* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download 10/4/06 version
Introduced species wikipedia , lookup
Theoretical ecology wikipedia , lookup
Ecological fitting wikipedia , lookup
Renewable resource wikipedia , lookup
Plant breeding wikipedia , lookup
Storage effect wikipedia , lookup
Plant defense against herbivory wikipedia , lookup
Biological Dynamics of Forest Fragments Project wikipedia , lookup
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 *> > ---------> CHAPTER 2 > > 4.4 Alteration in environment > I have found the good story behind Celtis - Hackberry is what > they call it in these papers by MAK Lodhi. You asked for an expansion. > "In bottomland forest in Oklahoma, Lodhi (1975, 1978) has > convincingly argued for a spectrum of allelopathic effects. > Observations of five major tree species present suggested > that Ulmus americana (elm) had the richest ground flora while > Celtis laevigata (Hackberry) had the poorest or even none. > Two oak species (Quercus alba, white oak and Q. rubra, red > oak) and an Acer (sycamore) had intermediate richness in > their subcanopy herb floras. > Lodhi showed that the leaf litters leached a series of > compounds into the topsoil, principally p-coumarin and > ferulic acid. These were shown to inhibit germination of the > grasses Bromus japonicus (brome grass ) and Andropogon > scoparium. Moreover, the concentration of the allelopathic > compounds was highest during leaf decomposition and declined > thereafter. > Hackberry litter decomposed fastest, providing the most toxic > seedbed during early spring, while the oaks and sycamore > leaves decomposed more slowly, and the soil toxicity was less > but lasted longer. Other interspecific subcanopy variation in > soils and nutrient cycling were, with allelopathy, sufficient > to account for the observed differences in subcanopy herb > vegetation. " > Lodhi, M.A.K. 1975 Soil-plant toxicity and its possible > significance in patterning of herbaceous vegetation in > bottomland forest. American Journal of Botany 62,612-622. > ----------------1977 The influence and comparison of > individual forest trees on soil properties and possible > inhiobition of nitrification due to intact vegetation. Ibid > 64, 260-264. > ----------------1978 Allelopathic effects of decaying litter > of dominant trees and their associated soil in a lowland > forest community. Ibid 65, 340-?? > > It seems I forgot to note the Acer species and the pagination > of last ref. > Can't have everything. Will correct. [‘found’] The nature of the plant community: a reductionist view 45 46 J. Bastow Wilson Botany Department, University of Otago, P.O. Box 56, Dunedin, New Zealand. 47 48 Andrew D.Q. Agnew Institute of Biological Sciences, University of Wales Aberystwyth, SY23 3DA, U.K. 49 Chapter 2: Interactions between species 50 1 Introduction ................................................................................................................... 2 51 52 53 54 55 56 2 Interference: negative effects between plants ............................................................... 4 2.1 Competition ........................................................................................................... 5 2.2 Factors for which competition occurs ................................................................... 5 2.3 Cumulative effects of competition ........................................................................ 8 2.4 Allelopathy .......................................................................................................... 11 2.5 Spectral Interference (red/far-red) ....................................................................... 13 Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 2 of 47 57 3 Parasitism .................................................................................................................... 14 58 59 60 61 62 4 Subvention: positive effects between plants ............................................................... 14 4.1 Water and nutrient redistribution ........................................................................ 15 4.2 Shelter.................................................................................................................. 16 4.3 ‘Talking trees’ ..................................................................................................... 18 4.4 Subvention conclusion ........................................................................................ 18 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 5 Litter: the necessary product ....................................................................................... 19 5.1 The timing and type of litter production ............................................................. 20 5.2 Litter decomposition ........................................................................................... 22 5.3 Effects of litter ..................................................................................................... 22 5.4 Alteration in environment ................................................................................... 23 5.5 Control by litter of nutrient cycling..................................................................... 24 5.6 Peat formation ..................................................................................................... 25 5.7 Can reaction via litter evolve? ............................................................................. 27 71 72 73 74 6 Autogenic disturbance: plants as disturbers ................................................................ 28 6.1 Movement and contact ........................................................................................ 29 6.2 Growth and gravity.............................................................................................. 31 6.3 Lianas and epiphytes ........................................................................................... 32 75 76 77 78 79 7 Plant-plant interactions mediated by heterotrophs ...................................................... 33 7.1 Below-ground benefaction .................................................................................. 33 7.2 Pollination ........................................................................................................... 34 7.3 Herbivory ............................................................................................................ 35 7.4 Diseases ............................................................................................................... 39 80 81 82 83 8 Interactions .................................................................................................................. 41 8.1 Litter/herbivore interactions ................................................................................ 41 8.2 Litter/fire interactions .......................................................................................... 42 8.3 Higher-order interactions .................................................................................... 42 84 85 9 Conclusion ................................................................................................................... 44 86 1 Introduction 87 Our reductionist plan was to start by considering the nature of plants, their ecesis and 88 their place in the community. Once the plant, with its colonial, and thus plastic structure, is in 89 place, it alters the environment around it, the reaction of Clements. This reaction of every 90 plant, specific to each species by definition, forms a nexus of relationships within the 91 community. In this chapter we provide an inventory of the reactions possible, as they affect 92 interspecific relationships. These are mechanisms available for the development and 93 organisation of communities, and if any commonality and structure is to be recognised, 94 reactions must be involved. Plant reactions can be important at all scales from 95 geomorphological processes down to contiguous leaves and root hairs. We have attempted to 96 identify and categorise the major reactions, but the scope is enormous and there must be many 97 that are yet to be described. Plants also interact via heterotrophs: one plant affecting the 98 population of a heterotroph which then affects, positively or negatively, another plant. Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 3 of 47 99 Sometimes plants interact directly, most dramatically in parasitism or in strangulation. 100 We consider first the relations between plants caused by reaction: interference 101 (negative effects) and subvention (positive effects). Parasitism is not caused by environmental 102 reaction, and we must consider it separately. We next describe the ecosystem properties of 103 plants as structural entities, that is as litter producers (supporting decomposer systems as well 104 as causing interference, subvention) and as autogenic disturbers (some categories of which are 105 caused by litter). These represent general ecosystem properties of plants as autotrophs. Plants 106 have many interactions with heterotrophs, in fact plants are almost the sole basis for the 107 existence of heterotrophs and plants in turn rely on heterotrophs for many functions. 108 However, we consider here only those effects that cause plant-plant interactions. Finally there 109 are infinite possibilities of interactions between reactions; we confine ourselves to discussing 110 herbivory and fire because these constitute ecosystem (often geomorphological) structural 111 forces. 112 There are classifications of interactions between organisms. Occasionally their labels 113 are easy to apply, though ‘commensalism’ and ‘amensalism’ are found much more frequently 114 in textbooks than in real scientific work. In other cases, application is problematic. Take an 115 experiment on competition. This can be set up to examine a replacement comparison, or an 116 additive comparison, or both (Fig. 2.1). Suppose species A has a somewhat stronger Monoculture of A A A A B Replacement Comparison (de Wit) Additive Comparison B Mixture A B B A Mixture B Monoculture of B Fig. 2.1. Replacement and additive comparisons in experiments that examine the interaction between species. 117 competitive ability than species B. If we ask about gains and losses (+ and –) as textbook 118 tables do, the question is gain or loss relative to what? In the replacement comparison (i.e. 119 comparison with monocultures at the same overall density, the de Wit replacement design: 120 Fig. 2.1), A will gain and B will lose: a +– situation. Yet in an additive comparison (i.e. Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 4 of 47 121 comparison in which each species has the same density in monoculture as in mixture), both 122 will suffer competition to some extent: a – – situation. This looks at first sight to be an 123 artefactual distinction of experimental design, but it applies equally to questions in the field. 124 This leads to misunderstandings between animal and plant ecologists, who are really quite 125 different species (though they have been known to interbreed). Animal ecologists think in 126 terms of adding one species to another. However, the de Wit replacement design (Harper 127 1977) is so entrenched in the mind of most plant ecologists that they will expect that in 128 competition one plant will gain and the other lose1. Often ‘competition’ is written of as if it were the only negative interaction between 129 130 plants, ‘mutualism’ is used without any evidence that the relations are mutual, litter effects are 131 considered only for their effects on nutrient cycling and autogenic disturbance is ignored as an 132 interaction type. We hope to redress those imbalances. 133 2 Interference: negative effects between plants 134 The literature is confused on ‘interference’. Generally plant ecologists regard 135 competition as one kind of interference, whilst animal ecologists regard interference as one 136 kind of competition. Our usage comes from a definition of competition that recognises the 137 precedence of Clements et al. (1929) and conforms to Begon et al. (1996; section 2.1 below). 138 Even when terms have been defined they have often been used carelessly. For example, 139 ‘competitive ability’ and ‘competitive exclusion’ have been used when there is no evidence 140 that the negative effects were exclusively, or even mainly, due to competition. When the 141 overall effect is negative we shall use ‘interference’, even though subvention, i.e. positive 142 influences, may be present reducing the effect of interference sensu stricto. Of course, when 143 we are discussing models or processes of competition, or when we are citing authors who 144 claim to be, we shall write ‘competition’. When the overall effect is positive we shall use 145 ‘subvention’ (section 4), even though interference may be present reducing the positive 146 effects. 147 The five kinds of interference all depend on reaction (Box 2.1). We include a switch 148 as a type of interference because it is reaction in an environmental (i.e. non-resource) factor 149 and by definition it has a relatively negative effect on (an)other species. Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 5 of 47 150 Box 2.1. Types of plant-plant interference. 151 competition = via removal of resources from the environment: sects. 2.1-2.3 152 allelopathy = via toxic substances: sect 2.4 153 spectral interference: sect. 2.6 154 switch = via reaction on an environmental (i.e. non-resource) factor: chap. 3, sect. 3 155 negative litter effects: sect. 5 156 157 2.1 Competition We use the definition: “Competition is an interaction between individuals, brought 158 about by a shared requirement for a resource in limited supply, leading to a reduction in the 159 survivorship, growth and/or reproduction of at least some of the competing individuals 160 concerned” (Begon et al. 1996), which is very close to: “The tendency of neighbouring plants 161 to utilise the same quantum of light, ion of mineral nutrient [or] molecule of water …”2 162 (Grime’s 2001). The rôle of limiting resources was emphasised by Clements et al. (1929 pp. 163 316-317): “Competition is a question of the reaction of the plant upon the physical factors that 164 encompass it and of the effect of these modified factors upon the adjacent plants. In the exact 165 sense, two plants do not compete as long as the water-content and nutrients, the heat and light 166 are in excess of the needs of both. The moment, however, that the roots of one enter the area 167 from which the other draws its water supply, or the foliage of one begins to overshade the 168 leaves of the other, the reaction of the former modifies unfavorably the factors controlling the 169 latter, and competition is at once initiated”, and later: “When the immediate supply of a single 170 necessary factor falls below the combined demands of the plants, competition begins”. Note 171 his emphasis that all competition is via reaction. 172 Here we can consider only selected topics out of the many that have occupied plant 173 ecologists since 1929. The suggestion that there is no observable competition in existing 174 communities is covered in chapter 6 (sect. 9.3), and whether interference ability is circular or 175 transitive in chapter 4 (sect. 4). 176 2.2 Factors for which competition occurs 177 Competition by definition occurs for resources, but what are resources? They are 178 molecules or types of energy necessary for plant growth/maintenance, that can be absorbed by 179 either one plant or another, but not both. This clearly includes light, C (CO2), water, 180 macronutrients (NPK) and micronutrients (Ca, S, Mg, etc.). What about radiant heat and soil 181 O2? These matters are difficult at the edges. 182 Light is the resource for which competition has most often been analysed, and we Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 6 of 47 183 shall refer to it several times. It differs from most other resources in that it is available 184 instantaneously, disappearing if not used, and it is directional in source (e.g. section 2.3). 