Download Can Environmentally Induced Epigenetic Changes be Trans

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Helitron (biology) wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Review Article
iMedPub Journals
http://www.imedpub.com
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
DOI: 10.21767/2472-1158.100003
Can Environmentally Induced Epigenetic
Changes be Trans-Generationally Inherited
by Offspring, Resulting in the Expression of
Psychological, Behavioural, or Psychiatric
Phenotypes? A Systematic Review
Abstract
Background: Epigenetic modifications have been found to be associated with
some psychiatric conditions. Environmental stressors can cause heritable changes
to the epigenome. Thus there is a possibility that parental epigenotype could
contribute to the expression of a psychiatric phenotype in offspring. Here we
summarise and evaluate research about trans-generational epigenetic inheritance
and its association with behavioural, psychological and psychiatric phenotypes.
Methods: Databases used included EMBASE <1947-Present>, Ovid MEDLINE(R)
<1946 to September Week 1 2014> & PsycINFO <1806 to September Week 1
2014>. Studies were included if at least two generations were assayed on both
epigenetic outcomes and behavioural/psychological/psychiatric outcomes. Given
the limited available data, a meta-analysis was currently not appropriate.
Findings: Results were heterogeneous and there were many different outcomes.
Generally behavioural alterations co-occurred with epigenetic modifications. There
was some evidence of inheritance of both these outcomes between generations.
Clinically relevant changes to gene expression levels and neuroanatomy were
observed in several cases. Possible modes of epigenetic transmission from the
experimentally exposed group to subsequent generations were implicated in
some studies.
Conclusions: Despite the idea of epigenetics mediating trans-generational
effects of the environment on brain and behaviour being widely touted, there is
currently very little evidence. Moreover, mechanisms mediating such processes
are unknown and thus whether epigenetic alterations induce behavioural change
is currently still a matter of debate.
Rachel A Caddick and
Anthony R Isles
Behavioural Genetics Group, MRC
Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics
and Genomics, Institute of Psychological
Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences,
School of Medicine, Cardiff University,
Cardiff, CF24 4HQ, United Kingdom.
Corresponding author
Rachel A Caddick

[email protected]
Behavioural Genetics Group, MRC Centre for
Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics,
Institute of Psychological Medicine and
Clinical Neurosciences, School of Medicine,
Cardiff University, Cardiff, CF24 4HQ, United
Kingdom.
Citation: Caddick RA, Isles AR. Can
Environmentally Induced Epigenetic
Changes be Trans-Generationally Inherited
by Offspring, Resulting in the Expression of
Psychological, Behavioural, or Psychiatric
Phenotypes? A Systematic Review. J Clin
Epigenet. 2016, 1:1.
Received: October 15, 2015, Accepted: November 03, 2015, Published: November
12, 2015
Background
Epigenetics (or more specifically epigenetic landscape) was a
term coined by Conrad Waddington (1942), originally used to
describe his conceptual model of how genes might interact
with the surrounding environment resulting in the expression
of altered phenotypes. He used this term to denote permanent
changes to the expression of genes that cause cell differentiation.
Epigenetics now most commonly refers to the heritable changes
of gene expression and activity that do not modify gene structure
[1]. Epigenetic modifications have numerous functions but
are most frequently associated with expression of genes that
mediate embryonic development. Within developing embryos,
epigenetic modifications influence erasure and re-establishment
of DNA methylation; imprinting of the genome; inactivation of the
X-chromosome; pluripotent stem cell development and control
of somatic cell differentiation [2]. Recently, epigenetic alterations
have also been implicated in disease pathology of psychiatric,
neurodevelopmental and neurodegenerative disorders. In
addition to this, some epigenetic alterations have been found
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License | This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
1
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
to be heritable across several generations. As the epigenome
is sensitive to environmental influences, there is a possibility
that environmental factors could cause heritable epigenetic
modifications that result in changes in gene expression that alter
behavioural, psychiatric or psychological phenotypes in offspring.
Structure of the Genome
DNA exists within the nucleus of animal cells as a double
helix structure usually tightly wrapped around proteins called
histones. The four histones around which DNA is coiled are
H2A, H2B, H3 and H4. These are repeated twice [3] making an
organised octamer structure with DNA, which is known as a
nucleosome. Linker histones from the H1 family prevent the DNA
from becoming incorrectly wrapped by sitting on the entry and
exit strands of DNA. Nucleosomes make up the repeating units
that form eukaryotic chromatin - a highly ordered structure
that ensures DNA can be packaged into the cell nucleus. One
of the functions of chromatin is to control gene expression and
replication [4].
Transcription and subsequently gene expression is dependent
on the structure of chromatin. RNA polymerases are responsible
for initiating transcription. However, RNA polymerase does not
transcribe every gene within the genome. Areas of the DNA with
a more open structure are more difficult to transcribe, and are
known as heterochromatin. Open areas (known as euchromatin)
allow the enzymes access to the genes and therefore they
can be more easily transcribed and the genes expressed as
RNA. Heterochromatin is basically tightly packed DNA, usually
located on the periphery of the nucleus. It generally consists of
genetically inactive satellite sequences of DNA [5], an excellent
example of this is the inactivated X chromosome in females.
There are two forms of heterochromatin: constitutive and
facultative. Constitutive heterochromatin domains are highly
condensed and usually consist of repetitive lengths of DNA that
are mostly transcriptionally silent in every cell of an organism.
For instance large stretches of the Y-chromosome, and the
centromeric- and telomeric-regions of all chromosomes. In
contrast, facultative heterochromatin refers to regions of the
genome that are transcriptionally silent in some cell-types, but
transcriptionally active in others. Once again the example of the
inactive X chromosome and the active X chromosome is a good
demonstration of this difference in chromatin structure.
The structural arrangement of chromatin and subsequent gene
expression is subject to reversible epigenetic changes including
DNA methylation, histone post-translational modifications
(PTMs) and alteration by non-coding RNAs (ncRNAs). These are
the epigenetic modifications of interest in this systematic review.
DNA Methylation
The 5’ position in the pyrimidine ring of cytosine bases in DNA
can be modified with methyl groups to create 5-methylcytosine
(m5C) by methyltransferases [6]. There are broadly two classes
of DNA methyltransferase (DNMT) enzymes are responsible for
this cytosine conversion; these are DNMTs responsible for de
novo methylation and those that copy the methylation already
existing onto daughter strands during cell replication [2]. Within
2
2015
2016
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
mammals DNA methylation is localised to CpG dinucleotides.
These are commonly found to cluster in sequences of 200 bases
made from a minimum of 50% cytosine and guanine, which
are known as CpG islands. In 40-50% of genes found in humans
CpG islands can be found close to or within promoter regions of
genes. However, such islands are often unmethylated and are
transcription factor targets.
DNA methylation is generally associated with transcriptional
silencing or repression and is implicated in gene regulation and
development [6]. Consistent with its commonly found location on
CpG islands, DNA methylation possibly blocks promoter regions
and prevents the binding of activating transcription factors. Thus,
DNA methylation is commonly associated with a lack of gene
expression [7].
However, there is now an increasing awareness of the importance
of non-CpG DNA-methylation, at so called CpH site (H = A, C, or
T) and also of hydroxymethylation (5hmC), an intermediate in
the recently identified active de-methylation process [8]. The
existence of 5hmC in particular points towards a more dynamic
role for DNA-methylation, which contrasts with prior notions of
this being the most robust and stable epigenetic mechanism [9]
Histone Modifications
Post translational modifications of the histone proteins (PTMs)
affect the 3 dimensional structure of chromatin and therefore
alter gene expression [10] PTMs are an example of epigenetic
modifications and are thought to affect gene expression
through changing the structure of the condensed chromatin
so different genes are exposed [11]. Histones can be altered
with covalent modifications including acetylation, methylation,
phosphorylation, sumoylation and ubiquitylation [12].Histone
acetylation and methylation regulate binding of effector molecules
by altering chromatin structure [13]. Lysine residues on histones
are subject to acetylation which neutralises the positive charge of
lysine that attracts histones to the DNA backbone. Subsequently,
acetylation of histones is associated with euchromatin and
increased expression [14]. Histone methylation, in contrast,
is associated with both enhancement of gene expression and
repression depending on location of the modifications [15].
DNA methylation is often dependent on histone methylation of
a particular lysine residue in histone H3 (H3-K9 methylation),
which is another mechanism in which histone methylation is
involved in gene repression. DNA methylation is also thought to
be involved in the deacetylation of histones, further increasing its
role in transcriptional silencing. Proteins can bind to methylated
DNA to form complexes that result in deacetylation. Similarly,
non-methylated DNA does not attract deacetylating enzymes
thus chromatin structure remains more open and available for
transcription.
Non-Coding RNAs
Non-Coding RNAs (ncRNAs) are RNA transcripts with no obvious
protein product. ncRNAs have a role in transcriptional processes
like DNA methylation and histone PTMs, but do not directly affect
the structure of DNA. There are several classes based on size and
function of ncRNAs including microRNAs, long RNAs (lncRNAs) and
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
small RNAs. Small RNAs are thought to regulate gene expression
as well as defensively suppress transposons. Of the numerous
types of small RNAs, microRNAs (miRNAs) are thought to be
most important in setting mRNA expression levels in eukaryotes
through post-transcriptional modulation [16]. Generally, miRNAs
are thought to bind to untranslated regions on the 3’ strand of
target mRNAs, this represses the production of protein via mRNA
destabilisation and silencing of translation [17]. In addition to
this, miRNAs and other small RNAs target epigenetic machinery
and transcription factors. Transcription factors (TF) and miRNAs
can work in pairs to regulate gene expression and suppress
functionally related proteins [18].
Evidence of Trans-generational Inheritance of Epigenetic Modifications
As this review has already defined epigenetics as the study
of heritable changes that alter gene expression but not DNA
structure, it should be elucidated that this means the epigenetic
features persist through both mitosis and meiosis [19]. This
systematic review will only focus on the trans-generational
inheritance of epigenetic changes caused by the environment.
Whilst some epigenetic markers such as DNA methylation have
been established as heritable throughout these processes, others
such as histone modifications have been disputed but for the
purposes of this review will still be considered.
There are, generally speaking, two fundamental types of cells
in the body. Somatic cells, which replicate by mitosis, and germ
cells, which can replicate by both mitosis and meiosis. Germ
cells produce gametes (sperm and ovum) that are able to fuse
during fertilisation to produce offspring. Mitotic inheritance of
epigenetic marks is common, for example the DNMTs responsible
for the copying of methylation of cytosines from mother strands
to daughter strands. The inheritance of histone modifications
appears much more complex. Whilst epigenetic information
provided by histone modifications should be inherited between
mitotic cell generations in order to maintain cell fate [20], such
modifications (like H3K27me3 and H3K9me3) aren’t precisely
transmitted from the parent histones to the new histones.
