as factors predictive of self-reported CLLs, the small sample size of this study attenuates the extent to which these results can be generalized. Nevertheless, further investigation of the other HBM constructs is warranted to evaluate the relationships between attitudes and behaviors in more detail. A larger and more representative sample population may allow for extrapolation to a general population of adolescents. Moreover, a larger data set would allow for more sensitive measures, including path analysis or structural equation modeling for evaluating the strength of each construct in the HBM. Future studies of CLL and health beliefs should evaluate a large population to take advantage of these statistical techniques. Additionally, future studies in this area should consider expanding the HBM to include a standardized scale of adolescents’ risk-taking behavior or sensation-seeking behavior, as suggested by Bohlin and Erlandsson (2007). CONCLUSIONS C onsistent with previous research, this study shows that the current generation of PLDs is capable of producing output levels that could cause MIHL. Further, this study validates a method for evaluating the CLLs of adolescents. In this sample, a small but substantial number of adolescents listened at levels that increased their risk for MIHL. Though the percentage of adolescents who are at higher risk of MIHL is not higher than those reported for users of older technologies, adolescents in this study listened at higher levels than graduate students using similar technology (Fligor and Ives, 2006). Moreover, background noise levels and earphone sound isolation were important predictors of CLL. Interestingly, this study also found relationships between self-reported CLLs and measured CLLs in several laboratory conditions. As no clear, singular relationship was found, the results open the door to additional examinations of the validity of self-reported CLL to further understand why these relationships were identified. Finally, this study validates the use of the HBM for evaluating what factors influence CLL. With the exception of the self-efficacy construct, the HBM is an effective method for modeling self-reported listening behaviors. Further investigation applying this model to a larger population of PLD users is needed to examine the complex relationship between attitudes and beliefs about PLD use and hearing loss and real-world behavior. NOTE 1. For the purposes of this study, an earbud is a small earphone that fills the concha of the ear with the housing sitting medial to the tragus. An isolator is a flanged earphone that has been designed to attenuate background noise when properly inserted into the ear canal. A supra-aural earphone rests on the pinna of the ear. REFERENCES Abood DA, Black DR, Feral D. (2003) Nutrition education worksite intervention for university staff: application of the Health Belief Model. J Nutr Educ Behav 35:260–267. Ahmed S, King M, Morrish TW, Zaszewska E, Pichora-Fuller K. (2006) A survey of the use of portable audio devices by university students. Can Acoust 34:64–65. Airo E, Pekkarinen J, Olkinuora P. (1996) Listening to music with headphones: an assessment of noise exposure. Acta Acustica united with Acustica 82:885–894. Apple, Inc. (2011) Apple brings great new features and more affordable pricing to iPod touch and iPod nano. (Press release.) http:// www.apple.com/pr/library/2011/10/04Apple-Brings-Great-NewFeatures-More-Affordable-Pricing-to-iPod-touch-iPod-nano.html (accessed November 22, 2011). Becker MH, Drachman RH, Kirscht JP. (1974) A field experiment to evaluate various outcomes of continuity of physician care. Am J Public Health 64:1062–1070. Bryan AD, Aiken LS, West SG. (1997) Young women’s condom use: the influence of acceptance of sexuality, control over the sexual encounter and perceived susceptibility to common STDs. Health Psychol 16:468–479. Bohlin MC, Erlandsson SI. (2007) Risk behavior and noise exposure among adolescents. Noise Health 9:55–63. Canalys. (2005, September 8). Portable music player market expected to double by end of 2005. Retrieved October 15, 2006, from http://www.canalys.com/pr/2005/r2005091.htm Danhauer JL, Johnson CE, Byrd A, et al. (2009) Survey of college students on iPod use and hearing health. J Am Acad Audiol 20:5–27. Epstein M, Marozeau J, Cleveland S. (2010) Listening habits of iPod users. J Speech Lang Hear Res 53:1472–1477. European Parliament and Council of