185 Competition for mineral nutrients, mainly N, P and K, is well established by experiments in 186 pots and in the field (Clements et al. 1929; J.B. Wilson and Newman 1987). The main issue is 187 that because N is more mobile in soils than P it is possible for two plants to come into 188 competition for N before they compete for P (Bray 1954). The mobility of K is intermediate. 189 Competition could in theory occur for the other essential elements. 190 The rôle of competition for water between trees and their understorey is well known, 191 particularly in savannah. This can be two-way, the trees reducing the growth of the 192 understorey and vice-versa (Knoop and Walker 1985). Anderson et al. (2001) found that 193 although positive effects of the canopy of Prosopis glandulosa (mesquite) subvent the 194 survival of saplings below the canopy, competition for water develops as they age. 195 Robberecht et al. (1983) found that in the Sonoran Desert removing all other plants 196 surrounding a tussock of Hilaria rigida increased the water potential which led to the plants 197 remaining green for longer into the dry season and growing more. Perez-Salicrup and Barker 198 (2000) found that after lianas had been removed from seasonally-dry tropical forest in Brazil 199 the water potential of trees increased and they grew faster but the authors did not discuss how 200 much of this was due to the obvious increase in light due to removal of the liana canopy. Like 201 mineral nutrients, but in contrast to light, soil water remains available for some time if not 202 absorbed, though not indefinitely. 203 Competition could theoretically occur for the carbon source, CO2. Plants can reduce 204 the CO2 levels around themselves through photosynthesis, and this could affect the growth of 205 neighbours (Oliver and Schreiber 1974). However, there is also production of CO2 by 206 respiration of soil and litter (Bazzaz and McConnaughay 1992) and considerable air mixing 207 (Reicosky 1989). This means depletion of CO2 within a canopy may be uncommon, but it has 208 been recorded from dense canopies. Bazzaz and McConnaughay (1992) recorded a decrease 209 by 9 ppmv (parts per million by volume) compared to that above the canopy in Abutilon 210 theoprasti (velvet-leaf) and Buchmann et al. (1996) recorded a decrease of up to 26 ppmv in 211 daytime in the understorey of temperate forests with an open tree canopy and a dense field 212 layer. Species do differ greatly in their effects on the CO2 regime, e.g. Reicosky (1989) found 213 daily minimum CO2 levels differed between crops Zea mays (maize) and Glycine max 214 (soybean) by 5 ppmv. Most species show growth responses to CO2 concentration over this 215 range (Hunt et al. 1991). Competition for CO2 is probably more important than has usually 216 been assumed. Grime (2001) did not list CO2 as one of the resources for which competition 217 would occur, though Clements et al. (1929) did. Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 7 of 47 218 Ambient temperature is a rate-regulator, not a resource, but Clements et al. (1929) 219 suggested that plants may compete for radiant heat. They also suggested that plants could 220 compete for soil O2 in waterlogged conditions. In a sense it is a resource because a molecule 221 absorbed by one plant means it is not available for another, but it is related to redox potential, 222 which seems more like an environmental factor. 223 Ecologists often write of competition for physical space. When they use ‘space’ as 224 shorthand for all resources this is justifiable, though it misleads the young and impressionable 225 and obscures the differences between resources in their mechanisms of competition3. 226 However, sometimes writers have explicitly seen space as a resource separate from light, 227 water, NPK, etc., e.g. “competition is defined as ‘the tendency of neighbouring plants to 228 utilize the same quantum of light, ion of a mineral nutrient, molecule of water, or volume of 229 space’” (Grime 1979, 2001); “weeds competing for light and space in the first year of growth, 230 rather than moisture or nutrient stress” (Sage 1999). Grist (1999) attempted to model “plant 231 competition for light and space”. Yodzis (1986) envisaged that: “competition for space is so 232 different from what we normally think of as consumptive competition that it makes more 233 sense … to think of it as a completely different category of competition. Certainly space is 234 quite different from any other resource”. In these cases space should be equivalent to volume, 235 though we suspect that most authors have soil surface area in mind. Chiarucci et al. (2002) 236 actually measured the percentage volume occupancy of eight plant communities (four in each 237 of Italy and NZ, comprising four grasslands and four shrublands), the first time this had been 238 done. They found that only 0.44 to 2.89 % of the available volume within the canopy was 239 occupied by plant tissue and concluded: “physical space is probably never limiting by itself in 240 terrestrial higher-plant communities, so that competition for space, distinct from competition 241 for resources such as light, water and nutrients, is not likely to exist”. Clements (1916, 72) 242 understood this of course: “In a few cases, such as occur when radish seeds are planted too 243 closely, it is possible to speak of mechanical competition or competition for room. … 244 However [this] seems to have no counterpart in nature. There is no experimental proof of 245 mechanical competition between root-stocks in the soil, and no evidence that their relation is 246 due to anything other than competition for the usual soil factors – water, air and nutrients.” 247 Actually, there was no evidence for almost 80 years that it occurs between close radishes, and 248 even then the effect is very small (J.B. Wilson et al. in prep). Even Tilman (1982), though 249 devoting a chapter to competition for space, is careful to note that physical space “may be 250 irrelevant”. He notes that disturbance can create open space, but that “it would seem better to 251 study explicitly the resources supplied by disturbance”. We agree. 252 It is often forgotten the competitive abilities of the plant are determined not by its Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 8 of 47 253 characters in monoculture but by those changed by plasticity in interference. This is well- 254 known above-ground, for example in the process of etiolation (Bradshaw 2006). Collins and 255 Wein (2000) found that two Polygonum species (arrowleaf tearthumb and smartweed) 256 increased in internode length when crowded by plants of the same two species. Plasticity 257 occurs in less visible characters too. For example, Huber-Sannwald et al. (1996) found that 258 two steppe grasses differed markedly in their plastic change of specific root length, and 259 Bookman and Mack (1982) found by double-labelling of shoots that when Bromus tectorum 260 (downy brome) was growing in mixture with Poa pratensis (meadow grass) its root system 261 was constricted laterally, but extended slightly deeper. 262 2.3 Cumulative effects of competition 263 The term ‘asymmetric interference’ (sometimes as the more limited term ‘asymmetric 264 competition’) is used in two senses. Firstly, it can mean that the interference effect of Species 265 A on Species B is different from that of B on A. Perhaps A suppresses B, but B has no effect 266 on A, or the difference can be quantitative: more effect of A on B than of B on A (e.g. 267 Roxburgh and Wilson 2000a; Ives and Hughes 2002). This would be expected since species 268 by definition differ in characters and their interference effect is therefore very likely to be 269 different, but the degree of A/B asymmetry can be of interest as a feature of the community 270 matrix and hence of the community (chap. 3, sect. 7.3). This concept can be applied to 271 individual genotypes too. 272 The second meaning of ‘asymmetric interference is not so easy to define. Wedin and 273 Tilman (1993) synonymise it with ‘resource preemption’, apparently implying an advantage 274 to the first plant to arrive. For Schwinning and Fox (1995) it implies that “large plants greatly 275 suppress the growth of smaller neighbors”, and more precisely for Weiner et al. (1997) that 276 “larger plants are able to obtain a disproportionate share of the resources (for their relative 277 size) and suppress the growth of smaller individuals”. It has therefore been termed ‘size 278 asymmetry’ to distinguish it from A/B asymmetry. The problem is what is a plant, so what is 279 size? If a large grass tussock divides into two small, physiologically-independent ones, is it no 280 longer large? Many authors have reached the conclusion that initial differences are magnified 281 into a large effect, causing exclusion by interference. There is truth in this, but it is too 282 superficial because it ignores the mechanism. 283 The classic study is that of Black (1958). He established swards of Trifolium 284 subterraneum (subterranean clover), using large seeds (mean mass 10.0 mg) and small seeds 285 (mean mass 4.0 mg), all of cultivar Bacchus Marsh and therefore of near-identical genotype. 286 In monoculture, the two sizes of seed and a 50:50 mixture of them all produced essentially Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 9 of 47 287 identical sward mass throughout the experiment. However, in the mixture the initial seed 288 mass was crucial: plants from large seeds suffered no mortality and came to be 93 % of the 289 mixture biomass, whereas more than half of the plants from small seeds died and those 290 remaining contributed only 7 % of the biomass. The distribution of leaf area showed the 291 cause: plants from small seeds held their leaf area a little lower than those from large seeds. In 292 monoculture, t his did not matter, but in mixture they captured a small share of the resources 293 (disproportionately small for their relative leaf area), so that by the end of the experiment, 294 although they contributed 10 % of the leaf area it was held so low that they captured only 2 % 295 of the light. It is essential to notice that this result was obtained because of a specific 296 mechanism (Fig. 2.2): (a) in T. subterraneum greater growth results in the leaf area being held 297 higher, and (b) in competition for light the height of the leaf area is crucial. This gives 298 cumulative competition: the results of competition change the competitive abilities. Here, 299 ‘height asymmetry’ would be more appropriate than ‘size asymmetry’. 300 It cannot be assumed that competition for light will always be cumulative. Hirose and 301 Werger (1995) found that in competition for light the taller species of a wet meadow 302 community did not have as much advantage as expected from the higher position of their 303 leaves in the canopy because in order to achieve that height they had less of their biomass in 304 leaf area and more in stems, and Bernston and Wayne (2000) failed to find biomass size- 305 asymmetry in competition for light between Betula alleghaniensis (yellow birch) seedlings, 306 apparently because of height plasticity in small, shaded plants. Larger individual leaves Longer petioles Greater biomass Leaf area held higher Faster growth (RGR) Disproportionate percentage of the light captured Fig. 2.2. In Trifolium subteranneum, the results of competition affect competitive ability. 307 This effect would not be expected in below-ground competition, because there is no 308 common equivalent to the growth/height/light/growth positive feedback. A plant that is larger 309 with longer roots, and therefore able to access deep resources generally has no advantage. The Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 10 of 47 310 deep roots are hardly likely to ‘shade’ roots nearer the surface from NPK rising up through 311 the soil. In any case, nutrients are generally much more available near the surface, where 312 small plants can easily root. Uptake of water is sometimes from aquifers, but it is hard to 313 envisage a root shield comparable to the canopy aboveground. In terms of allometric growth, 314 if a plant is larger by ×2 in each dimension, it will be ×23 = ×8 larger in volume and in 315 biomass. Therefore, its NPK requirements will be ×8. However, its root system will cover an 316 area only ×22 = ×4 larger to cope with the ×8 demand. The large plant will be the one 317 deficient in nutrients. Thus, it seems reasonable that Rajaniemi (2003) could find only very 318 ambivalent evidence of size-asymmetry in below-ground interference, and all other 319 investigations have found none. In fact the larger plants have usually been at a disadvantage, 320 due to greater within-plant interference (J.B. Wilson 1988b; Weiner et al. 1997). 321 We could think of possible exceptions. When nutrients are available patchily, small 322 plants can preferentially grow in high-NPK areas, whereas a large plant might need to include 323 in its root system the nutrient-poor matrix, with actual disadvantage to the large plant. 324 However, if nutrients were available intermittently in time and space, such as animal 325 micturition patches, perhaps a large plant could take up enough to satisfy the whole of itself 326 from a few roots in the patch, whereas a small plant outside the patch could not reach in 327 before they disappear (Fig. 2.3). This argument applies only when the patches are transient, so 328 it seems reasonable that Blair (2001) could find no evidence that experimentally-imposed 329 spatial heterogeneity in nutrients led to size-asymmetric interference. This plant has some roots in the sheep-urine patch, and can access nutrients from there for the whole plant A sheep micturated here This plant has access This plant has no access Fig. 2.3: A possible mechanism for size-asymmetric competition belowground, when nutrient supplies become available patchily in time and space, e.g. from sheep micturition. 330 331 332 We conclude that cumulative interference, due to size-asymmetry, i.e. the effect of initial advantage, is not a necessary feature of plant interference. It will occur only when there Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 11 of 47 333 is a specific feedback mechanism, such as that via height/light, that results in competitive 334 success increasing competitive ability per unit biomass. The conditions under which it occurs 335 have huge implications for community structure, because if interference be cumulative 336 exclusion by interference will occur much more readily, and faster, and coexistence less 337 readily. 338 Competition is basically the same in plants and in animals. One owl eats a mouse, so 339 another cannot and perhaps dies. One plant takes up water from the soil, so another cannot 340 and perhaps dies. However, since animals are quite aplastic interference normally affects 341 density rather than individual size; plants are plastic, so competition affects plant size first, 342 before density. Nevertheless, interference often does result in some decrease in density, and 343 this has been generalised as an approach to a straight line on a plot of log (mean individual 344 plant weight) against log (density). The slope of such a line has been claimed from geometric 345 theory and from observations to be any of -3/2, -1/2 or -3/4 (Harper 1977; Torres et al. 2001). 346 When competition is for light and is cumulative, there is a particular reason to expect 347 mortality: a dominance hierarchy will result in which the small/short plants lag further and 348 further behind the large/tall ones, until they die (Black 1958). When competition is for 349 nutrients and water no such effect will be present (J.B. Wilson 1988b) so self-thinning should 350 be much less prominent and the -3/2 (etc.) relation may be absent. This has not been 351 investigated, probably because it would be difficult to impose realistic competition for 352 nutrients or water without there being competition for light. 