On both daughter strands, histones are randomly distributed
in clusters [21]. Whilst mechanisms of histone transmission
between mitotic states are unknown, since daughter cells inherit
some histones from parental cells, it is worth considering histone
modifications as mitotically heritable.
Meiosis is a type of cell division that allows gametogenesis or the
production of gametes. During gametogenesis the germ cells are
subjected to various chromatin remodelling events and eventually
their chromatin is condensed, becoming transcriptionally inert
[22]. This process is mediated by histones and transition proteins.
Small proteins called protamines replace histones in the late
haploid stage of spermatogenesis [23]. However, recent evidence
suggests that in approximately 1-10% of the genome in mammals
tested, histones within nucleosomes are retained throughout
spermatogenesis [24], though precise structural nature of these
nucleosomes is currently unknown.
After fertilisation, it is necessary that the maternal and paternal
chromatin is decompressed before transcription and other
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
somatic processes can take place. The protamines of the
paternal chromatin are exchanged for histones and genome wide
demethylation occurs, causing further chromatin remodelling.
However, the epigenetic marks associated with imprinted genes
remain intact. These processes happen rapidly after fertilisation
[22]. Generally most epigenetic modifications are thought to be
erased through these mechanisms between generations but DNA
methylation modifications are sometimes found to be heritable.
Histones maintained in sperm may persist in the zygote at least
to the one-cell embryo stage [25], for example, H3K27me3
retained in sperm are thought to persist causing repression in
the pre-implantation embryo. Furthermore, H3K4me3 in human
sperm correlates with corresponding early gene expression in the
embryo [25]. Thus such persistence of epigenetic marks could
have major functional consequences in developing embryos.
Epigenetic Modifications Implicated in Behavioural, Psychiatric
and Psychological Phenotypes
Firstly, various epigenetic modifications have been found to be
associated with psychiatric illness and particular behavioural
traits. In the study of disease discordant monozygotic twins, there
is differential methylation on specific CpG sites between twins
affected with bipolar, schizophrenia and those with psychosis
symptoms and the unaffected groups [26]. In addition to this,
there was decreased methylation in BDNF promotor exon IV and
IX in the frontal cortex of schizophrenic participants compared
to controls [27]. Increasingly, evidence suggests lncRNAs are
involved in disease pathology including mental disorders and
autism [28]. Whilst the association of epigenetic modifications
and a disease phenotype may not be causal in these cases, they
at least indicate that altered epigenetic regulation may play a role
in disease pathology and could be key early warning indicators of
disease risk or treatment targets.
The onset and development of psychiatric illness is often
attributed to being induced by an interaction of environmental
with an underlying genotype, as suggested by the Diathesis
Stress Model [29]. Environmental and genetic factors influence
the epigenome in the brain – which could potentially be the
gene-environment interaction mechanism responsible for the
expression of psychiatric phenotypes [30]. Various risk factors
for psychiatric disease have been found to induce epigenetic
changes as well as behaviours and symptoms associated with
such disorders, thus implying epigenetic modifications may have
a role in the onset of psychiatric conditions. Chronic social defeat
stress (CSDS), a risk factor for behavioural mal-adaptations in
rodent models, has been found to decrease H3K14 acetylation
and histone deacetylases (HDAC2 in this case) in the nucleus
accumbens, which are features also seen in depressed humans
[31]. CSDS paradigms have also been found to transiently enhance
then repress global H3K14 acetylation in the hippocampus,
modifications which can then be reversed by chronic imipramine
treatment [32], indicating histone H3 acetylation may be a target
of such antidepressant medicines and possibly involved in the
symptoms are depression.
Furthermore, such environmentally induced modifications of
phenotype and epigenotype have been found to persist over
several generations. For example, poor maternal care persists
3
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
through several generations with altered DNA methylation
profiles in offspring [33]. A dysmasculinized phenotype was
observed in two generations of male mice after chronic prenatal
stress in the first generation and this correlated with altered
miRNA profiles in the second generation offspring [34]. In
addition to this, altered histone PTMs have been implicated in
the inheritance of a cocaine-resistant phenotype [35]. A causal
link between epigenetic modifications and the production of a
particular disease phenotype is plausible [36]. Injected sperm
RNAs of males that had been exposed to unpredictable maternal
stress and separation (MSUS), into wild-type fertilised oocytes.
Behaviour observed in MSUS males were then observed in the
genetically unrelated MSUS-sperm injected offspring. This has led
to the development of the research question of whether transgenerational epigenetic inheritance induced by environmental
factors can produce behavioural, psychiatric and psychological
phenotypes in unexposed offspring.
Aims of the study
As research on trans-generational epigenetics is expanding
rapidly, the need for a systematic review of the research already
conducted is necessary. Currently, many systematic reviews on
epigenetics focus on a single generation or on outcomes other
than behavioural, psychological or psychiatric phenotypes.
However, trans-generational epigenetic inheritance potentially
has an important role on the transmission of psychiatric
disease and will surely be the focus of many future studies. This
is why a systematic review should be conducted, to summarise
current methodologies, theories and understandings – the
results of such a systematic review can be utilised by future
researchers to create methodologically and theoretically
sound experiments.
This systematic review aims to screen current literature for
relevant studies that examine the role of epigenetic inheritance
in the expression of behavioural, psychological or psychiatric
phenotypes. This aim can be split into several components.
Firstly, can epigenetic modifications induced by external stimuli
be trans-generationally inherited by progeny? Secondly, if
the epigenetic modification is trans-generationally inherited
by the offspring, is a change in behavioural, psychological or
psychiatric phenotype also present? Subsequently, this review
aims to assess whether this change in phenotype can possibly
be associated with the epigenetic change and through what
mechanisms this is possible. Finally, the impact of the findings
will be discussed in addition to a justification for further research
into the field of trans-generational epigenetics. Thus, to produce
a systematic review that summarises the information from these
papers in a comprehensive manner, it is necessary to assess the
methodological quality of the studies featured in this review. In
addition to this, the conclusions of the authors featured in these
papers need to be examined.
This systematic review aims to assess the hypothesis that
environmentally induced epigenetic modifications can
be inherited by offspring and expressed as a behavioural,
psychological or psychiatric phenotype.
4
2015
2016
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Methods
PICOS Structure in the Methodological Design of This Systematic
Review
Whilst the research question covers a fairly novel subject area for
systematic reviews, the methodology will still follow a traditional
structure as recommended by the Cochrane Collaboration [37].
The research question was designed and constructed through
the PICOS structure [38]. Below, the components of the PICOS
structure are listed and an explanation of how each aspect
pertains to the design of this systematic review is given.
Types of Participants
There was no particular participant group defined for this
systematic review as the study of trans-generational epigenetics
is a relatively new field, subsequently, it was expected that very
few papers would be identified and narrowing participant group
criteria would produce an extremely small yield. Thus it was also
assumed that most papers found would feature animal trials
rather than human-patient groups. The main participant criteria
of this systematic review was that papers should feature at least
two generations of participants for the measures of epigenetic
modifications – which is essential for the study of the transgenerational aspect of the research question. In addition to this,
this review did not accept papers studying unicellular populations
or plant populations.
Types of Intervention
Due to predicted restriction of study yield, very few intervention
requirements were stipulated. Most essentially, the experimental
intervention had to be implemented with the intent of producing
alterations to some aspect of the epigenome in participants. In
addition to this, as this paper is interested in trans-generational
inheritance, the intervention should be implemented in the
parental generation i.e. there should be at least one offspring
group not directly exposed to the intervention (not including
controls).
Types of Comparison Group
No particular comparison group was outlined but it was assumed
that most studies would have a control group of some description
(i.e. a group of participants that did not receive the intervention
expected to produce an epigenetic change and behavioural
change). Studies without an appropriate comparison group will
not be considered for further investigation. The conditions under
which the comparison group was raised/kept should be the same
for at least one epigenetic assay and one behavioural assay within
each paper, so meaningful associations can be made. However,
this is not a requirement and such paradigms will be discussed in
the results section.
Types of Outcome Measures
There are two main outcome types that were required for a paper
to be considered, the first being a heritable epigenetic change
caused by the intervention. For example, a stressor in the first
generation causes an epigenomic alteration - the study must then
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
investigate for similar epigenetic modifications in at least one
subsequent generation. Epigenetic modifications on imprinted
genes are not of interest for this systematic review.
The second outcome type is the measure of phenotype change –
the phenotypes of interest being either behavioural, psychological
or psychiatric. A neuroanatomical, cognitive or morphological
change by itself is not sufficient for inclusion, it must be linked to
behaviour or to a psychological/psychiatric disorder.
Types of Studies
Multiple types of study were considered for this review. The study
of trans-generational epigenetics has only been studied relatively
recently due to methodological restrictions prior to this. As a
result, many of the studies of interest are expected to be more
in the primary stages of research using experimental design more
familiar to psychological research than epidemiological research.
Experimental rather than observational studies are more likely
to be featured in this systematic review; limited theoretical
knowledge would probably render large scale epigenetic assays
hard to administer and evaluation of results of such studies
would be quite difficult to interpret due to a large number of
confounders that arise in such non-controlled conditions.
As most of the papers identified are likely to use experimental
laboratory-research design, it is also likely that most of the studies
will use one-way designs to study the direction of epigenetic
and behavioural changes e.g. performance of the experimental
group vs the control group on epigenetic/behavioural outcome.
However, many experiments will look at other factors affecting
the outcome, in addition to effect of experimental group, such
as gender; these will be factorial designs. It is also probable that
some paradigms will use a within-subjects design as participants
will be tested on multiple occasions or on multiple assays.
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
in PubMed, which established that different terminology or
synonyms often produced additional hits with possible relevance
for this review. These hits would otherwise not have been found.
Whilst constructing the search term procedure, known relevant
studies, such as [41] were used as a reference point for searching
i.e. a search procedure should be able to retrieve this study if
it was in the database, if it wasn’t able to then further relevant
search terms were required.
Each synonym was entered into each individual database (using
Ovid) and then expanded and mapped to subject headings to
find further useful categories, this produced 3 different search
term procedures. The 3 search term procedures were then
reintegrated so one unified search procedure was produced.
Search terms and subject headings that were valid in one or two
databases invalid in the others were converted to text terms so
scope was not reduced.