the European Union. (2003) European directive on physical agents (noise), directive 2003/10/ EC. Official J Eur Communities L42:38–44. Fligor BJ, Cox LC. (2004) Output levels of commercially available portable compact disk players and the potential risk to hearing. Ear Hear 25:513–527. Fligor BJ, Ives T. (2006). Does earphone type affect risk for recreational noise-induced hearing loss? Paper presented at the NoiseInduced Hearing Loss in Children at Work and at Play Conference, Covington, KY. Greening L, Stoppelbein L, Chandler CC, Elkin TD. (2005) Predictors of children’s and adolescents’ risk perception. J Pediatr Psychol 30:425–435. Gunderson E, Moline J, Catalano P. (1997) Risks of developing noise-induced hearing loss in employees of urban music clubs. Am J Ind Med 31:75–79. 675 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 22, Number 10, 2011 Hochbaum GM. (1958) Public Participation in Medical Screening Programs: A Sociopsychological Study. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Hodgetts WE, Rieger JM, Szarko RA. (2007) The effects of listening environment and earphone style on preferred listening levels of normal hearing adults using an MP3 player. Ear Hear 28:290–297. Hodgetts WE, Szarko RA, Rieger JM. (2009) What is the influence of background noise and exercise on the listening levels of iPod users? Int J Audiol 48:825–832. Hoover A, Krishnamurti S. (2010) Survey of college students’ MP3 listening: habits, safety issues, attitudes, and education. Am J Audiol 19:73–83. ISO. (2002). Acoustics—Determination of Sound Immission from Sound Sources Placed Close to the Ear—Part 1: Technique Using a Microphone in Real Ear (MIRE Technique). ISO 11904-1. Geneva, Switzerland: ISO. ISO. (2004). Acoustics—Determination of Sound Immission from Sound Sources Placed Close to the Ear—Part 2: Technique Using a Manikin. ISO 11904-2. Geneva, Switzerland: ISO. Janz NK, Becker MH. (1984) The Health Belief Model: a decade later. Health Educ Q 11:1–47. Janz NK, Champion VL, Strecher VJ. (2002) The Health Belief Model. In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Lewis FM, eds. Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research and Practice. 3rd edition. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 45–66. Keith SE, Michaud DS, Chiu V. (2008) Evaluating the maximum playback sound levels from portable digital audio players. J Acoust Soc Am 123:4227–4237. Keppler H, Dhooge I, Maes L, et al. (2010) Short term auditory effects of listening to an MP3 player. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 136:538–548. Mencher GT, Gerber SE, McCombe A. (1997) Audiology and Auditory Dysfunction. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health. (1998) Occupational Noise Exposure: Revised Criteria 1998. Cincinnati: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. National Institutes of Health. (1990) Consensus conference: noise and hearing loss. JAMA 263:3185–3190. Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA). (1981) Occupational noise exposures: hearing conservation amendment. Fed Regist 39:4078–4179. Price GR. (1981) Implications of a critical level in the ear for assessment of noise hazard at high intensities. J Acoust Soc Am 69:171–177. Rawool VW, Colligon-Wayne LA. (2008) Auditory lifestyles and beliefs related to hearing loss among college students in the USA. Noise Health 10:1–10. Reyna F, Farley F. (2006) Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making—implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychol Sci (Suppl S):1–44. Rosenstock IM. (1960) What research in motivation suggests for public health. Am J Public Health 50:295–302. Rosenstock IM, Strecher VJ, Becker MH. (1988) Social learning theory and the Health Belief Model. Health Educ Q 15: 175–183. Royster JD, Royster LH, Killion MC. (1991) Sound exposures and hearing thresholds of symphony orchestra musicians. J Acoust Soc Am 89:2793–2803. Serra MR, Baissoni EC, Richter U, et al. (2005) Recreational noise exposure and its effects on the hearing of adolescents. Part I: an interdisciplinary long-term study. Int J Audiol 44:65–73. Shargorodsky J, Curhan SG, Curhan GC, Eavey RD. (2010) Change in prevalence of hearing loss in US adolescents. JAMA 304:772–778. Sobel J, Meikle M. (2008) Applying health behavior theory to hearing-conservation interventions. Semin Hear 29:81–89. Torre P. (2008) Young adults’ use and output level settings of personal music systems. Ear Hear 29:791–799. Vogel I, Brug J, van der Ploeg CPB, Raar H. (2007) Young people’s exposure to loud music. Am J Prev Med 33:124–133. Vogel I, Brug J, van der Ploeg CPB, Raar H. (2009) Strategies for the prevention of MP3-induced hearing loss among adolescents: expert opinions from a Delphi study. Pediatrics 123: 1257–1262. Ward WD, Royster JD, Royster LH. (2000) Auditory and nonauditory effects of noise. In: Berger EH, Royster LH, Royster JD, Driscoll DP, Layne M, eds. The Noise Manual. 