353 Shoots generated vegetatively via rhizomes or stolons often arise at lower densities 354 than germinating seeds, potentially reducing interference, and mutual ramet support (Marshall 355 and Sagar 1968) should reduce mortality, perhaps eliminating self-thinning. However, self- 356 thinning mortality has been seen in these situations: in the culms of the Gynerium sagitatum 357 (uva grass; de Kroon and Kalliola 1995), in the tufted regeneration of two Sasa spp. (bamboo; 358 Makita 1996) and in shoots of Urtica dioica (nettle; Hara and Srutek 1995). Developmental 359 controls are certainly capable of suppressing bud development, as they do during tree growth, 360 so that we can only presume that its failure in clonal herbs has selective value, such as in the 361 maintenance of stands in the monospecific condition in spite of accidental death of individual 362 shoots. 363 2.4 Allelopathy 364 Allelopathy is a type of interference that, unlike competition, does not involve a 365 “shared resource in limited supply” (section 2.1). Rather, it is based on the environmental 366 reaction of toxin production. This toxin is either leached or volatilised from the living parts of Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 12 of 47 367 plants (above- or below-ground), from standing dead plants or from decomposing litter, and 368 then reduces the growth of neighbouring plants (Gliessman and Muller 1978; Kuiters 1991; 369 Inderjit et al. 1999; Kato-Noguchi 2004). Some ecologists believe that allelopathy is a 370 widespread and important process (Rice 1983; Inderjit et al. 1994). Others are sceptical 371 (Harper 1977). Most plants contain chemicals that are toxic to other plants, at least in the right 372 circumstances. It is very unlikely that the strong effects that can readily be demonstrated by 373 leaf elutants in petri dishes will happen in nature, but it is equally unlikely that soil bacteria 374 and colloids will completely neutralise the effect. Field experiments are difficult to perform 375 (Williamson 1990). 376 Most work has been with shoot elutants, but Mahall and Callaway (1991; 1992) 377 conducted greenhouse experiments between Larrea tridentata (creosote bush) and Ambrosia 378 dumosa (white bursage). The two species occur together in the Mojave Desert. Roots of A. 379 dumosa were inhibited when they came near to roots of L. tridentata. This effect was reduced 380 by activated carbon, so it was presumably via a chemical. The two species apparently have 381 similar water extraction capabilities (Yoder and Nowak 1999b), so this signalling would 382 enable to the plants to avoid each other and postpone the onset of competition. This may be a 383 more common phenomenon than is realised at present. More direct evidence for the 384 production of toxins from roots was obtained by Welbank (1963), but only after they had 385 started to decompose in anaerobic conditions. We allelopathic effects of litter further in 386 section 5.4. Interference can occur extremely early in the development of the plant 387 community, as seed-seed interference before the seeds emerge. This could be through 388 competition for water for imbibition and mucilaginous anchorage, but allelopathy is more 389 likely (Harries and Norrington-Davies 1977; Murray 1998). 390 Ecologists are inclined to think in teleological terms, of plants deliberately inhibiting 391 their neighbours. Although this is clearly not true, it leads to an assumption that allelopathic 392 ability has evolved as an adaptation: species that make their locale unfavourable for the 393 growth of other species will gain in selection. But can this evolve? If a mutant produces an 394 allelochemical against other species this will be at some cost to itself (many of the secondary 395 compounds involved are energetically expensive for the plant to make), yet unless the effect 396 is very local it will benefit non-mutant plants of the same species as much as itself, and the 397 trait will not evolve. It is then necessary to invoke group selection, a controversial and 398 probably weak force (J.B. Wilson 1987). This problem would not occur with selection for a 399 plant to protect itself, and thus eventually its species, against its own allelochemical. 400 Williamson (1990) commented that there would be equal selective pressure on the species’ 401 neighbours to develop defences, but this ignores the issue that a species always grows with Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 13 of 47 402 itself, but has a variety of neighbours against which it would have to protect itself. Newman 403 (1978) questioned whether selection was involved at all, finding in a literature survey that 404 auto-toxicity is as common as toxicity to other species. He argued that this indicates that 405 allelopathy is an incidental result of the production of secondary compounds. 406 2.5 Spectral Interference (red/far-red) 407 Plant canopies change the spectral quality of light that passes through them: another 408 type of plant reaction on the environment. Of particular importance for plant-plant interaction 409 is that the red/far-red ratio (R/FR) is lower in light that has passed through a leaf or been 410 reflected off one. It is therefore lower under canopies and somewhat lower in canopy gaps 411 than in the open (Turnbull and Yates 1993; Davis and Simmons 1994). This can decrease the 412 leaf chlorophyll content of plants in the lower strata and hence reduce their photosynthetic 413 capacity, leaf life, leaf area ratio, tiller production, relative growth rate and seed germination 414 (Barreiro et al. 1992; Dale and Causton 1992; Skinner and Simmons 1993; Rousseaux et al. 415 1996; Batlla et al. 2000). 416 Potential exists for assembly rules based on R/FR because the response depends on the 417 species; Leeflang (1999) found that only one species out of the six that they examined 418 responded to R/FR ratio in terms of biomass accumulation. Dale and Causton (1992) found 419 differences between three Veronica species (speedwell): the species that grew in deeper shade 420 responded more to R/FR. It can be difficult to separate effects of light intensity and spectrum, 421 but sometimes plants respond morphologically to the presence of neighbours even before they 422 are directly shaded, by sensing the change in R/FR in light that has been reflected off 423 neighbours (Schmitt and Wulff 1993). 424 R/FR may be less important for its direct effect on the growth of the recipient than as a 425 signal to the plant that competition is imminent. Some R/FR responses would increase 426 competitive ability for light: enhancing leaf elongation, making shoots and leaves more 427 upright and increasing the shoot:root ratio (Skinner and Simmons 1993; Vanhinsberg and 428 Vantienderen 1997). Other responses can help the plant avoid of competition by foraging for 429 light gaps: altered stem angle and longer internodes (Ballaré et al. 1995). Some plants sense 430 and grow away from each other, even at some distance. Manipulation of R/FR ratios, and 431 experimentation with pieces of plastic with the same spectral characteristics as plant leaves, 432 show this to be due to R/FR effects (Fig. 2.4; Novoplansky 1990). Germination can be 433 reduced under a canopy, but faster germination is also possible, and these R/FR responses can 434 be seen as competition avoidance and greater competitive effort, respectively (Dyer et al. 435 2000). R/FR is not everything: Muth and Bazzaz (2002) found that visible (PAR) light was Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 14 of 47 436 437 438 more important than R/FR in the gap-seeking of Betula papyrifera (birch) seedling. Very little is known of the processes causing the 3-dimensional positions of plant modules in a mixed-species canopy, but R/FR effects may be involved. 439 Experimental setup The relative frequency of shoots developing in different directions Fig. 2.4: The effect of R/FR light, as seen in surrounds of different colours. After Novoplansky (1990). 440 441 442 3 Parasitism There are partial (Loranthaceae, Viscaceae) and total shoot parasites (e.g. Cuscuta, 443 Cassytha), as well as partial (e.g. Rhinanthus spp.) and total root parasites (e.g. 444 Balanophoraceae, Orobanchaceae, Rafflesiaceae, Striga gesnerioides). These parasites can 445 reduce the growth of their hosts considerably, by 70 % with some root parasites of crops 446 (Graves et al. 1992; Press 1998). We are concerned with impact on the community, and the 447 result is often an increase in species diversity (Press 1998). It is not clear why. Perhaps the 448 parasite’s favourite hosts are the dominant species and a reduction in their productivity allows 449 subordinate species to flourish, possibly also reducing cumulative interference (section 2.3), 450 but it is not clear why parasites should favour particularly the dominants. Perhaps we notice 451 the effects more when they do. An increase in species diversity could be caused by non- 452 selective effects of parasites if whole-community productivity is reduced from high to 453 medium, allowing more species to coexist according to the humped-back theory of Grime 454 (1973), which is really another suggestion of reduced cumulative interference. 455 4 Subvention: positive effects between plants 456 Reaction does not necessarily result in interference, it can well result in subvention, 457 i.e. a positive effect by one species on the survival and/or growth of another. Gleason (1936) 458 gave subvention equal weight with negative interference, but whilst the interference between Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 15 of 47 459 species has been a focus of attention subvention has been neglected until recently (Lortie et al. 460 2004). We note that the existence of subvention by species A on species B does not imply that 461 they have co-evolved. It is simply a corollary of reaction. 462 Box 2.2. Types of subvention (positive effects of reaction) 463 mutualism (= synergy) = both species benefit relative to their being at the same density on 464 465 466 467 their own (i.e. an additive comparison), benefaction = one species benefits as above, with no known advantage or disadvantage to itself. facilitation = one species benefits another, to its disadvantage. 468 The terminology of subvention is confusing. We use the scheme in Box 2.2. 469 ‘Facilitation’ was used by Connell and Slatyer (1977) for Clements’ (1916) concept of the 470 mechanism of succession, but our usage is exactly parallel. There are many 471 environmental/resource factors that can be modified by plants to the benefit of others, and we 472 have surely not included them all (see Callaway 1995). We do consider subventions via litter 473 in section 5.4 and those via heterotrophs in section 7.1. Just as the plant community provides 474 habitats for heterotrophic organisms, so it augments the diversity of sites available for plants 475 by increasing environmental heterogeneity both horizontally and vertically, and we consider 476 this niche construction in chapter 4, section 1.3. 477 4.1 Water and nutrient redistribution 478 Plants intercept rainfall. Up to half of the input can be lost by evaporation from the 479 canopy (Cape et al. 1991) but input is occasionally increased by fog capture (chap. 3, sect. 480 5.4.B). Plants make soil water patchy in forests because of canopy gaps, leaf drip and 481 stemflow. In the clustered or striped vegetation (‘tiger bush’) typical of arid and semi-arid 482 areas (chap. 3, sect. 5.4.B), there can be similar benefaction by shrubs and trees of their 483 understorey. For example, litter can increase local infiltration into the soil by preventing 484 runoff, again increasing patchiness (Tongway et al. 2001). However, care must be taken in 485 interpreting these patterns. L.R. Walker et al. (2001) demonstrated by plant manipulations 486 that, whilst islands of soil fertility around woody plants can increase water and nutrient 487 availability, the net effect of nurse plants can be negative due to shading and/or root 488 competition. 489 Just as plants can redirect water from above, so can they redirect water from below. 490 One species with deep roots can take up water from deep layers in the soil, perhaps from an 491 aquifer, and release it in the surface layer where shallow-rooted species can absorb it: Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 16 of 47 492 ‘hydraulic lift’. This is likely to be important in arid climates and Facelli and Temby (2002) 493 show that in arid South Australia two chenopods, Atriplex vesicaria and Maireana sedifolia, 494 enhance the water relations of their surrounding annuals, though they also compete for light, 495 leading to a complex interaction. The phenomenon is also known in temperate forest where 496 Dawson (1993) and Emerman and Dawson (1996) have shown that Acer saccharum (sugar 497 maple) uplifts considerable quantities of water, much of which may be taken up by 498 understorey plants from near the exuding tree roots. In a nice variant of the story, the CAM 499 photosynthesising Yucca schidigera in the Mojave Desert apparently redistributes soil water, 500 creating a higher upper-soil water potential during the day rather than at night when it has 501 open stomata and a higher water demand (Yoder and Nowak 1999a). Surrounding non-CAM 502 plants with a high water demand must benefit. Hydraulic lift is benefaction or possibly 503 facilitation, and may be much more common than formerly supposed (Caldwell et al. 1998), 504 but most authors stress that soil particle size and structure can be critical in the process. Roots 505 can also distribute water down to deeper soil layers (Ryel et al. 2003). 506 Plants make nutrients patchy in the same way. Stemflow can be an important source of 507 nutrients for epiphytes, especially of N for which stemflow concentrations can exceed ten 508 times those in rainwater (Awasthi et al. 1995; Whitford et al. 1997). We suggest this as an 509 explanation of the trunk-base habitat of a few mosses such as Isothecium myosuroides in 510 Western Europe. Nutrients such as N can be leached from canopy leaves and absorbed by 511 plants below (McCune and Boyce 1992; Cappellato and Peters 1995). This is best categorised 512 as benefaction; although one organism loses and the other gains, there would be leakage from 513 the loser whether there was a recipient below or not. 514 These effects are good examples of subvention without there being any question of co- 515 evolution. 516 4.2 Shelter 517 518 There are many ways in which one plant can give shelter from stress. These are clearly reactions that can cause subvention. 519 Plants alter the physical environment around them in obvious ways: ambient 520 temperature, evapotranspiration and air movement. This is reaction. Canopy trees dampen 521 diurnal and seasonal fluctuations in subcanopy air temperature which is significant because 522 temperature stresses of heat load or frost can kill cells (Stoutjesdijk and Barkman 1992; Chen 523 et al. 1993). Daytime temperature in the field layer of a forest is generally 1-3 oC less than 524 above the canopy. Windspeed is lower, reducing transpirational load and the abrasion of 525 photosynthetic organs (section 6.1). Humidity is higher, reducing water stress. The same Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 17 of 47 526 effects are seen in other community types: Liancourt et al. (2005) gave evidence of 527 benefaction between species in a calcareous grassland due to amelioration of water stress. In 528 extreme environments some plants survive only through benefaction from another. Plant 529 species differ in their reaction on the microclimate (Castro et al. 1991) and in their response to 530 microclimate (Stoutjesdijk and Barkman 1992; Tretiach 1993), so these effects could drive or 531 modify community processes. 