Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Tony Mowrer is assistant professor of music at California State University, Fresno, where he teaches courses in music education and conducts the Men’s and Women’s Choruses. [email protected] Choral directors use tonal memory tests as one of their ability assessment instruments to gain knowledge about their singers’ tonal memory. Moreover, memory tests can serve as audition information when selecting students for choral ensembles. Louis Diercks reported the implementation of tonal memory tests as part of the audition process for 225– 270 singers at Ohio State University.1 Thomas Wine reported that tonal memory tests are part of all-state audition procedures in many states.2 Authors of choral methods textbooks, including Brinson,3 Hylton,4 and Phillips,5 suggest the value of their function and usefulness in assessing singers’ tonal memory abilities. Frequently, many students who audition for choral ensembles prove to be poor sight-readers. Consequently, conductors require certain musical assessment information to help determine not only whether their students may become better readers, but also whether they are potential candidates for learning the vast and varied repertoire presented throughout a given school year. Because tonal memory tests have been shown to be good predictors of a student’s musical contribution to a choral ensemble,6 an explication of their characteristics and application is warranted. ory. While in short-term memory, information must be rehearsed, or repeated, to prevent its rapid decay. Most of us experience this decay phenomenon when trying to remember small pieces of information such as an address, a date, or a phone number. If this information is repeated, it becomes associated with other relevant pieces of information already stored in long-term memory. The new information is then placed in the final stage of the memory process: long-term memory. To retrieve information from long-term memory, one must rely on cues that help determine if the information is there. Memory System Before exploring the nature of tonal memory, it may be helpful to have some concept of how the memory system works. According to Loftus,7 information must pass through short-term memory to become part of long-term memChoral Journal • May 2010 21 Tonal Memory in the Choral Audition Almost anything can be a cue: sights, smells, or sounds are common cues. The sight of campfire flames, for example, can be a reminder of a particular unpleasant fire experienced as a child. The sound of screeching tires may be a reminder of an automobile accident. Cues may, of course, be different for different people because the cues and the information to which they are linked are rooted in individual experiences. How much can the average person remember? George Miller8 studied the amount of information a person can place in short-term memory. He assumed that nearly all information given a person is recoverable, if initial observations are correct. If, on the other hand, the observer makes errors, the information recovered will be considerably less than the amount of information originally transmitted. Moreover, Miller assumed that the more information transmitted to the observer, the more mistakes the observer will make when repeating the information. The amount of information accurately repeated will level off at some value. When recalling information, people will begin to become confused as they reach their personal limit. After reviewing several studies about memory, Miller concluded that the limit of a person’s short-term memory constitutes—on average—seven pieces of information. Miller believed that we compensate for this limitation in a variety of ways. For example, one way we compensate is by reorganizing information so that it becomes organized into larger and larger chunks of information. An inexperienced reader, for example, might listen to each individual vowel and consonant as a separate chunk. As the reader becomes more experienced, these sounds become organized as words and, later, as phrases. Miller refers to this reorganization as recoding. His suggestion that a person learns to recode information is important to musicians. A musician routinely recodes tonal information into chunks that make possible the retention of large amounts of music. As with the spoken word, 22 the chunks used to recode tonal information will most likely be related to the musical syntax a person is most accustomed to hearing. Consequently, rather than remembering the individual pitches C– E– G, one may simply remember a pattern as a major triad and reduce the information remembered from three pieces of information to a single piece of information. Davies9 noted that the human mind naturally remembers a melody better than a random sequence of tones. He observed that a melody will always be a sequence of tones, but a sequence of tones will not always be a melody. He further notes that the ability to perceive melody depends on the listener’s ability to organize (recode) tones into a meaningful pattern (chunk). Other researchers (Long,1 0 Dowling,1 1 Pembrook,1 2 ) observed that subjects responded more accurately to tonal melodies than to atonal melodies. Attneave and Olson1 3 found that people rarely attend to individual pitches. Rather, a person gives attention to an entire sequence that is recognizable as a melodic phrase. Korenman and Peynircioglu1 4 observed that musicians learned meaningful melodies (Figure 1) faster than did non-musicians, and they remembered them better than lessmeaningful melodies (Figure 2). Korenman and Peynircioglu also discovered that non-musicians learned less meaningful melodies more rapidly than did musicians. From this study, one may conclude that, although a musician may be expected to learn and perform highly dissonant music, when dealing with short-term memory, as is the case of a tonal memory test, the use of less-meaningful melodies may be more likely to identify non-musicians rather than musicians. Tonal Memory Tests Many choral directors create their own tonal memory tests. These