Search Term Procedure
The following databases were used in the identification of
relevant papers:
A. Ovid Medline (R) Without Revisions (1996-Present)
B. Embase
C. Psych Info
The following search terms were used in each database for
the identification of papers:
1. Epigenetic/
2. DNA acetylation.mp.
3. DNA methylation.mp.
4. DNA phosphorylation.mp.
Search Strategy
5. Epigen*.mp
Though the concept of epigenetics was first mentioned in the
1940s [39] studies on epigenetics have been limited by the
equipment and understanding until recent years, for example,
bisulfite sequencing was implemented for chemical analysis of
DNA methylation in 1994 [40]. Thus, it was deemed unnecessary
to carry out a hand search of data as it is unlikely that any relevant
papers would not have been written in electronic form or already
converted to electronic form.
6. exp epigenetics/
Electronic Searches
11.ncRNA*.mp.untranslated RNA.mp. or long untranslated
RNA.mp. or non coding RNA*.mp.
The outcome criteria of this systematic review are quite broad as
initial PubMed searches did not yield many studies of relevance.
As the field of trans-generational epigenetics has only recently
expanded, not all studies have been fully indexed to the relevant
area – i.e. not all papers referring to epigenetics will be retrieved
if the term “epigenetic” is used when searching in a database
(this was discovered during preliminary searches on PubMed).
As a consequence of this, in this review, multiple synonyms
have been used for each aspect of the research question.
Every possible search term was also expanded upon within
each individual database, so that as many papers as possible
are identified. Several preliminary searches were conducted
12.Epigenetic Inheritance.mp.
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
7. Histone post translational modification*.mp. or chromatin.
mp.
8. Histone PTM*.mp. or histone modification.mp.
9. MicroRNA*.mp.
10.miRNA*.mp. or small interfering RNA.mp.
13.multigener*.mp.
14.multigenerational.mp.
15.Soft Inheritance.mp.
16.transgener*.mp.
17.trans-generational.mp. or Inheritance Patterns.mp.
18.ancest*.mp.
19.descend*.mp.
5
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
20.Hereditary.mp.
46.43 and 44 and 45 and 46
22.inherit* .mp.
Study Yield
24.anxi*.mp. or exp anxiety disorder/
25.exp anxiety/
26.behav*.mp.
27.exp behavior/ or Mentally Ill Offenders.mp. or behaviour.
mp. or exp Social Behavior/
28.brain.mp. or exp brain/
29.depress*.mp.
30.exp reactive depression/ or depression.mp. or exp
endogenous depression/ or exp Depression/ or exp
Treatment Resistant Depression/
31.affect*.mp. or emotion*.mp. or stress.mp. or Arousal.mp.
or exp Expressed Emotion/
32.exp fear/ or exp conditioning/ or exp phobia/ or fear.mp.
33.habit*.mp.
34.exp neuroanatomy/ or neuroan*.mp.
35.neurobe*.mp.
36.exp neurobiology/ or neurobi*.mp. or exp attention deficit
disorder/ or substance related disorders.mp.
37.exp neurology/ or neurolo*.mp. or exp Epilepsy/ or
exp Neurology/ or exp Conversion Disorder/ or exp
Neuropsychology/
38.neuron*.mp. or exp Neurons/
39.mental disease.mp. or exp posttraumatic stress disorder/
or psychiat*.mp.
40.exp child psychiatry/ or exp psychiatry/ or exp forensic
psychiatry/ or psychiatry.mp. or exp Child Psychiatry/ or
exp Biological Psychiatry/ or exp Psychosis/ or exp Alcohol
Abuse/ or exp Drinking Behavior/ or exp Mental Health/
41.exp coping behavior/ or exp schizophrenia/ or exp
cognition/ or psychol*.mp.
The abstracts and titles of the studies yielded by the search
procedure were used to identify relevant papers.
A. Ovid Medline (R) Without Revisions (1996-Present):
a. 456 papers found, 109 were identified.
B. Embase:
a. 541 papers found, 124 were identified.
C. Psych Info
a. 72 papers found, 35 were identified.
At this stage review papers were included in order to ascertain
whether further relevant papers could be identified from their
contents that had not been found using the search procedure.
Inclusion & Exclusion Criteria
The 268 total identified papers were individually screened to
ascertain whether they were relevant. The same inclusion and
exclusion criteria were applied but at this stage review papers
were also excluded. No further relevant results were yielded and
the search procedure was thought to be sufficient. It should be
noted that the phenotype related outcomes of interest needed
for inclusion could be any paradigm relating to behaviour,
psychology or psychiatry; these included tests of locomotive
activity, sucrose consumption (as a measure of depression), etc.
Memory assays and brain chemical assays were not included in
this review. As the outcome criteria was so wide and may not have
been explicitly mentioned in the abstracts, many studies were
not excluded at the initial identification stage when abstracts
were scanned. However, imprinting disorders were excluded.
Epigenetic modifications of interest also excluded changes at
imprinted genes.
Papers would not be considered unless at least two generations
were studied. In addition to this, at least one generation must
not have been exposed to the experimental manipulation.
Furthermore, the study population could not be plants or
unicellular. Books or reviews were excluded if they did not
contain meaningful data. The inclusion and exclusion criteria are
summarised in Table 1.
Results
42.psychology.mp. or exp clinical psychology/ or exp
psychology/ or exp child psychology/ or exp medical
psychology/ or exp Adolescent Psychology/ or exp
Trauma/ or Addiction.mp. or exp Psychopathology/ or exp
Eating Disorders/ or personality.mp. or exp Motivation/ or
cogniti*.mp. or exp Decision Making/ or eating*.mp.
Study Selection Process
43.1 or 2 or 3 or 4 or 5 or 6 or 7 or 8 or 9 or 10 or 11 12 or 13
or 14 or 15 or 16 or 17
Of the 268 papers (162 were duplicates) identified using the
search procedure outlined in Section 2.7-2.9, only 106 were
retrieved. Some papers could not be found using the references
provided (N=17) and some were Non-English Language papers
that could not be translated (N=24). From these 100 retrieved
44.18 or 19 or 20 or 21 or 22 or 23
45.24 or 25 or 26 or 27 or 28 or 29 or 30 or 31 or 32 or 33 or
6
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
34 or 35 or 36 or 37 or 38 or 39 or 40 or 41 or 42
21.heritable.mp. or heritability.mp.
23.progeny.mp. or Offspring.mp. or Family Members.mp. or
exp Family Relations/ or Grandparents.mp. or Holocaust
Survivors.mp. or Family Background.mp. or Family History.
mp. or exp Mothers/ or human relation.mp. or exp family/
2015
2016
The initial search procedure yielded 1068 papers in total. In the
scanning of abstracts, 109 were found to be relevant in Embase;
124 in MedLine and 35 in PsychInfo. The articles were then
retrieved and read fully.
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
papers, six papers met all five inclusion criteria and did not meet
the parameters of the exclusion criteria. Please refer to Appendix
1 for the list of excluded papers from this stage.
Description of studies
All of the 6 papers identified were heterogeneous laboratory
conducted experiments. Within these the following behavioural
paradigms were studied: Accelerating Rotarod (n=1); Auditory
Fear Conditioning (n=1); Cocaine Self-Administration (n=1);
Ethanol Consumption/Preference (n=1); Elevated Plus Maze
(n=2); Forced Swim Test (n=2); Free Exploratory Paradigm
(n=1); Light-Dark Box (n=1); Maternal Behaviour Assay
(n=3); Olfactory Fear Potentiated Startle (OPS; n=1);
Olfactory Sensitivity Assay (n=1); Open Field Paradigm (n=2);
Sensorimotor Assay (n=1); Sucrose Consumption (n=1);
Sucrose-Self Administration (n=1); Tail Suspension Test (n=1) &
Two-Bottle Choice Assay (n=1). Please note that from now on
as no psychiatric or psychological outcomes found, the altered
phenotype will be referred to as a behavioural phenotype.
In addition to this the following epigenetic features were
examined: DNA methylation (n=3); Histone H3 Acetylation
(n=2); miRNA/ncRNA levels (n=2).
The results of the data extraction of each paper featured in this
systematic review can be seen in five parts. The behavioural
and epigenetic assay results included manually assessed
interpretations of visual data. Such interpretations were
included as many authors did not include statistical results that
were non-significant, it is generally good practise to publish nonsignificant results thus as much information pertaining to the
direction of non-significant results was recorded for this review.
Whilst some authors such as [42] often recorded results that were
approaching significance as “a trend in” other authors did not
do so. As this systematic review aims to provide information for
other researchers, it was decided that any available information
should be extracted should other experimenters wish to use
similar experimental paradigms in the future.
Design of Identified Studies
As predicted, the studies yielded were not epidemiological
in nature. Instead experiments conducted in laboratories
were found. Whilst systematic reviews traditionally examine
randomised control trial (RCT) designs and other epidemiological
studies, it was still decided that a systematic review of the
laboratory studies was possible as they can be viewed as the nonhuman equivalent of RCT studies (Kramer, 1998). At this stage
of research into the field of epigenetics, the use of laboratory
experiments is more appropriate for assaying the effects of
epigenetic inheritance on behavioural phenotype as extraneous
variables can be controlled, measured and their effect on the
results assessed. Table 4 provides a general summary of relevant
methodological design features of the identified papers.
Systematic Review of Participant Groups
Unfortunately, total participant number (n) for all experiments
cannot be reported as all data pertaining to n is not available in
identified papers (see Table S1, Appendix 2). Table 5 summarises
all information available about participant groups, including species
and breeds, reason for selecting these breeds and species and how
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
subsequent generations were produced. Four identified papers
examined mice [41-,43, 36) and two examined rats [44,9].
All four papers using mice used a C57Bl/6J strain. [42] outbred
these to Strain 1295v/lmJ mice to produce hybrids. [36] also used
an addition strain for IVF component of their study (where fertilised
eggs of BD62F1 mice were injected with C57Bl/6J sperm RNAs).
Dias and Ressler used an additional strain of mice, M71-IRES-tauLac
Transgenic Mice (M71-LacZ), which were involved in a variety of
behavioural and neuroanatomical experiments but not epigenetic
assays. [44]) bred Sprague-Dawley male rats who were subjected
to the cocaine self-administration paradigm to naïve females 3 days
after the last administration of cocaine (3/18 males bred continued
cocaine-self administration but offspring of these produced results
no different from the other sires thus groups were combined).
Finally, [9] used Long-Evans hooded rats in their experiment.
Systematic Review of Experimental
Manipulation
Firstly, please note that experimental manipulation is equivalent
to the environmental factor aspect of the research questions.