532 Such benefaction is especially important in arid and semi-arid areas, where isolated 533 shrubs and trees have a herbaceous flora on the fine soil and organic matter accumulated 534 beneath them. The association between nurse plants and their beneficients is often so clear 535 that no attempt is made to determine the precise environmental variable that is responsible, 536 but it is generally assumed to be temperature combined with humidity (e.g. Brittingham and 537 Walker 2000). Temperature differentials are easy to demonstrate and very well documented 538 (e.g. Suzán 1996; Godínez-Alvarez et al. 1999). In hot deserts with summer rainfall, shelter 539 from heat load can be critical in cell survival, especially for succulents which cannot lose as 540 much heat by convection as thin-leaved plants. The canopy shade also reduces evaporative 541 load. Shrub nurse effects are sometimes attributed to nutrient buildup and soil water-holding 542 capacity, but Gómez-Aparicio et al. (2005) found that in dry Spanish shrublands most of the 543 effect was due to canopy shade. Belsky et al. (1993) have meticulously described the complex 544 of interactions in East Africa, while Holzapfel and Mahall (1999) carefully separated 545 subvention from interference between Ambrosia dumosa (white bursage) shrubs and annuals 546 in the Mojave. Amiotti et al. (2000) show that isolated Pinus radiata trees in Argentina have 547 similar effects. In a fascinating series of subventions in western Texas, Prosopis velutina 548 (mesquite) canopy allows the establishment of a Juniperus pinchotii (juniper), while J. 549 pinchotii helps the establishment of three other woody species with a more mesic distribution 550 (Armentrout and Pieper 1988; McPherson et al. 1988). Taking the latter as indicator species, 551 there must be more mesic conditions of temperature and water relations under the canopies. 552 There could be plant-to-plant shelter from salt spray deposition (Malloch 1997), or 553 another plant’s salt spray load could be increased, but neither effect seems to have been 554 demonstrated. However, soil salinity benefaction has. Bertness and Hacker (1994) removed 555 Juncus gerardii (a rush) from a New England, USA, saltmarsh and showed that its presence 556 had raised the redox potential of the soil and, by reducing evapotranspiration, kept down the 557 soil salinity. Without this benefit, the associated Iva frutescens (marsh elder) died. 558 Transplants of J. gerardii to the low- and mid-zones of the marsh also benefited from the 559 presence of J. gerardii and/or I. frutescens neighbours because of lower salinities. However, 560 transplants into higher zones in the marsh were suppressed by the I. frutescens there, so this is Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 18 of 47 561 a one-way subvention, benefaction rather than mutualism. 562 In alpine and arctic habitats, krummholz trees can shelter shorter species from wind 563 desiccation, storm damage and wind-born ice particles (Marr 1977, Callaway 1998). Similar 564 subvention can occur amongst alpine herbs, possibly as a mutualism (e.g. Choler et al. 2001). 565 Shelter from frost heave was implicated when Ryser (1993) found that, of six species 566 examined in limestone grassland in Switzerland, two appeared to require the relief from both 567 frost heave and drought provided by neighbouring plants. Shelter from ultra violet light (UV- 568 B) is a possible type of subvention; in some species it damages cells and their photosynthetic 569 apparatus, and UV-B shielding from above could benefit plants. This would be especially 570 important in alpine communities with their higher UV-B. The transmittance of UV-B to lower 571 strata does differ with the canopy structure of different species (Shulski et al. 2004) and 572 species and ecotypes do react differently to UV-B in growth rates (Dixon et al. 2001; Robson 573 et al. 2003), so all the components for a benefaction seem to exist, but we can find no 574 evidence that it exists in nature. 575 576 Subvention via shelter is also unlikely to be the result of co-evolution. 4.3 ‘Talking trees’ 577 Plants can often increase their chemical defences when grazed. It has been suggested 578 that they can do this upon receiving a chemical signal such as methyl jasmonate released by 579 nearby grazed plants: the ‘talking trees’ mechanism. Although early work was plagued by 580 problems such as pseudoreplication, more careful work has validated the effect (Karban et al. 581 2000). The remaining puzzle is whether it can be a product of natural selection since it 582 benefits the neighbours of a plant (genotype) producing the signal, not the plant itself. It could 583 have evolved by kin selection (J.B. Wilson 1987) or between-plant signals could be an 584 incidental result of within-plant signalling (Karban and Maron 2002). 585 4.4 Subvention conclusion 586 One of the categories above is mutualism, and examples are well known between 587 plants and heterotrophs, for example in mycorrhizae. However, we have been able to discover 588 hardly any examples of demonstrated mutualisms between two embryophytes. We note that 589 the list above contains no clear example of facilitation either. This suggests that subventions 590 may be accidents of evolution. This would be expected anyway. Plants do not rise to the top 591 of the interaction league table by being nice to others. Probably facilitation during succession 592 is a combination of mechanisms, for example an N-fixing and litter-accumulating pioneer that 593 is shaded out by subsequent species. Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 19 of 47 594 595 5 Litter: the necessary product We described plants in chapter 1 as colonies of modules with a lifespan that is limited, 596 programmed and often curiously short. This is true even of translocatory (vascular) elements. 597 The discard of old modules and tissue comprises litter. The organs are mainly shed when 598 dead, but some material is lost to the living plant through damage. All types of plant organ are 599 included in litter, especially leaves, but also twigs, coarse woody debris (CWD, i.e. branches 600 and whole trees) and reproductive parts. Roots and underground stems also have limited life 601 and die belowground. There are probably subterranean interspecific effects of root litter but 602 for want of research on it we are writing here of above-ground litter. Considerable amounts of 603 litter can be deposited. Buck-Sorlin and Bell (1998) recorded 37,000 plant fragments from 604 one Quercus robur (oak) tree in a year; mostly short shoots up to two years old. Chambers et 605 al. (2000) recorded 3.6 tonnes ha-1 yr-1 (CWD only, >10 cm diameter) in an Amazonian rain 606 forest. Amounts are naturally less in more open forests in drier areas, for example c. 1-2 607 tonnes ha-1 yr-1 (total; mostly leaves) for two Eucalyptus savannahs in Queensland, Australia 608 (Grigg and Mulligan 1999). Christensen (1975) recorded only 0.77 tonnes ha-1 yr-1 (woody 609 litter only) in a Quercus robur (oak) forest in Denmark, probably because growth is slower in 610 the far north, and because it was a young forest with little CWD. 611 Litter production is a mandatory process in the life of plants and the ultimate fate of 612 most plant production. Most often, litter is examined for its rôle in nutrient cycling (e.g. 613 Orians et al. 1996), but it can play a major part in all aspects of community structure and 614 ecosystem function. It must change the plant’s immediate environment, i.e. cause a 615 Clementsian reaction, and we here discuss evidence for a range of its effects on species- 616 species interactions, and hence for the community processes of relay floristics, switches, 617 cyclic succession and even assembly rules. 618 There are two conceptual difficulties in this respect. The first is that although each 619 species must produce litter that is unique in its abundance, form, chemistry and phenology, 620 litter can subvent neighbours as well as increasing the fitness of the plant that produces it, and 621 if the litter is produced when the plant dies it cannot affect the producer’s fitness at all. Can it 622 then be considered as part of the plant’s environmental reaction? We shall discuss this 623 problem further in section 5.7. Secondly, litter produced above ground can vary in its mobility 624 and fate. It can be removed from its site of deposition by wind, floodwater or animals. It may 625 be buried by river gravels or wind-blown loess. If it breaks down more slowly than it is 626 deposited, the climate and substrate determine its ultimate fate. In moist conditions the litter 627 becomes peat (section 5.6). In a dry climate and/or on a dry substrate, accumulations of litter Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 20 of 47 628 629 must ultimately disappear by fire (section 8.2). These processes are indirect effects of litter. Faced with all these problems, we behave as biologists usually do, and try to classify 630 the material available. We categorise the phenology, morphology and fate of litter, then the 631 effects engineered by this important material, and finally we discuss whether litter production 632 or its effects can be susceptible to Darwinian selective processes. 633 5.1 The timing and type of litter production 634 The dead plant modules that comprise litter can be retained on the plant, discarded in 635 response to seasonal cues, or shed by accidental, environmental or herbivore damage. We call 636 these respectively standing dead litter, programmed litterfall and stochastic litterfall. We 637 distinguish them because we think that selection can act more easily on predictable events. 638 Standing dead litter 639 Litter can be retained on the plant for some time, decaying. This standing dead litter 640 includes not only dead wood but also partially-detached bark, flowering stems, and erect or 641 prostrate attached leaves. In many perennial herbs the vegetative shoots collapse in winter, 642 but the dead flowering stems remain erect and may have effects such as channelling rainfall or 643 acting as bird perches, with consequent input of nutrients and disseminules. Many annuals die 644 by programmed senescence. How such plant suicide evolves is something of a puzzle (J.B. 645 Wilson 1997), but anyway it can result in a plant-plant interaction. Bergelson (1990) 646 demonstrated in a field experiment that the seedling emergence of the annuals Senecio 647 vulgaris (groundsel) and Capsella bursa-pastoris (shepherd’s purse) was very sparse where 648 there was a high density of dead Poa annua plants. 649 Programmed litter 650 We define litter as programmed when it is disconnected by abscission, the result of 651 physiological processes that are endogenous to the plant and initiated some time before the 652 litter actual falls. It is almost always pulsed by season (e.g. Arianoutsou 1989; Enright 1999), 653 exceptions being tropical figs (Nason et al. 1998) and the whole-plant senescence of short- 654 lived ruderals in oceanic climates. There can be several peaks of programmed litterfall during 655 the year, often of different types of a species’ replaceable plant organs (leaves, twigs, flowers, 656 fruit, etc.) all with their special phenology (Figure 2.5). The signal for abscission can be 657 exogenous, e.g. daylength, or endogenous, e.g. the mutual shading of crowded twigs (Buck- 658 Sorlin and Bell 1998). Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 21 of 47 659 660 Fig. 2.5: The seasonal pattern of litterfall in an oakwood. After Christensen (1975). Programmed litter is usually lightweight, so it can be redistributed by wind and rain 661 into deep patches, perhaps having its greatest local effect some time after it first falls 662 (Fahnestock et al. 2000) (This applies to lightweight stochastic litter too.). In some species, 663 twig fall and branch fall (= branch shedding = limbfall = cladoptosis) are programmed with 664 well-defined cleavage zones, with a cork band produced from a phellogen (Millington and 665 Chaney 1973; Christensen 1975). Programmed wood fall generally peaks in autumn, but in a 666 few species in spring (Millington and Chaney 1973). Programmed woody litter usually 667 comprises twigs and small branches, but in some species, especially conifers, large branches 668 can be abscissed (e.g. up to 17 cm mid-branch diameter in the conifer Agathis australis: W.R. 669 Wilson et al. 1998). The process has been well described but its potential wider evolutionary 670 and synecological significance appears to have been overlooked. 671 Stochastic litterfall 672 Litter shed in response to disturbances that are allogenic to the plant is stochastic in 673 that it is not programmed, not initiated by the plant. Causes or triggers of stochastic litterfall 674 include storms, droughts, floods, geomorphological change, gravitational overload, herbivore 675 destruction and pathogenic dieback. Some of these stimuli occur irregularly, though others are 676 seasonal, for example equinoctial storms. The production of litter adds to the directly 677 disturbing effects of exceptional storms, frosts, droughts, etc. For stochastic litter to exert a 678 selective effect it must be frequent and non-lethal. 679 Stochastic litter can be dead or alive. Leaves are a major component, 68 % in the 680 upland forest studied by Shure and Phillips (1987), but twigs and CWD are often prominent, 681 e.g. 46 % of the litter recorded in an Amazonian rain forest (Chambers et al. 2000). Broken 682 dead trees can also contribute, e.g. 7% in a boreal forest in Finland (Siitonen et al. 2000). In 683 boreal forests CWD is an important constituent of forest function, covering over 5 % of the 684 forest floor, the larger tree trunks taking up to a century to decompose (Zielonka and Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 22 of 47 685 Niklasson 2001). In many climates, epiphytes add appreciably to the litter fall, being 5-10 % 686 in old tropical cloud forest in Costa Rica and c. 5 % (all lichen) in Picea abies (Norway 687 spruce) old-growth forests in Sweden (Esseen 1985). 688 5.2 Litter decomposition 689 The rate of litter decomposition sets the period during which litter can have 690 mechanical and chemical effects, as well as driving nutrient cycling (section 5.5). Litter is 691 broken down by a succession of organisms: invertebrate shredders, commutators, fungi and 692 mycotrophs, and bacteria (Cadisch and Giller 1997). The breakdown rate varies with the 693 climate, but also with the particular characteristics of the litter: mechanical, nutrient content, 694 polyphenol content, cellulose:lignin ratio, etc. (Cornelissen and Thompson 1997; Osono and 695 Takeda 2004). There is evidence that mixtures of leaf litter from several species sometimes 696 decompose faster than single-species litter (McTiernan et al. 1997; D.A. Wardle et al. 2002). 697 This would reduce litter residence times and hence the effects of litter. Gartner and Cardon 698 (2004), reviewing the literature, found that enhanced decomposition in mixtures has been 699 found in 47 % of cases reported. For example, Kaneko and Salamanca (1999) found that in 700 litter bags containing species collected from a Japanese Quercus serrata (oak) / Pinus 701 densiflora (red pine) forest, mixtures containing Sasa densiflora lost weight faster than 702 expected from the decomposition rate of their components. Such effects might operate by 703 mixtures enhancing the commutating fauna or by the high N content of some species speeding 704 the decomposition of their lower-N associates (Kaneko and Salamanca 1999; Hoorens et al. 705 2002; D.A. Wardle et al. 2006). In any case, the effect could be important in species co- 706 existence, giving multi-species communities different properties from monocultures. 707 5.3 Effects of litter 708 As we have explained, plant litter is universal and it would be artificial to deal with all 709 litter effects in one place. In many cases litter affects system functions through interaction 710 with heterotrophs, and these are described in Section 8 of this chapter. The fundamental effect 711 of litter is to disturb the environment of communities, but simple mechanical effects occur 712 and are discussed in the next section (6). Programmed and stochastic shed litter must have 713 both physical and chemical effects. For CWD the physical effect is predominant, but finer 714 litter, e.g. epiphytic litter of bryophytes and lichens, may have mainly a chemical effect. 