5th edition. Fairfax, VA: American Industrial Hygiene Association, 123–147. Williams W. (2005) Noise exposure levels from personal stereo system use. Int J Audiol 44:231–236. Williams W. (2009) Trends in listening to personal stereos. Int J Audiol 48:784–788. 676 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33 Teenage MP3 Player Use/Portnuff et al Appendix 1. Means and standard deviations of responses to Listening Habits Questionnaire questions organized by Health Belief Model construct. Indexes include Cronbach’s alpha, a measure of internal consistency reliability for a scale variable. Responses ranged from 1 to 7, with greater numbers indicating agreement with the statement. Question Mean SD Alpha Susceptibility to Music-Induced Hearing Loss (MIHL) 1. How susceptible to hearing loss do you feel? 2. What is the chance that you will experience hearing loss from listening to loud music? 3. How likely do you think it is that you will experience hearing loss resulting from listening to loud music on an MP3 player? 4. Would you say that you are the type of person who is likely to experience hearing loss? Susceptibility to MIHL Index 3.14 3.62 3.83 1.55 1.86 1.91 3.00 3.48 1.79 1.63 0.87 Severity of MIHL 1. How disruptive would hearing loss be to your quality of life? 2. How disruptive would the cost of treating hearing loss be? 3. How disruptive would it be to have to wear a hearing aid? 4. How disruptive would hearing loss be to your ability to communicate with your friends and loved ones? 5. How disruptive would it be to sustain permanent hearing loss as a result of listening to loud music? 6. Overall, how disruptive would hearing loss be in your life? Severity of MIHL Index 5.86 5.21 5.34 6.24 6.31 6.34 5.88 1.19 1.57 1.70 1.38 0.97 0.97 0.97 0.83 5.17 6.14 4.86 5.72 5.21 4.34 5.00 1.58 1.19 1.75 1.20 1.54 1.84 1.63 5.21 1.05 0.81 Barriers to Preventing MIHL 1. If I turned my music down to a safe level in a loud environment, I wouldn’t be able to hear it. 2. If I turned my music down to a safe level in a loud environment, I wouldn’t enjoy my music as much. 3. I don’t know what level my music should be turned down to in a loud environment to protect my hearing. 4. I don’t know what level my music should be turned down to in a quiet environment to protect my hearing. Barriers to Preventing MIHL Index 5.24 5.31 4.38 3.41 4.59 1.77 1.91 1.61 1.96 1.47 0.83 Self-Efficacy for Taking Preventative Action 1. I feel confident in my ability to monitor the volume at which I listen to my music. 2. I feel confident in my ability to make sure I listen to music at a safe level when I’m in a quiet environment. 3. I feel confident in my ability to make sure I listen to music at a safe level when I’m in a loud environment. 4. I feel confident in my ability to set the volume limiter of my MP3 player to a safe level. 5. If I knew I were listening at an unsafe level, I would be willing to turn down the volume. Self-Efficacy for Taking Preventative Action Index 5.55 5.93 5.14 5.17 5.41 5.44 1.40 1.16 1.55 1.61 1.66 1.24 0.89 Benefits of Preventing MIHL 1. Making sure I listen to music at safe levels would prevent me from experiencing hearing loss. 2. Turning my music down to a safe level when I’m in a quiet environment would be a good thing for me to do. 3. Turning my music down to a safe level when I’m in a loud environment would be a good thing for me to do. 4. Making sure my music is at a safe level when I’m in a quiet environment would prevent hearing loss. 5. Making sure my music is at a safe level when I’m in a loud environment would prevent hearing loss. 6. Setting my volume limiter at a safe level would be a good thing for me to do. 7. Using special earphones that block out background noise when I listen to music would be a good thing for me to do. Benefits of Preventing MIHL Index 677 Delivered by Ingenta to: American Academy of Audiology Members IP : 187.35.25.127 On: Wed, 08 Feb 2012 20:45:33