Two studies used MSUS as the experimental manipulation [43]
[36] Dias & Ressler used an olfactory fear conditioning paradigm;
Finegersh & Homanics used vapour ethanol exposure; [44] exposed
rats to cocaine-self administration & [9] used a stress paradigm
during gestation. Only the F0 generation was exposed to the
experimental manipulation in studies conducted by [41,[42,44]
In the MSUS studies, both F0 dams and F1 litters were exposed to
the experimental manipulation. Finally, [9] had several generations
exposed to the experimental manipulation: for each generation the
participants were split into stressed and non-stressed groups.
Only [41] and [42] used at least one control group that had followed
exactly the same experimental procedure as the experimental
manipulation group. [44] used a similar stressful procedure in
controls (intravenous injection of saline). Franklin et al (2010) and
Gapp et al (2014) merely left the control groups in the home cage
with 1 cage change per week (though given the nature of MSUS
alternative control group procedures are difficult). Yao et al (2014)
did not specify further than “not stressed”.
[41] studied multiple different groups of unexposed progeny of F0conditioned sires. [42] [44] studied only one unexposed group of
progeny of F0-exposed sires each. [43] [36] studied 2 unexposed
group of progeny of F1-exposed males. Yao et al (2014) examined
3 unexposed progeny of F1 females who were stressed in utero.
Heritable Behavioural Phenotypes
Although there are limitations associated with the tests used
(see Appendix 2, Table S2), all the experimental manipulations induced
a behavioural phenotype in at least one gender of offspring (Table 6).
Inheritance of behavioural phenotypes was observed through
the maternal, and the paternal line depending on the procedure
used. Inheritance of behaviour was often sex linked, though many
studies only examined behavioural transmission in one gender.
Heritable Epigenetic Modifications
Again, although there are limitations associated with the
techniques used (see Appendix 2, Tables S3) overall, most studies
have demonstrated that some epigenetic modifications can be
7
2015
2016
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Table 1 Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria.
Inclusion Criteria
• Presence of trans-generational inheritance of traits
i.e. across more than one generation.
• Epigenetic modification studied in 2 generations.
• Behavioural, psychiatric of psychological phenotype
studied.
• Appropriate control group for comparison of results
of epigenetic assay and behavioural/psychological/
psychiatric assay.
• At least one generation must not have direct or in
utero exposure to experimental manipulation.
Exclusion Criteria
• Any of the five Inclusion Criteria were not met.
• If trans-generational inheritance refers to in utero effects between mothers
and offspring and no subsequent generation is studied.
• If behaviours/epigenetic modifications refers to imprinting disorders.
• If behavioural assay refers to memory.
• If paper was from book or other medium that does not contain meaningful
data.
• If study population is not animals/humans e.g. unicellular organisms and
plants.
Table 2 Numbers of Papers Identified and Excluded by Scanning of Abstracts.
Identified
Inappropriate Participant Group
No Epigenetic Outcome of Interest
No Trans-generational Aspect
No Phenotype Outcome of Interest
No Epigenetic & Trans-generational
No Epigenetic & Phenotype of Interest
No Phenotype of Interest & Trans-generational
No Epigenetic, Phenotype or Trans-generational
Book/No Data
Table 3 Numbers of Papers Identified and Excluded by Reading of Full
Paper.
Identified
Inappropriate Epigenetic Measure
Inappropriate Behavioural Outcome
No Trans-generational Study of
Epigenetic Inheritance
No Trans-generational Aspect /
Generation is not Removed from
Experimental Exposure
Review/No Data
Number of Papers
6
19
8
3
2
68
transmitted to subsequent generations. However, transmission
appears complex in most cases and often a direct transmission
to a subsequent generation. The alteration and inheritance of
miRNA/ncRNA profiles can differ in direction between generations
and between neuroanatomical area (Table 7).
Experimental manipulations modified DNA methylation levels at
various loci in both directions (Table 8). Altered DNA methylation
levels in specific areas tend to be inherited in the same direction
(i.e. either increased or decreased) as they are found in the
parental generation.
[44] found significantly increased AcH3 associated with Bdnf
promoter IV in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) of F1 males.
Furthermore increased AcH3 levels were found in the testes of F0
males and specifically (though global levels were not increased)
at Bdnf promoters I, IV and VI in F0 sperm. This indicates a
possible mode of transmission of Histone H3 acetylation (AcH3)
through the paternal germline should histones persist through
spermatogenesis (Table 9).
8
Embase
109
84
14
10
145
4
27
42
21
0
MedLine
124
80
13
9
210
4
36
50
15
0
PsychInfo
35
0
3
7
2
6
0
0
5
14
Discussion
This systematic review summarised all data from six papers
examining trans-generational inheritance of epigenetic marks and
their association with behavioural outcomes (Table 10). Specifically
we focused on the role of environmentally induced modifications
(i.e. those changes caused by experimental manipulation) in the
production of altered behavioural phenotype. Despite much
discussion in the literature, original studies were limited and
most studies included are in their early stages of methodological
design, thus, interpretations of the results extracted are mainly
associative. Furthermore, it is difficult at this point in time to
extend any findings of experimental research from laboratories
to humans. For example, [44] acknowledge that their findings of
a cocaine resistant phenotype varies considerably from evidence
of cocaine-related behaviours found in humans, where addiction
is found to be heritable (Kendler et al, 2003). Currently, there are
too many confounding variables in human life to be able to draw
parallels from laboratory experiments. However, some promising
evidence has been provided by the identified studies. Whilst
the evidence is not conclusive proof of the trans-generational
inheritance of environmentally induced epigenetic modifications
producing altered behavioural phenotypes, it at least provides an
evidence-based foundation for future studies to expand upon.
Of all the papers identified, [36] provides the most convincing
support for the hypothesis that environmentally induced
epigenetic modifications can be passed to subsequent
generations and expressed as behavioural phenotypes. Their
experimental procedure provides the only direct example of
an epigenetic induced change to behaviour. Firstly, the authors
found an altered non-specific behavioural phenotype was present
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Table 4 Summary of Identified Papers
Dias & Ressler [41]
Subject Species
Mice
Breed(s) of Subject M71-LacZ transgenic.
Species
C57Bl/6J.
Environmental
Stressor Paradigm
Used
(Experimental
Manipulation of
Interest)
Group(s) Subjected
to Environmental
Stressor
Manipulation for
Control Group
Epigenetic
Modification of
Interest
Area Epigenetic
Modification(s)
Assayed.
Gene(s)/Promoters
Epigenetic
Modification(s)
Studied On.
Generation
Epigenetic
Modifications
Studies In.
Initial
Inheritance through
parental Germline
Olfactory Fear
Conditioning
(acetophenone)
F0 Males.
F0 Females.
Finegersh &
Homanics [42]
Mice
C57Bl/6J (Sires)
C57Bl/6J x
Strain 129Sv/ImJ
(Offspring)
Vassoler et al
[44]
Rats
Franklin et al [43]
Gapp et al [36]
Mice
Mice
C57Bl6/J
C57Bl/6J
SpragueDawley
Long-Evans Hooded.
MSUS
Selfadministered
cocaine
infusion
Stress by Restraint
& Forced Swim.
Vapour EtOH
exposure.
MSUS
Yao et al [9]
Rats
F0.
F1.
F2.
F0 Males.
F0 dams & F1 litters.
F1 Litters.
F0
Room air exposure.
Undisturbed dams
and litters
Left in Home Cage.
Selfadministered
Saline infusion
Not Stressed.
DNA Methylation.
DNA Methylation.
miRNA expression
levels.
Histone H3
Acetylation.
miRNAs.
Sperm.
mPFC.
VTA.
Brain.
Sperm.
Serum.
Sperm.
Hippocampus.
Hypothalamus.
Cortex.
mPFC
Sperm
Brain.
Placenta.
Uterus.
Bdnf.
Dlk1.
5HT1A
CB1
CRFR2
MAOA
MeCP2
N/A.
Bdnf exon I, IV
& VI.
N/A.
F0
F1
F2
F0
F1
F1
F2
F1
F2
RNAinj
F0
F1
F0
F1
F2
Both.
Paternal.
Paternal.
Paternal.
Paternal.
Maternal.
Left in Home Cage
or Olfactory Fear
Conditioning with
Propanol.
DNA Methylation.
Histone H3
Acetylation.
Sperm.
MOE.
Epididymis.
Olfr151 (M71).
Olfr6.
Generations Not
Directly Exposed
to Experimental
Manipulation.
F1.
F2.
F1.
F2.
F2.
F1.
F1-SN
F2-SNN
F2-SSN
F3-SNNN
F3-SSNN
F3-SSSN
Gender Used in
Study for Epigenetic
Assays
Male.
Both.
Both.
Male.
Male.
Female.
Maternal Behaviour.
Forced Swim.
Cocaine Self
OPS.
Accelerating
Odour Sensitivity
Rotarod.
Sucrose
Administration.
Elevated Plus Maze.
Consumption.
Maternal
Assay.
Elevated Plus Maze.
Light-Dark Box.
Behavioural Assay(s)
Elevated Plus Maze.
Open Field.
Tail Suspension Test.
Behaviour.
Forced Swim.
Auditory Fear
Two-Bottle Choice.
Free Exploratory
Sucrose Self
Paradigm.
Administration.
Conditioning Assay.
Open Field.
in successive generations (this includes measures of altered
responses to aversive, stress inducing and anxiogenic stimuli),
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
Maternal PostPartum.
Sensorimotor
Behaviour.
the second generation of which were not directly exposed to the
environmental stressor (in this case MSUS). In addition to this,
9
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
2015
2016
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Table 5 Summary of Experimental Manipulation.
Study
Experimental Procedure
Control Group
Procedure
Groups Exposed to
Experimental Procedure
Age Exposed to
Experimental
Procedure
Unexposed Progeny of
Experimental Group
F1-C57-Ace(nfost)
F0 C57Bl/6J Dams
F1-C57-Ace(fost)
F1-IVF
Trained to associate
acetophenone (10s)
presentation with coterminating mild footLeft in home cage
or conditioned with
Dias & Ressler [41] shock (0.25-s 0.4mA).
propanol.
Average interval:
120s. Trained on 3
consecutive days: 5 odour
presentation trials/day.
F1-C57-Ace
F0 C57Bl/6J Sires
F2-C57-Ace
2 months old
F1-M71-Ace
F0 M71 Dams
2 months old
F1-M71IVF
F0 M71 Sires
Finegersh &
Homanics [42]
~250μl/min vapor EtOH
inhalation in custom
Placed in equivalent
0.5” plexiglass vapour
chamber but exposed
chambers (16”x16”x24”)
to room air.
from 8:00-16:00 5d/5wks
at 78˚F.