tests usually consist of having a student hear and sing several melodies that increase in melodic difficulty until the student “vocally stumbles.” Some directors may determine the content of each melody by whim of the moment, opinion of the student’s ability, or consideration of the anticipated memory needs of the chorus members. In the case of all-state auditions, however, some states employ a composer Choral Journal • May 2010 to create the tonal memory tests. While their use seems fairly universal, what do we know of how a tonal memory test works? Further, what research can guide us in the creation of appropriate tonal memory tests? With the exception of the study by Korenman and Peynircioglu, procedures used in studies of tonal memory did not require that subjects sing entire melodies. Researchers asked subjects to indicate whether pitches or patterns presented in pairs were the same or different. In some studies, subjects were asked to sing the apparent tonic of a particular pattern or another pitch integral to the pattern played.The procedures of these studies were different from the standard practices of most music teachers, especially those who direct choral ensembles. Typically, choir directors ask students to remember and sing complete melodies. During this author’s 30 years of choral directing, personal observations of many students indicate that they remember short melodic patterns easily while other students are more limited in their ability to remember them.This discrepancy does not seem to be related to a student’s ability to read prior to the audition. In fact, many students who cannot read, display a superior tonal memory. The ability to remember and rehearse short melodic phrases and place them in longterm memory are critical for a singer to function successfully in a choral ensemble. The ability to sing a short melodic pattern involves skill in singing along with memory for musical information. Singing skill requires the ability to coordinate the muscles of the throat, breathing mechanism, and the tongue and mouth with the mind’s ear. Having heard a melody or sequence of pitches, a person must audiate (engage in inner hearing) the pitch sequence to give it meaning.The brain must then tell the throat and related singing muscles what to do so that person can sing the pitches. In essence, two highly involved skill processes are at work; audiation followed by singing. Louis Diercks1 5 questioned whether he found the best candidates for his chorus through the standard audition. He presumed he found the best voices, but wondered whether he was choosing the musicians with the greatest potential. He observed that a student’s performance on sight-singing tests Choral Journal • May 2010 longer may prove to be counter-productive. Doumont1 6 suggests limiting otherwise unstructured material to fewer than seven notes with five as the limit—three notes being optimal. If an ensemble routinely performs atonal music, a director should consider inserting some atonal examples into the tonal memory test in the audition. If this procedure is done, it would be best to limit that sample’s length to between three and five notes. only reflected the student’s present status as a sight-reader rather than that student’s potential strength as a musician. One might presume we all want students who have potential as musicians. Consequently, some thoughts about tonal memory testing might be useful. Based on the information gathered from studies of tonal memory, the following recommendations are made: • An item in a test of tonal memory must be relatively short, approximately seven notes. Miller suggested that a person’s short-term memory would be about seven notes. To find how far a person might stretch, a sample melody might be as long as 9 notes, but anything Choral • An item in a test of tonal memory must be melodic rather than merely a sequence of tones. A melody will always be a sequence of tones but a sequence of tones will not always be a melody. Conductors Workshop Workshop Locations Alexandria, Virginia July 19 – 23, 2010 with Rod Eichenberger A five-day professional development workshop for choral conductors at all levels Cannon Beach, Oregon August 2 – 6, 2010 For more information, contact: George Fox University Performing Arts Department 503-554-2620 s [email protected] or visit choralconductorsworkshop.com 23 Tonal Memory in the Choral Audition Though each figure is short, Figure 3 has a clear tonic and the melodic line begins and ends on the tonic. The melody in Figure 4, however, may be perceived by some to be nothing more than a sequence of tones. A person’s ear listens for melodic information it can use as a cue for the memory (Figure 3). Observe the difference between Figure 3 and Figure 4. • Most, if not all, items in a test of tonal memory should be tonal. Some atonal music sounds much like a sequence of tones rather than a melody, particularly to the musically inexperienced student. Further, as Korenman and Peynircioglu learned, non-musicians seemed better able to repeat the less-meaningful melody than was the musician. If the aim is to identify potential musicians, it seems valuable for the melody to be meaningful. For some students, even a simple triad outlining a major or minor tonality may prove challenging (see Figures 5 and 6). • To discriminate between students with a poor tonal memory (weaker audiation skills) and students with a good tonal memory (stronger audiation skills), it is necessary to include items that are more musically demanding. These items may include melodies that include portions of different chords, played as broken chords (Figure 7). Melodies of this nature give insight into how well a student hears Western musical syntax. November 17 – 21, 2010 Come and meet the international choral world in the United States! All choirs from all continents are welcome! Competitions, gala concerts, encounter concerts, seminars and workshops. Also upcoming: The American International Choral Festival, Reno, May 4-8, 2011 www.interkultur.com INFORMATION & CONTACT: Christina Prucha, [email protected] Phone: (405) 232-8161 