715 Adjustments of community composition are possible because litter has different effects on 716 different species. It can facilitate or redirect succession, and deep persistent leaf litter can 717 inhibit succession by preventing the invasion of later-successional species (Crawley 1997). Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 23 of 47 718 An increase in nutrient supply leads, in many communities, to dominance by a few fast- 719 growing, tall species and to lower species richness (Willems et al. 1993). This has usually 720 been seen as due to competition, but it could equally well be due to litter effects (Berendse 721 1999). Many speculative examples cannot identify the precise ways in which litter may be 722 modifying plant interactions, but we can try a theoretical separation of mediation in 723 communities under three classes: 724 725 726 727 728 1. Physical effects, being alteration in physical environment (section 5.4), or direct damage (here called autogenic disturbance, section 6.2), 2. Chemical effects, being alteration in chemical environment (section 5.4) or control of nutrient cycling (section 5.5). 3. Complex environmental interactions. Peat formation (section 5.6) is a special case . 729 Interactions are discussed in section 8, with herbivores (section 8.1), fire (section 8.2) 730 and higher order interactions (section 8.3). 731 732 5.4 Alteration in environment Documentation has been particularly thorough in the case of leaf litter (Table 1). For 733 some of the effects listed in Table 1, opposite effects are seen in different situations, for the 734 effects depend on the characteristics of the litter, its depth, its phenology of production, the 735 affected species and the growth medium (Hamrick and Lee 1987; Nilsson et al. 1999; Facelli 736 et al. 1999). In addition many effects require the litter to decompose slowly enough to 737 maintain a constant presence in the A0 top soil layer as outlined in Section 5.2. 738 Litter changes the environment at the soil surface by its physical presence. Low 739 growing forest plants must be suppressed by the low light under litter, but we have not found 740 a study on this. Red/far red (R/FR) ratios, already reduced by a live canopy, can be reduced 741 by half again by the litter (Vázquez-Yanes et al. 1990). Litter might intercept rain and allow it 742 to quickly evaporate, reducing soil water content (Myers and Talsma 1992), but it can also 743 reduce evaporation from the soil itself (Eckstein and Donath 2005) and increase infiltration. 744 The temperature regime at the soil surface can also be affected (Fowler 1986b). Other 745 environmental effects of litter can cause switches (chap. 3, sect. 5.4), importantly including 746 acidification (section 5.4 below). There can be effects on community dynamics, for example 747 in forests of Michigan and Wisconsin, USA, hardwoods such as Quercus rubra (red oak) can 748 dominate the mid-successional forest. Tsuga canadensis (hemlock) cannot enter such stands 749 because its seedlings cannot penetrate the hardwood litter, whereas Acer saccharum (sugar 750 maple) seedlings can, and it therefore invades (Rejmánek 1999). 751 It has been long suspected that leachates from litter affect the germination and growth Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 24 of 47 752 of certain plant species: allelopathy. The case of the tree Celtis laevigata (sugarberry) is the 753 clearest (Turner and Rice 1975; Lodhi 1978). Bosy and Reader (1995) found that cover by 754 grass litter in an oldfield markedly reduced the seed germination of at least three of four forb 755 species, and for two species an appreciable component of the suppression could be explained 756 by allelopathy effect of leachate. Rutherford and Powrie (1993) showed that leaf and litter 757 leachates from the Australian Acacia cyclops (coastal wattle) affected the small-leaved shrub 758 Anthospermum spathulatum below it. Chapin et al. (1994) suggest that in Glacier Bay 759 successions Alnus sinuata (alder) suppresses the creeping Dryas drummondii partly by the 760 allelopathic effects of its litter, but as part of complex replacement interactions. However, 761 experiments are easier in vitro than in the field, and only a few clearly demonstrate that litter 762 allelopathic agents have real effects in nature. 763 Subvention via litter is also possible, and can be very significant for particular species. 764 Litter can subvent germination, probably by increasing humidity at the soil surface (Fowler 765 1986b), or it can act as a cue for germination from the seed bank (Preston and Baldwin 1999). 766 In temperate rain forests, CWD can be an essential substrate for tree seedling regeneration 767 (McKee et al. 1982; Agnew et al. 1993a; Duncan 1993). Although bryophytes are often 768 suppressed by litter cover (Barkman et al. 1977), especially by the combination of litter from 769 woody species and from herbs in forest, During and Willems (1986) named five bryophyte 770 species that they suggested “may thrive on litter”. It is possible that nutrients released from 771 litter are taken up by bryophytes (Tamm 1964). CWD often harbours a specialised bryophyte 772 flora and in some Scandanavian forests the protonemata of many bryophytes are energetically 773 dependent on it (Siitonen 2001). 774 Both interference and subvention via litter are types of reaction, and ways in which 775 species affect each other, driving community processes. 776 5.5 Control by litter of nutrient cycling 777 Litter returns nutrients to the soil as mineral compounds, or as organic compounds 778 which are then broken down to mineral ones. This is the well-known process of nutrient 779 cycling, but it will be affected by litter type. Berendse (1994) found that in fast-growing 780 species (i.e. with high RGRmax) less N was re-translocated to the living parts before the leaf 781 fell, and the resulting litter broke down quickly. Such species would be expected to 782 predominate in N-rich environments. In contrast, species with a low RGRmax re-translocated a 783 greater proportion of their leaf N. Such N-conserving species would be expected early in 784 primary succession, when N is in short supply. 785 These differences have the potential to drive succession. Berendse et al. (1987) Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 25 of 47 786 compared the deciduous Molinia caerulea (purple moor grass) with the evergreen shrub Erica 787 tetralix (heath). During one year, M. caerulea lost 63 % of the N present in its above-ground 788 standing biomass and 34 % of its P. Comparable losses in E. tetralix were 27 % and 31 %, 789 respectively. This suggests that under nutrient-poor conditions the low-nutrient-loss E. tetralix 790 will be the dominant species, and that if nutrient availability increases M. caerulea will 791 replace it. There is some empirical support for this: in a similar system, Berendse et al. (1994) 792 found that addition of nutrient solution caused Calluna vulgaris (a species ecologically 793 similar to E. tetralix) to decline and cover of four grass species to increase. The litterbag 794 decomposition rates given for various species by Cornelissen et al. (1999) make it clear that 795 plants of later successional stages have litter that is slower to decompose, giving a greater 796 potential to affect other species. 797 5.6 Peat formation 798 On wet substrates, if litter breaks down at a slower rate than it is produced, peat will 799 build as plant remains in various stages of humification, i.e. partial organic hydrolysis 800 mediated by micro-organisms (Fig. 2.6). Some types of litter especially impede drainage to 801 cause waterlogging. This yields humic colloids with properties that further restrict drainage 802 and oxygenation and, being complex molecules bounded by weakly acidic OH radicals, lower 803 the pH of the soil solution and reduce nutrient availability. This decreases litter production but 804 decrease its decomposition even more, so that peat accumulation increases. 805 Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 26 of 47 806 807 808 809 810 811 812 813 814 815 816 817 818 819 820 821 822 823 824 825 826 827 828 829 830 831 832 833 834 835 836 Sphagnum grows and builds a mound Empodisma grows associated Ericaceae cell walls with cellulose hydrolyse to uronic acid high concentrations of polyphenols in litter NPK scavenged from low concentrations because of high base exchange capacity for NH4, Ca and K substrate surface is isolated from ground water input of rain and CO2 acid, low NPK input + leaching low pH low NPK cell walls decompose only partially, to humic acids decomposition slows anoxia brown water from leachate of semidecomposed plant material methane produced peat buildup (litter>decomposition) porosity of peat decreases Key = Sphagnum-specific paths = Empodisma-specific paths = the main switch feedback cycle Fig. 2.6. The process of peat accumulation in mires dominated by Empodisma minus (wire 837 838 rush) or Sphagnum moss species. 839 There several modes of peat formation. In eutrophic or mesotrophic conditions, litter 840 production is high but decomposition is fast too, so peat will form only in still lakes, infilling 841 them: a classic succession story (D. Walker 1970). 842 In regions of high rainfall, soils are wet and also nutrient-poor through leaching. Here 843 blanket peat can form over dry land, a process known as paludification (Crawford 2000). The 844 species adapted to these conditions often bear leaves with a high polyphenol content, which 845 slows decomposition (Inderjit and Mallik 1997; Northup et al. 1998). This is a widespread 846 phenomenon, having important effects on community structure. For example Ehrenfeld et al. Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 27 of 47 847 (1995) propose that small ericaceous shrubs trap litter in New Jersey pine savannahs, 848 inhibiting decomposition and maintaining openings in the canopy, while Inderjit and Mallik 849 (1997) provide experimental evidence for litter of Ledum groenlandicum (Labrador tea) 850 limiting black spruce regeneration in Canada. These blanket bogs are not fully isolated from 851 the substrate, but because the rainfall is high, evaporation low and the terrain flat, almost all 852 of their water and nutrient input is from rain, so they can be regarded as ombrotrophic. 853 The ultimate stage after the formation of lake peat or after paludification is the 854 formation of a raised bog that is completely ombrotrophic because it is raised above its 855 surroundings (Klinger 1996, Anderson et al. 2003). The litter production and the conditions 856 needed for this come from species which can absorb nutrients directly from rainfall, mostly 857 bryophytes and particularly Sphagnum spp. In many New Zeland bogs the same function is 858 fulfilled by the restiad Empodisma minus (wire rush, Agnew et al. 1993b), an odd flowering 859 plant which has the same effect through its negatively geotropic root weft. This guild has, in 860 common, cell walls with the weak cellulose-derived uronic acid that can scavenge ions from 861 precipitation, yielding sufficient nutrients for annual growth and maintaining a high hydrogen 862 ion concentration in the interstitial water. The effect is well documented for Sphagnum spp. in 863 temperate mires (Clymo and Hayward 1982; Koojiman and Bakker 1994). The rate of 864 percolation, as seen in Darcy’s constant, is slowed by the peat formed. The waterlogging, low 865 pH and low-nutrient conditions favour this guild. Malmer et al. (1994; 2003) suggest that since 866 Sphagnum spp. can outcompete vascular plants under ombrotrophic conditions these 867 conditions may arise rather suddenly. This is the state change expected when a switch is 868 operating (chap. 3, sect. 5). There are indeed records of such rapid state changes where 869 Sphagnum spp. expansion has caused a brown moss fen to be replaced by bog (Kuhry et al. 870 1993), destroyed forest (Anderson et al. 2003) and invaded forest around the edges of basin 871 mires (Klinger 1996). These are alternative stable states (chap. 3, sect. 5.6), and the state 872 change to bog may be initiated by climatic change to colder, wetter conditions (Korhola 1996). 873 5.7 Can reaction via litter evolve? 874 875 876 877 878 879 880 881 882 *A: > 4.7 Can reaction via litter evolve? > Can we add a thought here? 8 lines from end, under Whitham's > idea for community selection. If the community is viewed as a > mosaic of patches (as I > argue) then surely a patch type may be favoured due to a > litter effect of its predominant species, which could favour > it and the few individuals of the species which engineered > the patch. Eh? 883 should gauge whether these processes are simply by-products of growth, or whether they 884 could have evolved. This is hardly in doubt for the other types of interaction discussed in this It is not enough to describe the processes that occur within plant communities; we Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 28 of 47 885 chapter – interference, subvention and autogenic disturbance – because the characters of the 886 plant involved affect its fitness and hence the transmission of its genes to the next generation. 887 As Clements (1904) pointed out, all plants react on their environment and the extent and type 888 of reaction will be genetically controlled. A particular reaction will evolve if it enhances the 889 fitness of the individual plant (i.e. genet) more than it enhances the fitness of its conspecific 890 neighbours, after taking into account any cost of reaction. However, litter affects all the 891 individuals and species in a community, not just the litter-producing plant. Moreover, 892 sometimes the characters of the litter are expressed too late to affect whether the genes that 893 caused them are transmitted: when the litter is produced as the plant dies or has its effect after 894 the plant’s death by paludification, by control of nutrient cycling or as fuel for fire. Then there 895 is then no possibility of plants enhancing their own fitness, in survival or fecundity, only that 896 of their offspring. That is to say, an explanation in terms of kin selection is needed, with the 897 mechanism that a plant benefits especially its neighbours, which are especially likely to carry 898 the same alleles (J.B. Wilson 1987). Another way of expressing this is selection through the 899 extended phenotype. Northup et al. (1998) commented that “decomposing leaf litter is not 900 generally considered to be part of the plant’s phenotype” so that litter characters must evolve 901 through selection on the extended phenotype. By the theoretical analyses of Odling-Smee et 902 al. (1996) and Laland et al. (1996; 1999) selection for characters of the extended phenotype, 903 i.e. ‘secondary’ characters, is possible. It has been remarked that just as selection in different 904 areas with comparable environments, but different species, often produces similar solutions in 905 terms of leaf characters, so similar solution in terms of reaction on the environment would be 906 expected in different species and communities (Lewontin 1985; Mooney et al. 1977). A 907 different solution was proposed by Whitham et al. (2003), that traits of the extended 908 phenotype could evolve because they affect the whole community and there could be 909 selection between communities, a concept that most biologists find hard to swallow (but see 910 D.S. Wilson 1997). Most plants produce litter during their life and have an opportunity for 911 that litter to affect their fitness and for this Northup et al.’s argument does not apply, but there 912 are still problems that a genet’s litter can benefit not only that genet but also nearby genets. It 913 is also a problem that litter effects seem to be a necessary by-product of growth, so often no 914 evolutionary explanation is necessary. 