F0 C57Bl/6J sires
2 months old
Dams not specified.
Litters PND1-14
Franklin et al [43]
MSUS 3hr/day
Undisturbed (1 cage
change/week).
F0 dams and F1 litters.
Gapp et al [36]
MSUS: 3hr/day proximal
separation
Undisturbed (1 cage
change/week).
F0 C57Bl/6J dams & F1
litters
Cocaine selfadministration in operant
chamber through
indwelling silastic catheter
in right jugular vein
Vassoler et al [44] (0.25mg dose over 5s,
20s intervals). Limited to
75 infusions/2hr/d for
60d. Mean infusions at
start= 3-4mg/day. Mean
infusions D60=7-8mg/day.
Pregnant dams GD-12-18.
Restraint in plexiglass 6cm
inner diameter container
for 20mins (prevented
turning and maintains
standing) in time period
Yao et al [9]
8:00-9:00.
Between 16:00-17:00
forced swim in tank
(45x77x50cm, water 21˚C)
for 5mins.
10
Yoked intravenous
saline injections.
F0-Sires
Dams: 2-3months
Litters: PND1-14
Not specified.
F1-E-Sired
F2-MSUS
F3-MSUS
F2-MSUS (C57Bl/6J)
F1-RNAinj (B6ND2F1)
F1-Coc-Sired
F0-S
F1-SS
Non-Stressed,
otherwise not
specified.
F2-SSS
(and therefore their in
utero pups)
F2-SNN
Not specified but on
GD12-18
F3-SNNN
F3-SSNN
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Table 6 Summary of Behavioural Results.
Overall
Outcome of
Interest
Experimental
Manipulation
Paradigm Used and Results Indicate Result
EPM: Decreased anxiety/reduced
behavioural control.
Forced Swim: Increased depressive
like symptoms.
Behavioural
Index of Maternal Care &
Manifestations MSUS of F0 Dams Index of Absence of Care
of Early Life
and F1 Litters
Stress
Generations Outcome Observed In
Relative to Respective Control Group
Decreased Latency to Enter
Open Arm
F1-MSUS male#
F2-MSUS male (Gapp Only)
F2-MSUS females (Franklin Only)
F3-MSUS females
Increased Latency to Enter
F3-MSUS males (Franklin Only)
No Differences in Locomotive
Activity
F1-MSUS male#
F2-MSUS male#
F1-MSUS-RNAinj males (Gapp Only)
Increased Time Floating
F1-MSUS male#
F2-MSUS female (Franklin Only)
F2-MSUS male#
F2-MSUS-RNAinj
(Gapp Only)
Decreased time floating after
desipramine treatment
F2-MSUS-Des vs. MSUS-Sal (Franklin
Only)
Increased time floating after
desipramine treatment
F2-MSUS-Sal vs. F2-C-Sal (Franklin Only)
Decreased time actively
nursing on PND1-7
F0-MSUS females
Increased time spent off nest
on PND1-7
F0-MSUS females
Light-Dark Box: Altered response to
F1-MSUS males
Increased Time in Illuminated
behavioural phenotype seen in two
F2-MSUS males
Compartment
generations of males.
F1-MSUS-RNAinj males
Open Field: Inheritance of MSUS
induced F1 male phenotype by
F2 and F3 females. Test showed
reduced behavioural control.
(Franklin Only)
Sucrose Consumption: Showed
MSUS produces anhedonia in first
generation only. (Franklin Only)
TST (Franklin Only): Increased
depressive like symptoms.
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
Decreased Latency to Enter
Unfamiliar Arm
F1-MSUS males
F2-MSUS females
F3-MSUS females
No Difference in Distance
Travelled
F1-MSUS males
F2-MSUS females on Trial 3
Increased Difference Travelled F2-MSUS females on Trials 1 and 2.
Decreased Sucrose
Consumption
F1-MSUS male
Decreased time immobile
after acute and chronic saline F2-MSUS-Sal vs. F2-C-Sal
treatment
Increased time immobile after
acute and chronic desipramine F2-MSUS-Des vs. F2-MSUS-Sal
treatment
11
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
Behavioural
Manifestations Chronic Stress
of Preterm Birth
Inclined Plane Test: Delayed
Sensorimotor Development
Increased time spent in
downward position (increases
with every successive
generation).
Maternal Post-Partum Behaviour
Decreased Tail Chasing and
Rotational Behaviours
Cocaine Self-Administration (FR1):
Cocaine Resistant Phenotype
Cocaine Self-Administration (PR1):
Cocaine Resistant Phenotype
Sucrose Self-Administration (FR1):
Cocaine-resistant phenotype
Cocaine-Self
Administration in in cocaine self-administration
paradigms not confounded by
F0 males
learning deficits.
Sucrose Self-Administration (PR):
Cocaine-resistant phenotype
in cocaine self-administration
paradigms not confounded by
learning deficits.
Maternal Behaviour
Decreased Cocaine Infusions
at 0.5mg
2015
2016
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
F1-SN
F2-SNN
F2-SSN
F3-SNNN
F3-SSNN
F3-SSSN
F1-SN
F1-SS
F2-SNN
F2-SSN
F2-SSS
F1-C females
F1-C males
F1-C-DMSO
Decreased Cocaine Infusions
at 1.0mg
F1-C males
Reduction of breakpoint at
1.0mg/g
F1-C males
No Difference Between
Sucrose Self-Administration
F1-Coc-Sired males vs. F1-Sal-Sired males
No Differences in Breakpoint
of Sucrose Administration
F1-Coc-Sired males vs. F1-Sal-Sired males
No Differences in Licking/
Grooming
F0-Dams of Cocaine Sired Offspring
No Differences in Time off Nest F0-Dams of Cocaine Sired Offspring
Drug Related
Behaviours
Accelerating Rotarod: Paternal
alcohol exposure subtly increases
motor enhancement after ethanol
exposure.
EPM: Paternal alcohol exposure
increases anxiolytic and enhanced
locomotor effects of alcohol.
Open Field: Basal Activity not
increased in mice found to have
Ethanol Exposure ethanol sensitive phenotype on
other assays.
in F0 males.
Two-Bottle Choice Assay (Ethanol):
12
Increased Time Spent on
Rotarod on 5th Trial
F1-E-Sired-EtOHinj Males vs. F1-E-Siredsalineinj
Increased Time in Open Arm
F1-E males
F1-E-ETOH-inj males
Increased Proportion of Open F1-E males
Arm Entries
F1-E-ETOH-inj males
Increased Number of Total
Arm Entries
F1-E-ETOH-inj males
No Differences in Distance
Travelled
F1-E-ETOH-inj males vs F1-E-Siredsalineinj
Decreased Preference at 3%
Concentrations
Decreased Preference at 6%
Concentrations
F1-E males
F1-E males
Decreased Preference at 9%
Concentrations
F1-E males
Decreased Consumption at
12% Concentrations
F1-E males
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
Olfactory
Sensitivity
Olfactory
Conditioning to
Acetophenone in
F0 males.
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Two-Bottle Choice Assay (Quinine
and Saccharin):
No Differences in Quinine or
Saccharin Preference at 0.03
and 0.06mM Concentrations
F1-E males vs F1-C males
Auditory Fear Conditioning
No Significant Difference in
Acquisition, Consolidation
and Extinction Retention of
Aversive Memory Cue.
F1-Ace-C57 vs. F1-Home-C57
EPM
Equal Time in Open & Closed
Arms and Equal Entries into
Open Arms
All Groups
Increased OPS to
acetophenone
F1-Ace-C57
F2-Ace-C57
F1-Ace-M71
F1-Ace-C57-nfost
F1-Ace-C57-fost
Increased OPS to propanol
F2-Prop-C57
F1-Prop-M71
OPS
Olfactory Sensitivity Assay
altered miRNAs levels were found in neural areas associated
with stress response in two successive generations of mice.
One of the altered miRNAs (miR-375) and its predicted target
catenin β1 (Ctnnb) may be particularly implicated in the altered
phenotype, as both are associated in the corticotropin-releasing
factor signalling pathways. These pathways are involved in stress
response. They also observed modified levels of both miR-375
and Ctnnb1 in the hippocampus – an area directly involved in
stress response. A possible causal link between MSUS altered
miRNAs and the expression of this modified phenotype can be
supported by their findings that show an injection of RNAs from
F0-MSUS sperm into naïve fertilised oocytes produces the same
behavioural phenotype in the adult progeny as that observed in
F0 males. The MSUS RNA-injected test group, also demonstrated
similar metabolic phenotypes to the MSUS mice and upregulation
of miR-375 in the hippocampus. It is possible that other
mechanisms also mediate the transmission of this behavioural
phenotype. There was no difference, compared to controls, of
sncRNA levels found in F2-MSUS sperm and serum. Furthermore,
normal sncRNA profiles were found in F3 progeny. As the miRNA
profile did not differ from controls, the behaviour of the F3-MSUS
offspring should not be altered. However, F3 progeny still exhibit
the same behaviour found in the F1 and F2-MSUS generation. The
authors suggest miRNAs alter other epigenetic aspects i.e. DNA
methylation, and these are responsible for further transmission.
Whilst posited, these alternative modes of inheritance have not
been further explored.
[44] provide some interesting evidence of an environmentally
induced (cocaine exposure) epigenetic modification (increased
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
Increased Aversion Index
to 0.003% and 0.006%
F1-Ace-C57
acetophenone concentration.
Increased Aversion Index to
0.003% and 0.006% propanol
concentration.
F1-Prop-C57
AcH3) that is expressed in the sperm and testes of F0 males and
subsequently found in functionally relevant areas of the brain in
offspring (mPFC). The AcH3 increase was found on gene promoters
that correspond to the observed cocaine-resistant phenotype
found both in the F0 and F1 male test subjects. Increased AcH3
was associated specifically to Bdnf promoters (I, VI and VI), which
would increase BDNF expression (AcH3 is generally thought of
as facilitative to transcription). Furthermore, the authors found
that decreased BDNF activity at the TrkB receptor reversed
the observed cocaine-resistant phenotype in cocaine-exposed
offspring, implicating a causal role of BDNF. Whilst this evidence
seems fairly convincing, the data still lacks a clear mechanism for
which the histone modifications could be inherited as histones are
mostly replaced by protoamines during spermatogenesis. Thus
such results may not be as a common occurrence as it is thought.
Only 1-10% of histones persist throughout spermatogenesis [24]
It is unclear whether epigenetic modifications persist in both
genders as only male profiles were tested – the inheritance of
epigenotype and expression of phenotype may be much more
complex than the data presented in this paper would first imply.