Hugh Ballou, [email protected] Phone: (888) 398-8471 24 INTERKULTUR event in partnership with the American Choral Directors Association, in collaboration with St. Louis represented by St. Louis Convention and Visitors Commission and the Regional Arts Commission Choral Journal • May 2010 • Moreover, it is often valuable, as an auditory stretch for the listener, to go a bit beyond the seven-note limit established in Miller’s studies (Figure 8). Additionally, this melody includes a brief modulation. • A common practice when administering tonal memory tests is to play examples in increasing difficulty. In this way, the longer a student is able to accurately sing the examples, the examples become more difficult.When the student can no longer accurately repeat the example, the test is concluded. One rationale given for this approach: students gain confidence as they accurately sing and progress through the easier examples. As students achieve confidence, their ability to sing the more difficult examples increases, allowing them to perform better on the tonal memory test. Although this rationale seems logical, researchers have learned that this approach actually Choral Journal • May 2010 tends to create stress and pessimism on the part of the test-taker, reducing the performance accuracy of the testtaker and reducing the validity of the test. Consequently, it would be best for the tonal memory test to have the simple and difficult items interspersed throughout. Because this juxtaposition of test items serves somewhat to relax the test-taker, the results will be more likely to be valid. For example, it might be wise to administer several of the previous examples in the order shown in Figure 9. Conclusion That we are capable of remembering all that we do is amazing. For the memory system to work, people must take advantage of both short-term memory and long-term memory. New memories first pass through short-term memory where they are repeated and connected with old memories, or cues, after which they may become a part of long-term memor y. Miller suggested that a per son’s shor tterm memory is limited to about 25 Tonal Memory in the Choral Audition seven pieces of information. One way people compensate for this limitation is by reorganizing information into chunks. When working with language, individual letters will be reorganized (chunked) into words, words will be reorganized into sentences, and so on. This is important to musicians who reorganize tonal information into chunks (individual notes become part of longer musical words and sentences) so they can remember large amounts of music.This reorganization will be based on the musical syntax that musician is accustomed to hearing. Researchers have found that people respond better to tonal melodies than atonal melodies. This should not be surprising because of the way the memory system works. People naturally reorganize musical material into chunks that fit the musical syntax they are accustomed to hearing. What may be more surprising is the finding that non-musicians actually learn atonal melodies more quickly and remember them better than musicians. In a large choral program, the audition task may be arduous and time-consuming. To help them make wise membership ap- 26 pointments, conductors often include a tonal memory test in the audition. If properly constructed and administered, the tonal memory test will help a conductor predict which prospective choir member will best serve the ensemble’s needs. If not constructed and administered properly, however, the tonal memory test can potentially, and inadvertently, cause the director to choose poorer musicians. Tonal memory tests that are tonal and consist of about seven notes seem to work the best. This writer hopes that the ideas in this article will assist the choir director in constructing and administering tonal memory tests that help identify students who possess well-developed tonal-memory skills. NOTES 1 Louis H Diercks, “A Prognostic Approach to the Choral Auditions,” Choral Journal (November, 1962), 9–11. 2 Thomas Wine, “All-state Choruses: a Survey of Practices, Procedures, and Perceptions,” Choral Journal (1996, 36), 21–27. 3 Barbara A. Brinson, Choral Music Methods and Materials (Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Group, 1996), 34–36. 4 John B Hylton, Comprehensive Choral Music Education (Upper Saddle River, NJ: PrenticeHall Inc., 1995), 38–39. 5 Kenneth H. Phillips, Directing the Choral Music Program (New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2004), 62– 63. 6 Tony A. Mowrer, “Tonal Memory As An Audition Factor for Choral Ensembles.” (Doctoral Dissertation, Temple University, 1996). 7 Elizabeth Loftus, Memory (Reading, MA: AddisonWesley Publishing Company, Inc., 1980). 8 George Miller, “The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on our Capacity for Processing Information,” Psychological Review (1956, 63), 81–97. 9 John Booth Davies, “Memory for Melodies and Tonal Sequences: A Brief Note,” Bulletin for the Council of Research in Music Education (1981), 66–67. 10 Peggy Long, “Relationships Between Pitch Memory in Short Melodies and Selected Factors,” Journal of Research in Music Education (1977, 25), 272– 82. 11 Jay W. Dowling, “Scale and Contour : Two Components of a Theory of Memory for Melodies,” Psychological Review (1978, 85), 341–54. 12 Randall G. Pembrook, “Interference of the Transcription Process and Other Selected Variables on Perception and Memory During Melodic Dictation,” Journal of Research in Music Education (1986, 34), 238–61. 13 Fred Attneave and Richard Olson, “Pitch As A Medium: A New Approach To Psychophysical Scaling,” The American Journal of Psychology (1971, 84), 147– 66. 14 Lisa M. Korenman and Zehra F. Peynircioglu, “Individual Differences in Learning and Remembering Music: Auditory versus Visual Presentation,” Journal of Research in Music Education (2007, 55), 48 –64. 15 Diercks, 9–11. 16 Jean-Luc Dumont, “Magical Numbers: The SevenPlus-or-Minus-Two Myth,” IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication (2002, 45), 123– 27. Choral Journal • May 2010