915 6 Autogenic disturbance: plants as disturbers 916 Disturbances, marked changes in the environment for a limited period, often with the 917 removal of plant material, are more common than was once believed and important for plant 918 communities (White 1979; Rundel 1998; Grime 2001). Most consideration has been of Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 29 of 47 919 allogenic disturbance, i.e. that caused by external factors such as animals or extreme weather 920 events. For example, Bazzaz (1983) describes disturbance as continuous and necessary in all 921 ecosystems, but discusses only allogenic causes. No less important, we suggest, are the minor 922 disturbances within the community that inevitably result from the growth and death of plant 923 parts, the ‘autogenic disturbance’ of Attiwill (1994) and the ‘endogenous disturbance’ of 924 White (1979). Autogenic disturbance occurs by plant contact, litter, treefall, etc. (though there 925 seems little opportunity for autogenic disturbance by subterranean organs). Thus, vegetation, 926 by its nature, disturbs itself. As we pointed out in chapter 1: “Plants move, animals don’t”. 927 Autogenic disturbance has potentially positive and negative effects on population fitness and 928 density, yet stands apart from the general considerations of interference and subvention 929 described above. There is a wide range in the intensity of autogenic disturbances, from 930 delicate adjustments resulting from leaf growth to the fall of a group of trees, but plants must 931 tolerate all these and they may profoundly affect vegetation dynamics. Autogenic disturbance 932 may be more predictable than allogenic disturbances such as storms and earthquakes, and if 933 so selection for adaptation to it is more likely to occur. However, autogenic and allogenic 934 disturbance can interact. 935 6.1 Movement and contact 936 Even gentle contact between plants can affect their growth, producing effects such as 937 reduced growth, reduced soluble carbohydrates and increased transpiration (van Gardingen 938 and Grace 1991; Keller and Steffen 1995). For example, when Biddington and Dearman 939 (1985) brushed Brassica oleracea var. botrytis (cauliflower) and Apium graveolens (celery) 940 seedlings with typing paper for 1.5 min day-1 it reduced their shoot dry weight (Fig. 2.7). Control Brushed Fig. 2.7: The effect of brushing seedlings of Brassica oleracea for 1.5 min day-1After Biddington and Dearman (1985). 941 942 Experimentally shaking the plant can have an even greater effect, e.g. reduced shoot 943 extension, shorter and wider stems, lower shoot:root ratios, altered root development, shorter Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 30 of 47 944 petioles, shorter flowering culms and reduced reproductive effort (Neel and Harris 1971; 945 Braam and Davis 1990; Gartner 1994; Niklas 1998; Goodman and Ennos 1998). Whilst 946 brushing with typing paper and shaking by hand are artificial treatments, they are very similar 947 to the leaves of adjacent plants brushing each other, or close trees rocking each other. The 948 changes are induced by the up-regulation of a large number of genes (Braam and Davis 1990). 949 Plants can also change position as a result of their own growth (Evans and Barkham 1992), 950 for example Salix (willow) trunks collapse due to their own weight in wet carr forest (Agnew, 951 pers. obs), and shrub stems can be bent by their own weight and fall to the ground (B.F. 952 Wilson 1997). 953 Shoots can thrust each other aside physically as they grow, a more sustained contact 954 effect (Campbell et al. 1992). For example, the leaves of Juncus squarrosus are vertical when 955 they first emerge, but they then become horizontal, form a rosette and press down surrounding 956 plants (authors pers. obs.) as do leaves of Plantago species (Campbell et al. 1992). 957 Surprisingly little is known of this process. Secondary growth of tree trunks, branches and 958 roots continually creates new habitat, initiating succession by providing opportunities for 959 colonisation. Circumferential branch growth produces new sites for the establishment of 960 epiphytes. Some bryophyte species are tree-base specialists (Ashton and McCrae 1970). Their 961 specialisation is perhaps explicable by their greater shade tolerance (Hosokawa and Odani 962 1957) and the peculiar characteristics of trunk water-flow creating a damper, more nutrient- 963 rich environment, but this is also the zone where the fastest circumferential growth occurs. 964 Circumferential growth of tree roots can affect the environment above the surface: woody 965 roots near the soil surface create shallow soils and diversify the forest floor habitat (Agnew 966 pers. obs.). 967 968 969 The reader may like to take the ‘pers. obs’ in this section as confirmation that autogenic disturbance has not received due attention in ecological research. Crown shyness comprises gaps between the canopies of adjacent trees (Jacobs 1955), 970 probably caused by abrasion in the canopy (Putz et al. 1984). Franco (1986) observed the 971 process, reporting that when branches of Picea sitchensis (Sitka spruce) and Larix kaempferi 972 (Japanese larch) met, physical damage occurred by abrasion, causing the death of the leading 973 shoots. New shoots arising from lateral buds were also killed. Long and Smith (1992) 974 demonstrated the mechanism by fixing wooden pickets into the canopy as artificial branches: 975 they were broken back to the edge of the crown, due to wind-rock and consequential abrasion. 976 Tall slender trees were subject to greater wind-rock and thus greater frequency of impact with 977 neighbouring crowns, leading to greater crown shyness. Putz et al. (1984) found that branches 978 of Avicenna germinans (mangrove) bordering crown shyness gaps generally had broken twigs Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 31 of 47 979 and few leaves. As in Long and Smith’s (op. cit.) observations, flexible crowns were more 980 widely spaced than still crowns. These effects are the result of the compulsory growth and 981 movement imposed by the modular construction of plants (chap. 1, sect. 1.1). 982 6.2 Growth and gravity 983 CWD clearly has the potential to be very destructive on the forest floor. The most 984 destructive type of stochastic litter is tree fall, which is the autogenic disturbance par 985 excellence and implicated in all demographic processes in forests of mixed canopy species. It 986 is a normal event in many undisturbed stands, which can be measured and built into models of 987 species and biomass turnover in boreal (e.g. Hofgaard 1993), tropical evergreen (e.g. 988 Chandrashekara and Ramakrishnan 1994) and tropical rain forests (e.g. Kellman and 989 Tackaberry 1993). 990 The weight in a tree’s canopy can eventually result in its fall, obviously disturbing 991 surrounding plants. In a forest the upper canopy trees are more likely to fall, having reached 992 their greatest potential and being too heavy for their root support or too loaded with 993 lianas/epiphytes (Strong 1977; Putz 1995). Tree fall is associated with storm damage, 994 particularly in mid-latitudes (e.g. Busing 1996, B.P. Allen et al. 1997). Understorey trees are 995 more likely to become standing dead, since pathogens or light reduction by neighbours (self- 996 thinning) can kill whether or not the tree has the potential to grow further, and they are not as 997 exposed to storm damage as are the canopy species. An exception occurs in the dry Kysna 998 (South Africa) forest, where most canopy trees die in situ due to “competition, senescence and 999 secondary pathogens” and gradually break up (Midgley et al. 1995). There is not enough 1000 evidence to suggest this as a general feature of any forest types. 1001 In old-growth forest, tree fall is the usual process that forms canopy gaps, resulting in 1002 several sudden environmental changes take place. The most important is that the forest floor 1003 is brightly lit with light of a very different spectral quality. The effects of a gap are not all 1004 positive: Lovelock et al. (1998) found photoinhibition of shade-tolerant juveniles in gaps, 1005 especially in species with short-lived leaves. Another, often overlooked, factor is 1006 rainfall/hydrology. A full canopied forest evaporates up to 40 % of incoming rainfall and the 1007 soil is seldom at field capacity, but gaps receive the rain uninterrupted. There is also soil 1008 disturbance, with soil exposed in a root pit and adjacent root plate mound. The bare soil and 1009 higher light allow well-dispersed species to invade, as observed by Ellison et al. (1993) in 1010 neotropical forest, and Narukawa and Yamamoto (2001) for Abies spp. (fir) seedlings in 1011 boreal and subalpine Japan. 1012 Branches can also cause severe impact when they fall (section 5.1). For example, in Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 32 of 47 1013 the tropical liana Connarus turczaninowii in Panama, 20-45 % of the mortality of young 1014 plants is caused by branch fall (Aide 1987); in a tropical rain forest in Amazonia, Uhl (1982) 1015 found the most common (38 %) cause of death in small (1-10 cm dbh) trees to be a branch fall 1016 or treefall; in Costa Rican rain forest, Vandermeer (1977) found that 46 % of seedling deaths 1017 were due to falling leaves or branches, about half of these were due to falling palm fronds. 1018 Gillman and Ogden (2001) found in Podocarp/angiosperm forest in northern North Island, 1019 NZ, that 10-20 % of the annual seedling mortality was caused by litterfall. 1020 6.3 Lianas and epiphytes 1021 Lianas can be so abundant in the canopy of many tropical and sub-tropical forests that 1022 they compete with the emergent trees (Lowe and Walker 1977). Treefall, with its associated 1023 disturbance to the forest floor, can be generated by either adherent lianas (e.g. Hedera helix, 1024 ivy) or free-swinging lianas (e.g. Ripogonum scandens, supplejack). Phillips et al. (2005) 1025 found that in an upper-Amazon tropical rain forest large trees were three times as likely to die 1026 if they were invested by lianas. The resulting canopy gap can also benefit lianas. Needing less 1027 investment in support they can extend fast and keep pace with the fast-growing secondary 1028 species filling the gap. 1029 Twining or tendril lianas can constrict the host stem (Lutz 1943; Clark and Clark 1030 1991; Matista and Silk 1997). Usually there is little active strangulation, but the liana stays 1031 put and the tree grows, which can produce deformations in the trunk and lead to changes in 1032 the vascular tissue and the wood fibres (Falconer 1986; Reuschel et al. 1998; Putz 1991). 1033 Constriction can inhibit or even stop downward translocation of organic solutes, though 1034 sometimes new parallel conducting tissue is formed (Lutz 1943; Putz 1991, Hegarty 1991). 1035 The constriction can also be a point of weakness, at which a branch or young stem is more 1036 likely to break (Lutz 1943; Uhl 1982; Putz 1991). Deformed trees are also more susceptible to 1037 pathogens (Putz 1991). Not infrequently the tree dies (Lutz 1943; Clark and Clark 1991; Uhl 1038 1982). 1039 The interactions between support and liana are complex. Lianas are often supported by 1040 more than one tree (with a record of 27 trees for one liana on Barro Colorado Island, Panama: 1041 Putz 1984). This causes interdependence between trees that may stabilise canopy trees 1042 adjacent to treefalls (Putz 1984; Hegarty 1991). However, a corollary is that if such a 1043 connected tree falls, it damages more trees. For example, Putz (1984) found on Barro 1044 Colorado Island that 2.3 trees fell with each gapmaker, often ones that shared lianas. Liana- 1045 laden trees can also pull branches off neighbouring trees as they fall (Appanah and Putz 1046 1984). Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 33 of 47 1047 We have found no record of an epiphyte being associated with tree or branch fall, 1048 although their contribution to litter fall has been well documented (see above). Strangler 1049 epiphytes certainly damage trees (Richards 1996). The epiphyte species involved are 1050 evergreen; many are in the genus Ficus (fig) and curiously revered by the indigenous people 1051 (F. benghalensis in Hindustan, F. natalensis in Kenya). Such stranglers begin life as 1052 epiphytes, gradually extend aerial roots downwards, eventually root in the soil, then grow 1053 around their host as a tree. The host usually dies, and does so standing, though the cause of 1054 the host’s death is not clear (Richards 1996). 1055 7 Plant-plant interactions mediated by heterotrophs 1056 While a macrophyte remains sedentary4 the populations of organisms utilising the 1057 carbon energy source that it represents change. These include herbivores, parasites and 1058 pathogens, all capable of affecting a plant’s fitness. We deal here only with ways these 1059 modify a plant’s impact on its neighbours. In most cases these interactions occur on a 1060 particular spatial scale, in a local vegetation patch. 1061 7.1 Below-ground benefaction 1062 It is generally assumed that species with nitrogen-fixing micro-organisms in root 1063 nodules enhance the nitrogen economy of associated non-nodulating plants. These N-fixing 1064 plants are therefore held to be important in ecosystems and particularly in rangelands 1065 (Paschke 1997). However likely the transfer is, it is rarely proven. Kohls et al. (2003) set out 1066 to investigate this link in the well-studied site of Glacier Bay, Alaska. Using Δ15N values they 1067 concluded that nodulated plants accounted for most of the fixed N in soils and plants for up to 1068 40 years of succession, most of this being donated to the ecosystem by Alnus viridis (green 1069 alder). The assumption that fixed nitrogen is passed from nodulated to un-nodulated plants via 1070 above-ground and below-ground litter is probably justified in many cases. Another possibility 1071 is that N transfer occurs through a common mycorrhizal network (CMN) that links 1072 individuals below ground. In all three major types of mycorrhiza – vesicular-arbuscular, 1073 ectotrophic and ericoid/epacrid – some of the fungal species involved can infect more than 1074 one plant species (Simard and Durall 2004). The vesicular-arbuscular and ectotrophic ones are 1075 known to establish fungal networks that connect the roots of different species, and this may be 1076 true for the ericoid/epacrid type too (Cairney and Ashford 2002). There is evidence for greater 1077 nitrogen transfer when mycorrhizae are present, but it is not clear whether the transfer is 1078 through the CMN or whether the mycelia increase release of N from one plant into the soil 1079 and/or uptake by the other plant. Moreover, ideas that this would even out concentrations Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 34 of 47 1080 between plants do not stand up: He et al. (2004) found that there was net transfer from 1081 Eucalyptus maculata (gum) to Casuarina cunninghamiana (sheoke). This represents transfer 1082 from a plant that is not N-fixing, to one that is N-fixing and moreover had close to double the 1083 N concentration in its tissues. This transfer was at very low rates, but the benefaction was in 1084 the opposite direction from that usually assumed between fixing and non-fixing plants. 1085 Transfers via CMNs could operate for P. However, Newman and Ritz (1986) and Newman 1086 and Eason (1993) carefully monitored the transfer of 32P and concluded that any direct 1087 transfer via hyphal links was very minor, and too slow to substantially influence the nutrient 1088 status of the plants. 1089 It has also been suggested that CMNs affect the performance of plants via carbon 1090 transfer (e.g. Booth 2004). However, it is unclear whether plant-tissue to plant-tissue transfer 1091 of carbon actually occurs (Simard and Durall 2004). There has never been a convincing 1092 demonstration of it, certainly not to a degree sufficient to benefit the recipient plant. Even in 1093 cases where carbon has been traced by radioactivity to the recipient it is likely that it is 1094 retained in the fungal hyphae, not in the plant tissues (Robinson and Fitter 1999). 