However, the results are very much worth considering for further
research as potentially this paradigm could easily be used whilst
controlling for confounders with multifactorial analysis.
Similarly, [42] also found a male-only behavioural phenotype.
Ethanol exposure induced hypomethylation of Bdnf exon IXa in
F0 sperm, which provides a possible mechanism of inheritance of
the epigenetic modification and therefore possible transmission
of phenotype in offspring. In addition to this, Bdnf exon IXa
transcripts were found to be upregulated in functionally relevant
13
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
2015
2016
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Table 7 Summary of miRNA and ncRNA Results
Experimental Manipulation Area assayed
Generation Change Observed In
miRNA
miR-24-1-5p
Frontal Cortices
F0-MSUS male
miR-138-3p
miR-145-3p
miR-375
miR-375-3p
F1-MSUS male
Hippocampus
MSUS
Hypothalamus
Sperm
miR-466c-5p
miR-375-3p
F2-MSUS male
miR-375-5p
F1-RNA-inj
F1-MSUS male
miR-466c-5p
miR-375-5p
miR-375-5p
miR-200b-3p
miR-375-5p
F1-MSUS male
miR-466c-5p
miR-672-5p
miR-375-3p
miR-375-3p
F2-MSUS male
Serum
miR-375-5p
F1-MSUS male
miR-466c-5p
miR-672-5p
miR-466c-5p
miR-23b
F2-MSUS male
F1-SS
miR-200c
miR-96
miR-141
miR-182
Frontal Cortices
miR-183
F2-SSS
miR-200a
Stress GD12-18
miR-200b
miR-429
Placenta
miR-451
miR-181a
miR-181a
miR-200b
F2-SNN
F2-SSS
F1-SS
miR-429
miR-200b
Uterus
F2-SSS
miR-429
Please note that green refers to increased levels and blue refers to decreased levels of miRNAs.
14
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
2015
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Table 8 Summary of DNA Methylation Results
Experimental
Manipulation
Bdnf Exon IXa
Direction of Change Compared
to Controls
EtOH Exposure
Decrease
CB1
MSUS
Increased
CRFR2
MSUS
Decreased
IG DMR of Dlk1
MSUS
Decreased
MSUS
Increased
MeCP2
Location of
Analysis
Sperm
VTA
Sperm
Brain
Sperm
Brain
Sperm
Sperm
Brain
Olfr151
Conditioning to
Acetophenone
Decreased
Sperm
No Difference
MOE
Generation and Gender
F0 male
F1 male
F1 female
F1 male
F2 female
F1 male
F2 male
F2 female
F0 male
F1 males
F2 males
F2 females
F0 male
F1 male
F1 male
F2 male
Table 9 Summary of Histone PTM Results
Experimental
Manipulation
Direction of Changed
of Histone PTM
Area Assayed
Gene
Promoter
Assayed
Olfactory Fear
Conditioning
Decrease
Epidydymis
N/A
mPFC
Cocaine SelfAdministration
Increase
Sperm
Testes
Generation Studied In
Histone H3 Acetylation
F0 male
Histone H3 Methylation
F0 male
Bdnf Promoter
Histone H3 Acetylation
IV
Bdnf
Promoters I, IV Histone H3 Acetylation
& VI
N/A.
Histone H3 Acetylation
areas of the brain (the VTA). As Bdnf is thought to be a regulator
of drinking behaviour, this supports the implications that ethanol
induced hypomethylation may contribute to the behavioural
phenotype observed [33]. Interestingly, hypomethylation of the
Bdnf IXa promoter is found in both female and male offspring
of ethanol exposed mice. This result can be interpreted in two
ways. Firstly, hormonal or other biological differences between
males and females may interact with epigenetic modifications to
produce either no change (in the case of females) or the change
observed in male offspring. Secondly, the hypomethylation
observed may have little or no influence on the behavioural
phenotype observed. Unfortunately, the evidence linking
hypomethylation to the behaviour is still only speculative and
thus interpreting this complex sex-dependent transmission of
behaviour is particularly difficult.
[41] also begin to provide convincing evidence of a possibly
causative role of environmentally induced epigenetic
modifications in the expression of an altered behavioural
phenotype. An odour-sensitive behavioural phenotype specific
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
Histone PTM Type
F1 male
F0 male
F0 males
to the F0 conditioned odour was observed in two subsequent
generations, through both the maternal and paternal line and also
in offspring that were cross-fostered by non-exposed mothers.
These results taken together suggest biological transmission of
behaviour rather than behavioural transmission. In addition to
this, in these generations and an IVF-derived generation (sperm
from acetophenone conditioned males used to fertilise oocyte
of naïve females) neuroanatomical changes corresponded
accordingly to this odour sensitive phenotype – which would
be the biological basis possibly responsible for the observed
behavioural phenotype. Bisulfite sequencing also indicated
hypomethylation in sperm at Olfr151. As hypomethylation was
located on sperm, this indicates a mechanism that changes
could be inherited from F0 sires to F1 offspring and then to F2
offspring. Hypomethylation of Olfr151 theoretically corresponds
to increased Olfr151 expression that was observed in the brain.
However, there was no difference in methylation status of
Olfr151 in the MOE of F1 and F2 animals. Like [36] [41] attribute
the lack of expected epigenetic modification in the main olfactory
15
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
2015
2016
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Table 10 Summary of All Results Providing Evidence or Negating that Environmentally Induced Epigenetic Modifications can be inherited and
Contribute to the Expression of a Behavioural Phenotype in Offspring
Authors
Experimental
Manipulation
Behavioural Phenotype
Evidence Supporting
Epigenetic
Evidence Confounding/Negating
Contribution to
Epigenetic Contribution to Expression of
Expression of
Behaviour
Behaviour
OIfr151 (M71)
responds to
acetophenone.
Associated
Epigenetic
Mechanisms
No methylation
of Olfr6 which is
unresponsive to
acetophenone.
Dias & Ressler
[41]
F0 odour fear
conditioning to
acetophenone.
Olfactory sensitivity to F0
conditioned odour.
Decreased DNA Hypomethylation of
methylation of sperm inherited from
F0 to F1.
Olfr151
Transmission
of behavioural
phenotype through
the paternal line
and in F2 and crossfostered generation
suggest biological
transmission.
Bdnf regulates
drinking behaviour.
Decreased ethanol
Decreased DNA
Finegersh &
preference/consumption
F0 sire exposure to
methylation of
Homanics
and increased sensitivity to
ethanol.
Bdnf exon IXa
[42]
ethanol induced anxiolysis
promoter.
and locomotor enhancement.
Bdnf mRNA levels
in VTA and on
sperm increased:
corresponds to
hypomethylation.
Mechanism for inducing methylation
change unknown.
Lack of hypomethylation in MOE.
Lack of alterations to histone PTMs in
sperm.
Mechanism for inheriting epigenetic
change is unknown.
Females have hypomethylation but no
changes to phenotype.
Reduced alcohol
consumption seen.
Franklin et al
MSUS with F0
[43]
dams and F1 litters
Gapp et al
[36]
MSUS with F0
dams and F1 litters
Non-specific altered
behavioural phenotype on
various tests.
DNA
methylation
Non-specific altered
behavioural phenotype on
various tests.
sncRNAs
Vassoler et al F0 sire cocaine-self
Cocaine-resistant phenotype
[44]
administration
16
AcH3
Mechanism for inheriting epigenetic
Altered methylation
change is unknown.
at CB1, CRFR2 and
MeCP2 in F1 sperm
Normal methylation profiles in sperm
and female brain.
of F2 males and all areas tested in F3
CRFR2 and MeCP2 in
males but still expressed anxiety related
F2 sperm.
phenotype.
Injection of MSUSNormal miRNA profiles in sperm of F2
sperm RNAs into
males and all areas tested in F3 males
oocytes produces
but still expression of behavioural
behavioural
phenotype.
phenotype.
Increased association
of AcH3 with Bdnf
exon IV in both mPFC
of offspring and
sperm of sires. Bdnf
exon I and VI also
more acetylated in
sperm of sires.
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
Decreased postpartum
Successive
maternal behaviours and
generations
increased sensorimotor
exposed to
Yao et al [9]
deficits. Both of which were
restraint and
increasingly different from
forced swim stress
controls with each successive
on GD 12-18
generation.
miRNAs
Altered behavioural
phenotype and
altered miRNA
profiles.
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Confounding due to maternal behaviour
and in utero effects. Only one group
removed from in utero effects had
miRNA profiles measured and this was
only in one area.
epithelia (MOE) to alternative modes of epigenetic inheritance.
This was a reasonable assumption as other types of epigenetic
inheritance have been found to alter olfactory receptor loci
in the MOE [34]. Based on research suggesting a relationship
between DNA methylation and histone modifications [6] the
authors investigated histone H3 acetylation and methylation
but ChIP revealed no differences on either measure in sperm
chromatin – which the authors suggest could be due to incorrect
immunoprecipitation. As a result, a mechanism is needed that
explains how hypomethylation produces the neuroanatomical
and subsequent behavioural changes before conclusions can be
drawn on this data.
such epigenetic contributions to behaviour are very complex
and there are many variations in possible phenotype that they
could potentially produce. In order to better understand the role
of altered DNA methylation of these candidate genes, an assay
of the brain of F1 and F2-MSUS males and F3 offspring would
have been prudent to see if this could clarify the complex sexdependent results. The authors suggest alternative epigenetic
modifications such as ncRNAs could be responsible but no
additional assays were conducted. Currently, the most conclusive
thing that can be said of such data is there is a general association
between the MSUS induced behavioural phenotype and altered
DNA methylation profiles.
Despite a lack of hypomethylation of the Olfr151 locus in the MOE
and no evidence of histone modification mediated inheritance,
[41]still imply the observed neuroanatomical and behavioural
changes may be associated with epigenetic mechanisms. They
suggest that ncRNA inheritance may be mediating the transmission
of odour-sensitivity, citing [45] who found ncRNA inheritance
was observed in the Kit locus. Whilst the evidence found in their
paper is quite convincing, there are several components missing
in order to conclusively say that an environmentally induced
epigenetic modification has produced a behavioural phenotype
in offspring. Firstly, a mechanism for odorants to produce an
epigenetic change is required. [41] suggest blood-borne odorants
may activate receptors in sperm due to the fear conditioning
protocol [46] but there is no conclusive evidence yet.
The results from [9] do not contribute much to the argument.
Unfortunately, it is hard to distinguish any evidence of causality
as much of their data is subject to confounding. Only the F2-SNN
(stress, non-stress, non-stress), F3-SNNN (stress, non-stress,
non-stress, non-stress), and F3-SSNN (stress, stress, non-stress,
non-stress) generations would have not been directly affected
by the experimental manipulation (to avoid in utero confounding
F1-SN, F1-SS, F2-SSN, F2-SSS and F3-SSSN were excluded from
consideration in this systematic review). Of these experimental
groups, miRNA data was only taken for F2-SNN and this was only
in the placenta. Thus, it is still unclear whether miRNAs could
contribute to the increasingly distinctive altered phenotype
observed in successive generations of stressed mice. Certainly
at least, this study contributes results that are useful for future
research to build upon but the design of the study does not allow
for conclusions pertaining to this systematic review’s research
question to be drawn.
[43] perhaps provide the most difficult results to interpret.
Their behavioural assays can only be taken as suggestive of the
inheritance of a “non-specific behavioural phenotype”. Not all
results are provided and this makes it hard to comment on the
specific inheritance of a phenotype. For example, F1 female
behavioural data is missing but sometimes referred to and
statistical analysis of performance on a tail suspension test (TST)
by F1 males is also absent. From the data available, it appears
alterations of behaviour (in all tests) are generally observed in F1
MSUS males and their F2 female but not male progeny. The F3
MSUS generation males but not females showed inheritance of a
depressive-like phenotype seen in their F1-MSUS grandsires which
their F2-MSUS sires do not exhibit. In contrast F3-MSUS females
but not males tend to show similar performance on locomotor
assays to their F1-MSUS grandsires. Similarly, epigenetic assays
show various mixed results. MeCP2 hypermethylation and CRFR2
hypomethylation is seen in F1 and F2-MSUS sperm and in F2MSUS female brains. CB1 hypermethylation is observed in F1MSUS sperm and F2-MSUS female brains but not in F2-MSUS
sperm. These candidate genes are quite promising examples of
areas that might have a role in inducing the observed behavioural
phenotype as they are commonly associated with stress response,
depressive and affective disorders [47,48,49,50,51]. However,
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
Conclusion
This study aimed to examine the role of trans-generational
epigenetic inheritance in the expression of psychiatric,
psychological and behavioural phenotypes in progeny. A key take
home message is that currently there is a lot of hype and overlyzealous attribution of the role of epigenetics in contributing to
various phenomena [52-84]. This is clearly evidenced by the fact
that of the 100 retrieved papers in this systematic review, only
six original articles were identified, with 68 being review articles
and/or containing no original data.
However, taking all the results from the identified papers together,
this systematic review has found strong associations between the
inheritance of environmentally induced epigenetic modifications
and the presence of altered behavioural phenotypes in
offspring of experimentally manipulated subjects. Many of the
neuroanatomical, physiological and behavioural outcomes also
provide support for an epigenetic causality or contribution.
Unfortunately, confounding is a major problem in many papers
and thus definitive conclusions should not be drawn. Moreover,
17
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
experimental research currently lacks clear mechanisms
illuminating how environmental stressors alter the epigenotype
and how this altered epigenotype can be inherited. Nevertheless,
the results of this systematic review begin to provide a basis on
which future research can build.
Acknowledgements
The authors should like to thank Paul Buckland and Holly Thomas
for help with analyses, and Jan Chatting for support throughout
this study.
18
2015
2016
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
Funding
This work was funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC)
Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics (G0801418)
Competing and Conflicting Interests
The authors declare they have no competing or conflicting
interests.
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
References
1 Wu, C. & Morris, J.R. (2001). Genes, genetics, and epigenetics: a
correspondence. Science 239: 1103-1105.
2 Phillips, T. (2008). The Role of Methylation in Gene Expression.
Nature Education 1-116.
3 Luger, K Mader, A.W, Richmond, RK, Sargent, DF. & Richmond, TJ
(1997) Crystal structure of nucleosome core particle 2.8 A resolution.
Nature 389: 251-260.
4 Li, Q. & Zhang, Z. (2012). Linking DNA replication to heterochromatin
silencing and epigenetic inheritance. Acta Biochimica et Biophysica
Sinica, 44: 3-13.
5 Tost, J. DNA methylation: an introduction to the biology and diseaseassociated changes of a promising biomarker. Methods in Molecular
Biology 507: 3-20.
6 Bergman, Y. & Cedar, H. (2013). DNA methylation dynamics in health
and disease. Nature Structural and Molecular Biology 20: 274-281.
7 Suzuki, M.M. & Bird, A. (2008). DNA methylation lanscapes:
provovative insights from epigenomics. Nature Reviews Genetics 9:
465-476.
8 Kohli, R.M. & Zhang, Y. (2013) TET enzymes, TDG and the dynamics
of DNA demethylation. Nature 502: 472-479.
9 Yao, Y., Robinson, A., Zucchi, F., Robbins, J., Babenko, O., & Kovalchuk,
O. et al. (2014). Ancestral exposure to stress epigenetically programs
preterm birth risk and adverse maternal and newborn outcomes.
BMC Medicine 1: 121
10 Jenuwein, T. & Allis, CD (2001). Translating the histone code. Science
293: 1074-1080.
11 Greer, E.L. & Shi, Y. (2012). Histone methylation: a dynamic mark in
health, disease and inheritance. Nature Reviews Genetics 13: 342-357.
12 Kouzarides, T. & Berger, S.L. (2007). In Allis, C.D., Jenuwein, T. &
Reinberg, D. Epigenetics. 81-100. New York: Cold Spring Harbor
Laboratory Press.
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
21 Annunziato, A.T. (2012). Assembling Chromatin: the Long and
Winding Road. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta 1819: 196-210.
22 Ooi, S.L. & Henikoff, S. (2007). Germline histone dynamics and
epigenetics. Current Opinion in Cell Biology 19: 257-265.
23 Churikov, D., Zalenskaya, I.A. & Zalensky, A.O. (2004). Male germlinespecific histones in mouse and man. Cytogenetic and Genome
Research 105: 203-214.
24 Erkek, S., Hisano, M., Liang, C.Y., Gill, M., Murr, R., Dieker, J.,
Schubeler, D., Vlag, J., Stadler, M.B. & Peters, A.H. (2013). Molecular
determinants of nucleosomes retention at CpG-rich sequences in
mouse spermatozoa. Nature Structural and Molecular Biology 20:
868-875.
25 van de Werken, C., van der Heijden, G.W., Eleveld, C., Teeuwssen,
M., Albert,M., Baarends, W.M., Laven, J.S.E., Peters, A.H.F.M. &
Baart, E.B. (2014) Paternal heterochromatin formation in human
embryos is H3K9/HP1 directed and primed by sperm-derived histone
modificaitons. Nature Communications 5: 58-68.
26 Dempster, E.L., Pidsley, R., Schalkwyk. L.C., Owens, S., Georgiades,
A., Kane, F., Kalidindi, S., Picchioni, M., Kravariti, E., Toulopoulou,
T., Murray, R.M. & Mill, J. (2011). Disease-associated epigenetic
changes in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia and
bipolar disorder. Human Molecular Genetics 15: 4786-4796.
27 Ikegame, T. Bundo, M. Sunaga, F. Asai, T. Nishimura, F. Yoshikawa,
A. Kawamura, Y. Hibino, H. Tochigi, M. Kakiuchi, C. Sasaki, T. Kato,
T. Kasai, K. Iwamoto K. (2013). DNA methylation analysis of BDNF
gene promoters in peripheral blood cells of schizophrenia patients.
Neuroscience Research 77: 208-214.
28 Ziats, M.N. & Rennert, O.M. (2013). Aberrant expression of long
noncoding RNAs in autistic brain. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience
49: 589-593.
29 Oatley, K., Keltner, D. & Jenkins, J.M. (2006) Understanding Emotions
(2nd Ed). 321-351. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing.
30 Petronis, A. (2010). Epigenetics as a unifying principle in the aetiology
of complex traits and diseases. Nature 465: 721-727.
13 Bannister, A.J. & Kouzarides, T. (2011). Regulation of chromatin by
histone modifications. Cell Research, 21: 381-395.
31 Uchida, S., Hara, K., Kobayashi, A., Otsuki, K., Yamagata, H., Hobara,
T., Suzuki, Miyata, N. & Watanabe, Y. (2011). Neuron 69: 359-372.
14 Wang ,Z., Zang, C., Cui, K., Schones, D.E., Barski, A., Peng, W. & Zhao,
K. (2009). Genome-wide mapping of HATs and HDACs reveals distinct
functions in active and inactive genes. Cell 138: 1019–1103.
32 Cocington, H.E., Maze, I., Sun, H., Bomze, H.M., DeMaio, K., Wu, E.Y.,
Dietz, D.M., Lobo, M.K., Ghose, S., Mouzon, E., Neve, R.L., Tamminga,
E.J. & Nestler, E.J. (2011). A role for repressive histone methylation in
cocaine-induced vulnerability to stress. Neuron 71: 656-670.
15 Cedar, H. & Bergman, Y. (2009). Linking DNA methylation and histone
modification: patterns and paradigm. Nature Reviews Genetics 10:
295-304.
16 Fabian, M.R., Sonenberg, N. & Filipowicz, W. (2010). Regulation of
mRNA Translation and Stability by microRNAs. Annual Review of
Biochemistry 79: 351-379.
17 Cannell, I.G., Kong, Y.W. & Bushell, M. (2008). How do microRNAs
regulate gene expression? Biochemical Society Transactions 36:
1224-1231.
18 Zhou, X., Wang, G., Sutoh, K., Zhu, J.K. & Zhang, W. (2008)
Identification of cold-inducible microRNAs in plants by transcriptome
analysis. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta, 1779: 780–788.
19 Fineberg, A.P. & Tycko, B. (2004). The history of cancer epigenetics.
Nature Reviews Cancer 4: 143-153.
20 Margueron, R. & Reinberg, D. (2010). Chromatin structure and the
inheritance of epigenetic information. Nature Reviews Genetics 11:
285-296.
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
33 Logrip, M.L., Janak, P.H. & Ron, D. (2009). Escalating ethanol intake
is associated with altered corticostriatal BDNF expression. Journal
Neurochemistry 109: 1459-1468.
34 Magklara, A., Yen, A., Colguitt, B.M., Clowney, E.J., Allen, W.,
Markenscoff-Papadimitriou, E., Evans, Z.A., Kheradpour, P.,
Mountufaris, G., Carey, C., Barnea, G., Kellis, M. & Lomvardas, S.
(2011). An epigenetic signature for monoallelic olfactory receptor
expression. Cell 145: 555-570.
35 Novikova, S.I, He, F., Bai, J., Cutrufello, N.J., Lidow, M.S. & Undieh,
A.S. (2008). Maternal cocaine administration in mice altered DNA
methylation and gene expression in hippocampal neurons of
neonatal and prepubertal offspring, PloS One 3: e1919.