1095 Roots and microbial activity in the soil are intimately linked, since roots provide the 1096 most of the carbon source below ground. Chen et al. (2004) used microcosms to show that 1097 Pinus radiata roots increased microbial activity and the mineralisation of organic P. There 1098 was evidence that root exudates were responsible for these effects. It is possible that the plant 1099 can obtain N from free-living N-fixing bacteria in the same way. That these effects are species 1100 specific was demonstrated by Innes et al. (2004), who found that the grasses Holcus lanatus 1101 (Yorkshire fog) and Anthoxanthum odoratum (sweet vernal) could stimulate microbial 1102 activity, whereas dicotyledonous forbs depressed it. The effect was only seen in soil of higher 1103 fertility. 1104 7.2 Pollination 1105 When fruit set is limited by pollen, plants of the same or different species can interfere 1106 with each others’ reproduction by competing for the service of pollinating animals (Grabas 1107 and Laverty 1999). When pollen from another species lands on a stigma it may interfere with 1108 the germination or fertilisation of the species own pollen by occlusion or allelopathy, reducing 1109 seed set and perhaps increasing self-fertilisation (Murphy and Aarssen 1995). Plants can also 1110 compete for animal dispersers (Wheelwright 1985). 1111 There can be subvention too if one species attracts pollinators to the vicinity of 1112 another by mimicry or by being more attractive (a ‘magnet species’: Laverty 1992). Moeller 1113 (2004) found that, among Clarkia xantiana (an annual in the Onagraceae) populations in Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 35 of 47 1114 California, there were more pollinating bees and less pollination limitation in populations 1115 with a greater number of congeners, implying that the mass of plants of the same pollination 1116 type attracted pollinators. An interaction that we can count as a genuine mutualism between 1117 embryophytes was demonstrated by Waser and Real (1979). If species have staggered 1118 flowering times with shared pollinators and some overlap in flowering time, there will 1119 probably be some competition for pollinators. However, the continuous availability of flower 1120 rewards over the period may maintain pollinator populations. Waser and Real gave evidence 1121 from flower numbers, fecundities and pollinating hummingbird numbers across four years at a 1122 site in the Rocky Mountains, Colorado, that the earlier-flowering Delphinium nelsonii was 1123 benefiting the later-flowering Ipomopsis aggregata thus. If we consider the maintenance of 1124 hummingbird populations from one year to the next, I. aggregata could be benefiting D. 1125 nelsonii in a similar way, forming a genuine mutualism. These interactions have the potential to affect species’ co-existence patterns, and we 1126 1127 shall return to them in chapter 5 (Assembly Rules). 1128 7.3 Herbivory 1129 Herbivores can remove large amounts of all types plant material and this must affect 1130 the plant community. The word herbivory suggests grazing, with mammals biting off whole 1131 leaves, but most herbivores are invertebrates, and these maintain their trophic pressure more 1132 consistently and more specifically than the vertebrates. Contrary to the situation with 1133 vertebrates, invertebrate herbivore pressure can be as great below-ground as above-ground 1134 (Brown 1993; D.A. Wardle 2002). They can be very much smaller than their substrate, giving 1135 large populations. With fast reproduction this can allow efficient selection to overcome plant 1136 defences and specialise on particular plant species. Invertebrate herbivores have parasites and 1137 carnivores that control them. There are therefore complex systems affecting plant population 1138 fitness through herbivory, but since the observation of both the herbivory and the effect on the 1139 plant are difficult, little is known about the effects of invertebrates on the balance between co- 1140 occurring plant species and hence on community structure. 1141 Since herbivores are always selective to some degree (chap. 4, sect. 3.4) they can 1142 effect positive or negative interactions between species. There are several mechanisms (Table 1143 2.x): 1144 Table 2.x. Possible mechanisms of herbivore-mediated plant-plant interaction 1145 Positive = species S increases in fitness; Negative = it decreases in fitness. 1146 Comparison: Target species S can be affected if conspecific neighbours are replaced by Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 36 of 47 1147 heterospecific plants (comparisons R below), or S can be affected differently by 1148 a neighbouring plant species when a herbivore moves into the patch 1149 (comparisons A below). 1150 Diff. in palat. = species S and its neighbour differ in palatability to the herbivore. We 1151 note that differences in palatability will often be quantitative. 1152 Diff. in defence = species S and its neighbour differ in defences against the herbivore. 1153 Common = our estimate of whether this mechanism is common in the real world. +ve/-ve Mechanism Positive 1 The neighbour repels the herbivore, reducing the herbivory load on S 2 The neighbour attracts the herbivore’s enemy, reducing the herbivory load on S 3 Defences against herbivory are induced in the neighbour, reducing its interference against S 4 The neighbour attracts the herbivore, which creates open ground, which is the micro-beta niche of species S 5 The neighbour attracts the herbivore, diverting the herbivory load from S 6 The herbivore removes a more palatable, interfering, neighbour of S 7 The herbivore defends S against an interfering neighbour Negative 8 The neighbour attracts the herbivore, increasing the herbivory load on S 9 An unpalatable neighbour diverts the herbivore to S 10 The herbivore removes a more palatable beneficient neighbour of S 1154 Comp- Diff. in Diff. in Common arison palat. defence ? R ― yes yes R yes ― A ― yes A yes ― A yes ― A yes ― A no no R yes ― probably not yes (but little evidence) probably not probably R yes ― no A yes ― ? probably not small effects occasionally (1) +ve: The neighbour repels the herbivore, reducing the herbivory load 1155 The repelling of herbivores by a neighbour (Augner 1994) affects plant communities 1156 at the scale which the repulsion occurs. For example, a spiny shrub deters the herbivore from 1157 consuming any other species within its canopy. A recognisably unpalatable species in a grass 1158 sward deters herbivore from taking its bite at that point, sparing any palatable species growing 1159 closely with it. As examples among mammalian browsers/grazers, Hjältén et al. (1993) found 1160 that when Betula pubescens was associated with the less palatable Alnus incana (alder) the 1161 herbivores Microtus agrestis (voles) and Lepus timidus (hares) avoided those patches and B. 1162 pubescens suffered less herbivory damage. The avoidance of a less preferred patch would be 1163 part of an “extended phenotype” of the neighbour species, but confer benefit on all the plants 1164 in that community. Tuomi et al. (1994) suggested that the evolutionarily stable strategy would Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 37 of 47 1165 be for such benefit to conspecific neighbours to limited, or an undefended (‘selfish’) genotype 1166 would too readily invade. They suggested that plant defences should deter the herbivore 1167 without killing it. 1168 Chemicals from a species might repel insects not only from that species but also from 1169 their neighbours. The mechanism depends on the scale of effect of the defence mechanism 1170 (e.g. diffusion of a chemical) and on the foraging behaviour of the herbivore. The mechanism 1171 is widely discussed as a goal of organic gardeners, but supporting evidence is rare. 1172 Tahvanainen and Root (1972) found that planting Lycopersicon esculentum (tomato) amid 1173 Brassica oleracea (collards) plants decreased flea beetle populations on B. oleracea and 1174 increased its plant weight. In a choice experiment, adult flea beetles (Phyllotreta cruciferae) 1175 preferred excised B. oleraceus leaves with no tomato mixed in with them, which is evidence 1176 of the involvement of a repellent chemical. It is widely suggested that companion planting of 1177 some Tagetes species (African marigold) repels insect pests and thus benefits the crop. There 1178 is a little firm evidence for this (McSorley and Dickson 1995), but nematodes, below-ground, 1179 may be mediating the interaction, apparently not due to any chemical coming from the 1180 Tagetes sp. plants (Ploeg and Maris 1999). 1181 (2) +ve: The neighbour attracts the herbivore’s enemy, reducing herbivory load 1182 White et al. (1995) found that planting Phacelia tanacetifolia, which was attractive to 1183 hover flies probably for its pollen, around Brassica oleracea (cabbage) patches (the control 1184 was bare ground around the patches) reduced the density of Brevicoryne brassicae and Myzus 1185 persicae (both aphids) on the B. oleracea. 1186 (3) +ve: Defences against herbivory are induced in the neighbour 1187 Herbivore attack, or leaf damage mimicking herbivory, can induce in a plant chemical 1188 defences such as alkaloids, nicotine, phenolics and tannins (e.g. Boege 2004) and even 1189 morphological defences (e.g. Young et al. 2003). In most cases, there is a fitness cost of this 1190 induced defence (Strauss et al. 2002). One would expect a reduced interference ability, 1191 benefiting neighbours, and Baldwin and Hamilton (2000) produced some evidence of this, but 1192 only intra-specifically. In order for this to operate, the herbivore has to select between species at 1193 a small scale: to be exact, inside the neighbourhood within which interference occurs. 1194 (4) +ve: The neighbour attracts the herbivore, which creates open ground 1195 Mammalian herbivore hoof marks are the preferred microenvironment of some species 1196 (Csotonyi and Addicott 2004). This does not depend on the herbivore’s having any selection 1197 between species or selecting at any particular spatial scale. 1198 (5) +ve: The neighbour attracts the herbivore, diverting the herbivory load Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 38 of 47 1199 This is theoretically possible, and Atsatt and O’Dowd (1976) mention some possible 1200 examples. Mensah and Khan (1997) found that Medicago sativa (lucerne), which has a higher 1201 palatability in choice experiments, diverted Creontiades dilutus (a mirid) pests from a crop of 1202 cotton. There was a dramatic effect over the four-month season. The effect would be only 1203 temporary, because the herbivore population would build up on the neighbour and probably 1204 then increase the herbivory load on the target. This would not happen were the neighbour 1205 defended against the herbivore, and caused it to emerge (e.g. cysts of a nematode to hatch), 1206 leaving a lower level of infestation for the later-germinating target species (Scholte and Vos 1207 2000). We do not know of evidence for any of these effects in natural systems. 1208 (6) +ve: The herbivore removes a more palatable, interfering neighbour 1209 If an interfering neighbour is palatable, herbivory could reduce interference from it, 1210 allowing increased growth of the target. This is the principle: “my enemy’s enemy is my 1211 friend”. It is remarkably difficult to find evidence for this. Part of the reason may be the 1212 difficulty of obtaining estimates of relative palatability, but confounding effects of the 1213 herbivore such as creating gaps and species-to-species tradeoffs such as between palatability 1214 and regrowth ability are other reasons (Bullock et al. 2001). However, Cottam (1986) grew 1215 the grass Dactylis glomerata (cocksfoot) with the more palatable Trifolium repens (white 1216 clover). When there was no grazing D. glomerata suffered in mixture, dying out after 120 1217 days of grazing, but when there was grazing by Deroceras reticulatum (slug), D. glomerata5, 1218 grew as well as, or better than, in monoculture. Carson and Root (2000), in a very careful 1219 experiment, applied insecticide experimentally in an oldfield dominated by Solidago altissima 1220 and S. rugosa (goldenrods). It became clear that in the untreated community specialist 1221 Microrhopala spp. and Trirhabda spp. (chrysomelid beetles) were suppressing the Solidago 1222 spp. and thus allowing increased growth of other forbs and of woody seedlings. As with other 1223 mechanisms involving release from interference, the herbivore must select between species at 1224 a fine scale. 1225 (7) +ve: The herbivore defends against an interfering neighbour 1226 Ants are often associated with plants. For example, Macaranga is an old world genus 1227 in the Euphorbiaceae (often conspicuously part of secondary forest) some of whose species 1228 bear extra-floral nectaries which attract ants and domatia hollows which shelter them. The 1229 relationship is close and complex. Fiala et al. (1989) found that on trees of Macaranga spp. in 1230 Malaya ants considerably reduced the amount of herbivory damage, mainly by forcing 1231 lepidopteran larvae (caterpillars) off leaves, and possibly by harassing Coleoptera (beetles) 1232 and Acrididae (grasshoppers). The ants involved are generalist predators often in the genus Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 39 of 47 1233 Crematogaster, yet they also seemed to protect against lianas: Macaranga species that were 1234 myrmecophytic had far fewer lianas attached, and this difference was seen only with plants 1235 occupied by ants. The effect was caused by the ants biting the tips of invading liana, and 1236 Federle et al. (2002) showed that they also pruned adjacent tree species reducing canopy 1237 contact and presumably also competition. 1238 (8) -ve: The neighbour attracts the herbivore, increasing the herbivory load 1239 White and Whitham (2000) found that Populus angustifolium fremontii 1240 (cottonwood) plants growing under the highly palatable Acer negundo (box elder) suffered 1241 much more herbivory from Alsophila pometaria (fall cankerworm) than those under their own 1242 species or in the open. Sessions and Kelly (2002) suggested that the grass Agrostis capillaris 1243 (bent) created a moist habitat suitable for Derocerus reticulatum (a slug) that then attacked a 1244 nearby fern (Botrychium australe). Mechanism ‘8’ can operate at any spatial scale and the 1245 herbivore does not need to be selective so long as it is attracted to the patch by one species. It 1246 seems to be the situation to which the strange term ‘apparent competition’ has been applied. 1247 The term ‘magnet species’ for the neighbour is more helpful. 1248 (9) -ve: An unpalatable neighbour diverts the herbivore to the target 1249 The neighbour could be intrinsically unpalatable, or could have induced defences. We 1250 do not know of any explicit examples of this. The effect assumes constant herbivore attention, 1251 and so would probably be temporary. 1252 (10) -ve: The herbivore removes a more palatable, subventing neighbour 1253 This is a theoretical possibility, but we know of no example. As with mechanisms 1254 involving release from interference, the herbivore must select at a fine scale. 1255 7.4 Diseases 1256 For some of the mechanisms of positive plant-plant interactions via herbivores as 1257 discussed above, such as ‘2’, ’4’ and ‘7’, it is hard to envisage analogues for fungal or viral 1258 diseases. However, others seem possible. The neighbour could repel viral vectors such as 1259 aphids (Birkett et al. 2000), reducing the disease load on S, the equivalent of herbivory 1260 interaction ‘1 +ve’ above. Defences against fungal diseases are often induced in the host, and 1261 these might carry a cost (though the evidence for this is weak), reducing the host’s 1262 interference against S (cf. herbivory ‘3 +ve’). The effect (translating to disease terms) “5 +ve: 1263 The neighbour attracts the fungus, diverting the load from S”, could be applied when a 1264 neighbour had a strong ‘fly-paper effect’, trapping fungal spores (chap. 4, sect. 3.1). 1265 Disease could remove a more susceptible neighbour that normally interferes with S Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 40 of 47 1266 (cf. herbivory ‘6 +ve’). For example, the introduction of the Asian pathogen Cryphonectria 1267 parasitica (= Endothia parasitica; chestnut blight fungus) to forests of northeastern USA led 1268 to almost complete death of the dominant Castanea dentata (chestnut). In one area this 1269 resulted within c. 14 years to a doubling of the basal area of Quercus rubra (red oak) and a 1270 threefold increase in Quercus montana (Chestnut oak; Korstian and Stickel 1927). Almost all 1271 the other tree species increased slightly. In other areas different species took advantage of the 1272 released resources. 1273 A neighbour could be susceptible to the disease and, whilst not quite being a magnet, 1274 it could spread spores or vectors and increase the disease load on S. For example, Power and 1275 Mitchell (2004) found in a field experiment with plots of 1-6 grass species that plots 1276 containing Avena fatua (wild oats), which was highly susceptible to a particular generalist 1277 aphid-vectored virus, were over 10 × more heavily infected than communities lacking A. 1278 fatua. They termed this ‘pathogen spillover’, but it is a close equivalent of our herbivore 1279 situation “8 -ve: The neighbour attracts the [disease], increasing the [disease] load”. For those 1280 wishing to use the term ‘apparent competition’, it is the disease equivalent. The neighbour 1281 does not have to suffer from the disease; it can be just a carrier. For example, van den Bergh 1282 and Elberse (1962) found that Anthoxanthum odoratum (sweet vernal grass) was a 1283 symptomless carrier of a virus that suppressed Lolium perenne (ryegrass), even in conditions 1284 in which the latter was the stronger competitor. Similarly, preliminary evidence suggests that 1285 in California Umbellularia californica (bay laurel) is infected with the fungus Phytophthora 1286 ramorum, but its growth is little affected. However, many spores are produced and these 1287 cause the death of nearby Quercus spp. (oaks) and Lithocarpus densiflora (tannoak) (C.E 1288 Mitchell and Power 2006).There is evidence that the invasion of the exotic Avena fatua (wild 1289 oat) into Californian grasslands has been due to its increasing the incidence of a virus in the 1290 native tussock grasses, severely reducing their growth, though the effect may be indirect, due 1291 to its increasing the abundance of the aphids that are vectors of the virus (Malmstrom et al. 1292 2005). 1293 An equivalent to herbivory mechanism ’9 -ve’ seems just possible, and possibly the 1294 disease could remove a more susceptible neighbour that is a facilitator of S (herbivory 1295 ‘10 -ve’). 1296 The impact of diseases is obvious when they appear suddenly, as in the case of 1297 chestnut blight. Surely diseases in the past have had a major hand in shaping the plant 1298 communities that are around today, and continue to. Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 41 of 47 1299 8 Interactions 1300 8.1 Litter/herbivore interactions 1301 Grazing, especially by large grazers, can, but does not necessarily, result in: 1302 1. less litter production, because plant vigour is reduced by the removal of 1303 photosynthetic organs and leaves have a lower probability of reaching senescence, 1304 2. redistribution of lying litter by trampling and habitat use, speeding decomposition 1305 (Eldridge and Rath 2002); 1306 3. an increase in the production of leaf litter induced by animal movement damage, the 1307 fall of necrotic leaf tissue damaged collaterally in a browser or grazer, etc., and more 1308 woody litter including CWD from collateral damage by tree canopy browsers (notably 1309 by elephants: Plumptre 1994); 1310 4. material shed around the point of damage, e.g. Buck-Sorlin and Bell (2000), found 1311 that spring defoliation in Quercus spp. (oak) trees in Wales led to increased shoot 1312 shedding; 1313 1314 5. opening of the canopy, increased growth of sub-canopy species, giving more of their litter. 1315 The complexity of such processes and systems are well illustrated by Long et al. (2003), who 1316 examined the interaction between the beetle herbivore Trirhabda virgata and its food plant 1317 Solidago altissima (goldenrod). Beetle populations were highest in the densest S. altissima 1318 patches, leading to reduced plant vigour and lower litter production. Litter, when present in 1319 quantity, increased aboveground biomass and reduced species richness. Thus the net effect of 1320 the beetle and S. altissima abundance was to enhance plant species diversity by reducing litter 1321 disturbance. 1322 In heavily grazed vegetation, litter is reduced and can have little or no effect, so that 1323 more of the system of interspecific interactions must depend on interference, subvention and 1324 autogenic disturbance. This conclusion conflicts with the usual assumption that competition 1325 will be less intense under heavy grazing. If only living plant presence is recorded and litter is 1326 ignored, only growth interactions can be assessed. This may explain why the strongest 1327 evidence for assembly rules has been found in heavily grazed (i.e. mown) lawns (e.g. J.B. 1328 Wilson and Roxburgh 1994), a heavily grazed saltmarsh (J.B. Wilson and Whittaker 1995) 1329 and a sand dune (Stubbs and Wilson 2004), in all of which there is very little accumulation of 1330 litter and interactions due to growth are not obscured by the litter effect. We suggest that 1331 assembly rules should be sought which include the litter as well as the living components, as 1332 implied by Grime (2001) in his interpretation of the productivity-richness humpback curve. Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 42 of 47 1333 CWD can also interact with herbivory. Long et al. (1998) suggest that treefall mounds 1334 provide a refuge from herbivory for Tsuga canadensis (hemlock) regeneration, which is 1335 important in spite of its being a less favourable environment for establishment of that species 1336 than the forest floor. 1337 8.2 Litter/fire interactions 1338 Sometimes the autogenic effect of litter interacts with allogenic disturbance. Fire is 1339 perhaps the best example. If litter accumulates faster than it decomposes, and local conditions 1340 of climate and soil are too dry to allow peat accumulation, its ultimate fate on a landscape 1341 scale is destruction by fire. This is basically an autogenic effect because there can be no fire 1342 without fuel, and the initiating fuel of a fire is usually a buildup of standing-dead, 1343 programmed and stochastic litter. However, an allogenic process has to start the fire. This 1344 process is intrinsic to heathlands and dry forests worldwide. 1345 Various factors affect the type of fire. A flash fire burning mainly volatiles in either 1346 the field layer or the canopy will hardly involve litter, but a fire that consumes litter will burn 1347 for longer. All fire will impact more on the less fire-resistant and more fire-intolerant species 1348 (Whelan 1995). In many vegetation types, branch fall provides the fuel build up because 1349 CWD is long-lived, much of it persisting until burnt. Treefall also provides fuel (Whelan 1350 1995). The spread of fire into the canopy is facilitated by the retention of dead limbs (Johnson 1351 1992), so branch fall will help to confine fire to the ground flora, sparing the canopy stratum. 1352 Lianas can have a similar effect: Putz (1991) observed flames travelling up lianas, with the 1353 result that Pinus spp. (pine) trees with lianas (especially Smilax spp., greenbriar etc.) were 1354 more likely to suffer crown scorch than liana-free trees. Fire is especially prevalent in 1355 communities of conifers (particularly Pinus) and Eucalyptus (gum) apparently because of 1356 their flammable resin/oil content (Whelan 1995; Williams et al. 1999). At the other extreme, 1357 some types of vegetation are very fire resistant. For example, in arid central Australia, where 1358 most vegetation is fire-prone, Acacia harpophylla (a wattle) does not burn because of several 1359 features: the chemistry of leaves makes them barely flammable; the simple leaf shape results 1360 in flat, poorly-aerated litter fuelbeds; leaf fall is keyed to intermittent rains when fire is less 1361 likely; bark is not shed; and finally the microclimate of the forest floor discourages drying and 1362 promotes decomposition (Pyne 1991). 1363 8.3 Higher-order interactions 1364 1365 There are several good examples of higher-order interactions between grazing, litter and fire. For example, in North American Pseudotsuga menziesii (Douglas fir) / Physocarpus Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 43 of 47 1366 malvaceus (mallow ninebark) forest, grazing by livestock (including cattle, sheep and deer) 1367 reduces the herb layer and hastens the decay of litter by trampling and the unpalatable herbs 1368 left tend to be less flammable (Zimmerman and Neuenschwander 1984). This reduces fire 1369 frequency, but allows the buildup of coarse woody debris so that when fires occur they are 1370 more severe and may reach the canopy. Similarly grazing reduces the fuel load and hence fire 1371 frequency in the African savannah (Fig. 2.8; Roques et al. 2001). In systems with both a grass 1372 and a woody component (savannah or ecotone) a number of processes can occur (Fig. 2.8; 1373 Brown and Sieg 1999; Touchan et al. 1995). 1374 This process includes many complexities. It could converge to an equilibrium, it could 1375 operate as a cyclic succession, or a switch could also operate in the system, giving alternative 1376 stable states (Fig. 2.8; Dublin 1995). In the 1960s and 70s, fire caused a decline in Serengeti 1377 woodland because higher rainfall then led to greater grass growth (Dublin et al. 1990). 1378 Loxodonta africana (African elephant) inhibited the recovery and kept it as grassland, and 1379 because of their grazing the grass crop is not enough to support hot fires. On the other hand, 1380 elephants avoid the fire-resistant thickets, which are therefore stable as an alternative stable 1381 state. There are both negative and positive feedback processes here. 1382 Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 44 of 47 1383 1384 Fire repelled 1385 1386 1387 Woody thicket 1388 remains Elephant browsing 1389 repelled 1390 Woody patch escapes 1391 the fire 1392 Catastrophic fire, 1393 triggered by an 1394 exceptional event1 1395 Trees Woody plants killed, re-establish 1396 at least above ground 1397 1398 Fire frequency Less palatable 1399 reduced forbs dominate 1400 More light is allowed 1401 into the field layer 1402 Litter buildup 1403 reduced 1404 1405 1406 Invasion of Grass growth promoted woody species Fewer old 1407 Litter decays suppressed leaves 1408 faster 1409 1410 1411 Regular Trampling Grazing 1412 fires 1413 Low 1414 High rain rain The grazing Low 1415 resource increases rain Herbivore density 1416 increases 1417 1418 1 1419 A rainy season giving a heavy crop of grass followed by a dry season; or a dry season 1420 making savannah burnable; or high browser density causing CWD accumulation 1421 = switch 1422 Fig. 2.8: Pathways of community change in dry savannah. 1423 9 Conclusion 1424 We have described the many processes that can be involved in the development of Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 45 of 47 1425 mixed species stands. Most types of interference, subvention, litter effects and autogenic 1426 disturbance are based on the universal principle of reaction, that organisms in general, and 1427 plants in particular, modify their physical environment. All these reactions have effects on 1428 neighbours. The wide variety of modifications and the wide variety of responses by 1429 neighbours are the reasons for the long list of interactions we gave, a list that we tried to make 1430 exhaustive, but surely have not. 1431 Plants must be seen within the framework of the ecosystem. Autotrophs make up most 1432 of the stored living carbon framework of terrestrial ecosystems, i.e. its biomass, and a plant’s 1433 biomass is also a necessary part of its work in foraging for light and soil resources. But 1434 phytomass is never alone. Operators from other trophic levels are always present and can 1435 have profound influences on plant species presence, adding to the complexity. Clements 1436 envisaged reaction as being on the physical environment. We are reluctant to expand this to 1437 the biotic environment because of the ensuing complications, but we have listed interactions 1438 “mediated by other trophic levels”. There are complex interactions amongst heterotrophic 1439 levels too, which will also impact on our dear plants. Paine’s (1969) original usage of 1440 ‘keystone species’ (chap. 5, sect. 11) is a clear expression of these. 1441 The plant community comprises continual renewal: disturbance, death, establishment, 1442 growth and both sexual and vegetative reproduction of great numbers of offspring – greater 1443 than the habitat can support. The negative and positive interactions that we have listed 1444 moderate those processes. In the next chapter we consider how those processes operate at the 1445 whole-community level to determine successional changes after allogenic or autogenic 1446 disturbance and to affect the spatial pattern of vegetation. 1447 Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 46 of 47 1448 1449 Table 2.1. Effects of litter on the population dynamics of species in the community, as reported in the literature. 1450 1451 1452 1453 1454 1455 1456 1457 1458 1459 1460 1461 1462 1463 1464 1465 1466 1467 1468 1469 1470 1471 1472 1473 1474 1475 1476 1477 1478 1479 1480 1481 1482 1483 1484 Feature Change References Germination (or emergence) reduced or delayed Hamrick and Lee 1987 Peterson and Facelli 1992 Bosy and Reader 1995 Facelli and Kerrigan 1996 Molofsky et al. 2000 Barrett 1931 Facelli and Kerrigan 1996 Myster and Pickett 1993 Cintra 1997 Hamrick and Lee 1987 Myster 1994 Berendse et al. 1994 Xiong and Nilson 1997 Facelli et al. 1999 Facelli and Kerrigan 1996 Facelli et al. 1999 Barrett 1931 Hamrick and Lee 1987 Facelli and Pickett 1991a Moore 1917 Facelli and Facelli 1993 Facelli 1994 Nilsson et al. 1999 Facelli et al. 1999 Monk and Gabrielson 1985 Peterson and Facelli 1992 Facelli and Facelli 1993 Molofsky et al. 2000 Peterson and Facelli 1992 Facelli et al. 1999 Beatty and Sholes 1988 Facelli and Pickett 1991b Beatty and Sholes 1988 Helvey and Patric 1965 1485 ILLUSTRATIONS 1486 Fig. 2.1. Replacement and additive comparisons in experiments that examine the interaction 1487 increased Seed predation reduced Establishment reduced increased Seedling mortality Biomass increased increased reduced Shoot:root ratio increased Species composition changed Species habitat range Water interception changed increased between species. 1488 Fig. 2.2. In Trifolium subteranneum, the results of competition affect competitive ability. 1489 Fig. 2.3: A possible mechanism for size-asymmetric competition belowground, when nutrient 1490 1491 1492 supplies become available patchily in time and space, e.g. from sheep micturition. Fig. 2.4: The effect of R/FR light, as seen in surrounds of different colours. After Novoplansky (1990). Wilson & Agnew, chapter 2, Interactions, page 47 of 47 1493 Fig. 2.5: The seasonal pattern of litterfall in an oakwood. After Christensen (1975). 1494 Fig. 2.6. The process of peat accumulation in mires dominated by Empodisma (wire rush) or 1495 1496 Sphagnum moss species. Fig. 2.7: The effect of brushing seedlings of Brassica oleracea for 1.5 min day-1After 1497 1498 Biddington and Dearman (1985). Fig. 2.8: Pathways of community change in dry savannah. 1499 1 Weed ecologists normally use an additive design because they are interested in the effect of a weed on crop, compared to its not being present. 2 Grime added competition for space, but see section 2.2 below. 3 The attempt by neighbouring plants to use the same space can result in contact and damage, but this is not competition, it is autogenic disturbance (section 5). 4 Microscopic algae do move. 5 When planted as 66 % of the mixture.