36 Gapp, K., Jawaid, A., Sarkies, P., Bohacek, J., Pelczar, P., & Prados,
J. et al. (2014). Implication of sperm RNAs in trans-generational
inheritance of the effects of early trauma in mice. Nature
Neuroscience 17: 667-669.
19
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
37 Higgins, J.P.T. & Green, S. (editors). Cochrane Handbook for
Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0.
38 Wright, R.W., Brand, R.A., Dunn, W. & Spindler, K.P (2007). How
to write a systematic review. Clinical Orthopaedics And Related
Research 455: 23-29.
39 Waddington, C.H. (1942). The epigenoyupe. Endeavour 1: 18-20.
40 Frommer, M., Paul, C.L., Harrison, J. & Clark, S.J. (1994). High
sensitivity mapping of methylated cytosines. Nucleic Acids Research
22: 2990-2997.
41 Dias, B., & Ressler, K. (2013). Parental olfactory experience influences
behavior and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nature
Neuroscience 17: 89-96.
42 Finegersh, A., & Homanics, G. (2014). Paternal Alcohol Exposure
Reduces Alcohol Drinking and Increases Behavioral Sensitivity to
Alcohol Selectively in Male Offspring. Plos ONE, 9.
43 Franklin, T., Russig, H., Weiss, I., Gräff, J., Linder, N., & Michalon, A.
et al. (2010). Epigenetic Transmission of the Impact of Early Stress
Across Generations. Biological Psychiatry, 68: 408-415.
44 Vassoler, F., White, S., Schmidt, H., Sadri-Vakili, G., & Pierce, R.
(2012). Epigenetic inheritance of a cocaine-resistance phenotype.
Nature Neuroscience 16: 42-47.
45 Rassoluzadgan, M., Grandjean, V., Gounon, P., Vincent, S., Gillot, I.
& Cuzin, F. (2006). RNA-mediated non-mendelian inheritance of an
epigenetic change in the mouse. Nature 441: 469-474.
46 Vanderhaeghen, P., Schurmans, S., Vassart, G. & Parmentier, M.
(1997). Specific repertoire of olfactory receptor genes in the male
germ cells of several mammalian species. Genomics 39: 239-46.
47 Urdinguio, R.G., Sanchez-Mut, J.V. & Esteller, M. (2009). Epigenetic
mechanisms in neurological diseases: Genes, syndromes, and
therapies. Lancet Neurol 8: 1056-1072.
48 McGill, B.E., Bundle, S.F, Yaylaoglu, M.B., Carson, J.P, Thaller,
C. & Zoghbi, H.Y. (2006). Enhanced anxiety and stress-induced
corticosterone release are associated with increased Crh expression
in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Science USA 103: 18267-18272.
49 Bale, T.L & Vale, W.W. (2003). Increased depression-like behaviours
in corticotropin-releasing factor receptor-2-deficient mice: sexually
dichotomous responses. Journal of Neuroscience 23: 5295-5301.
50 Pelleymounter, M.A., Joppa, M., Ling, N. & Foster, A.C. (2002).
Pharmacological evidence supporting a role for central corticotropinreleasing factor(2) receptors in behavioural, but not endocrine,
response to environmental stress. Journal Pharmacology and
Experimental Therapeutics 302: 145-152.
51 Pelleymounter, M.A., Joppa, M., Ling, N. & Foster, A.C. (2004).
Behavioral and neuroendocrine effects of the selective CRF2 receptor
agonists urocortin II and urocortin III, Peptides 25: 659-666.
52 Isles, A. R. (2015). Neural and Behavioral Epigenetics; What it is, and
What is Hype. Genes, Brain and Behavior 14: 64-72.
2015
2016
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
56 Bailey, K.R. & Crawley, J.N. (2009). Anxiety-Related Behaviors in
Mice. In: Buccafusco J.J., editor. Methods of Behavior Analysis in
Neuroscience. 2nd ed. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press.
57 Bourin, M. & Hascoet, M. (2003). The mouse light/dark box test.
European Journal of Pharmacology 463: 55-65.
58 Braff, D; Stone, C; Callaway, E; Geyer, M; Glick, I; Bali, L (1978).
"Prestimulus effects on human startle reflex in normals and
schizophrenics". Psychophysiology 15: 339–343.
59 Bustin, S.A. & Nolan, T. (2004) Pitfalls of Real-Time ReverseTranscription Polymerase Chain Reaction. Journal of Biomolecular
Technology 15: 155-166.
60 Caldji, C., Diorio, J. & Meaney, M.J. (2000). Variations in maternal care
in infancy regulate the development of stress reactivity. Biological
Psychiatry 48: 1164-1174.
61 Can, A., Dao, D.T., Arad, M., Terrillion, C.E., Piantadosi, S.C. & Gould,
T.D. (2012a). The Mouse Forced Swim Test. Journal of Visualized
Experiments 59: 3638-3645.
62 Can, A., Dao, D. T., Terrillion, C. E., Piantadosi, S. C., Bhat, S., &
Gould, T. D. (2012b). The Tail Suspension Test. Journal of Visualized
Experiments 59: 3769-3774.
63 Crabbe, J., Phillips, T.J. & Belknap, J.K. (2010). The Complexity of
Alcohol Drinking: Studies in Rodent Genetic Models. Behavioural
Genetics 40: 737-750.
64 Deacon, R.M.J. (2013). Measuring Motor Coordination in Mice.
Journal of Visualized Experiments 75: 2069-2077.
65 Green, A.S. & Grahame, N.J. (2007). Reprogramming of genetic
networks during initiation of Fetal Alcohol Syndrome. Developmental
Dynamics 236: 613-631.
66 Hackett, J.A., Sengupta, R., Zylicz, J.J., Murakami, K., Lee, C., Down, T. &
Surani, M.A. (2012). Germline DNA demethylation dynamics and imprint
erasure through 5-hydroxymethylcytosine. Science 339: 448-452.
67 Huang, Y., Pastor, W.A., Shen, Y., Tahiliani, M., Liu, D.R. & Rao,
A. (2010) The behaviour of 5-hydroxymethylcytsoine in bisulfite
sequencings. PLoS 5: e8888
68 Jaluria, P., Konstantopoulos, K., Betenbaugh, M. & Shiloach, J. (2007).
A perspective on microarrays: current applications, pitfalls, and
potential uses. Microbial Cell Factories 6: 4.
69 Kamangar, F. (2012). Confounding variables in epidemiologic studies:
basics and beyond. Archives of Iranian Medicine 8: 508-516.
70 Kendler, K.S., Jacobson, K.B., Prescott, C.A. & Neale, M.C. Specificity
of genetic and environmental risk factors for use and abuse/
dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives,
stimulants, and opiates in male twins. American Journal of Psychiatry
160: 687-695.
71 Koob, G.F., Kenneth Lloyd, G. & Mason, B.J. (2009). Development of
pharmacotherapies for drug addiction: a Rosetta Stone approach.
Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 8: 500-515.
53 Charney, D.S., Nestler, E.J., Sklar, P. & Buxbaum, JD. (2013).
Neurobiology of Mental Illness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
72 Liu, D., Diorio, J., Day, J.C., Francis, D.D., Meaney, M.J. (2000). Maternal
care, hippocampal synaptogenesis and cognitive development in
rats. Natural Neuroscience 3: 799-806.
54 Allis, C.D., Jenuwein, T. & Reinberg, D. (2007). Epigenetics. 23-62.
New York: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press.
73 Miranda, T.B. & Jones, P.A. (2007). Journal of Cell Physiology 213:
384-390.
55 Anagnostaras. S.G, Josselyn, S.A., Frankland, P.W. & Silva, A.J. (2000).
Computer-Assisted Behavioural Assessment of Pavlovian Fear
Conditioning in Mice. Learning & Memory 7: 58-72.
74 Misra. S. (2012) Randomized double blind placebo control studies,
the “Gold Standard” in intervention based studies. Indian Journal of
Sexually Transmitted Diseases 33: 131-134.
20
This article is available in: http://www.clinical-epigenetics.imedpub.com/
Journal of Clinical Epigenetics
ISSN 2472-1158
75 Olek, A., Oswald, J. & Walter, J. (1996). A modified and improved
method for bisulfite based cytosine methylation analysis. Nucleic
Acids Research 24: 5064-5066.
76 Paylor, R., Spencer, C.M., Yuva-Paylor, L.A. & Pieke-Dahl, S. (2006).
The use of behavioural test batteries, II: Effect of test interval.
Physiology & Behaviour 87: 95-102.
77 Pellow, S. & File, S.E. (1986). Anxiolytic and anxiogenic drug effects on
exploratory activity in an elevated plus-maze: A novel test of anxiety
in the rat. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behaviour 24: 525-529.
78 Rustay, R., Wahlsten, D. & Crabbe, J.C. (2003). Assessment of genetic
susceptibility to ethanol intoxication in mice. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Science USA 100: 2917-2922.
79 Smith, C.J. & Osborn, A.M. (2009) Advantages and limitations of
quantitative PCR (Q-PCR)-based approaches in microbial ecology.
FEMS Microbiology Ecology 67: 6-20.
80 Teif, V., Beshnova, D.A., Vainshtein, Y., Marth, C., Mallm, J., Höfer,
T. & Rippe, K. (2014). Nucleosome repositioning links DNA (de)
© Under License of Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
2015
Vol. 1 No. 1:3
methylation and differential CTCF binding during stem cell
development. Genome Research 24: 1285–1295.
81 Turner B (2001) ChIP with Native Chromatin: Advantages and
Problems Relative to Methods Using Cross-Linked Material. In:
Mapping Protein/DNA Interactions by Cross-Linking [Internet]. Paris:
Institut national de la santé et de la recherche médicale.
82 Ward ID, Zucchi, F.C.R., Robbins, J., Falkenberg, E.A., Olson, D.M.,
Benzies, K. & Metz, G.A. (2013). Trans-generational programming
of maternal behaviour by prenatal stress. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth
12: S9.
83 Weaver, I.C.G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F.A., D’Alessio, A.C., Sharma,
S., Seckl, J.R., Dymov, S., Szyf, M. & Meaney, M. (2004). Epigenetic
programming by maternal behaviour. Nature Neuroscience 7: 847854.
84 Yu, M., Hon, G.C., Szulwach, K.E., Song, C., Jin, P Ren, B., He, C. (2012).
Tet-assisted bisulfite sequencing of 5-hydroxymethylcytosine. Nature
Protocols 7: 21-59.
21