Download Self-Monitoring and Susceptibility to the Influence of Self

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts
no text concepts found
Transcript
Self-Monitoring and Susceptibility to the
Influence of Self-Prophecy
ERIC R. SPANGENBERG
DAVID E. SPROTT*
Having people predict whether they will perform a socially normative behavior
increases their probability of performing that target action. Recent empirical evidence supports a dissonance-based theoretical explanation for this self-prophecy
effect. While the effect is robust, few boundary conditions have been identified.
We report two experiments within the dissonance paradigm providing theory-relevant evidence for the moderating effects of self-monitoring on people’s susceptibility to self-prophecy. In particular, low self-monitors are more likely than high
self-monitors to be influenced by self-prediction regarding normative behaviors.
Implications for theory and the practice of social influence are provided.
A
sking people to make a self-prediction regarding a
normative behavior influences the performance of that
behavior in the future. Since its introduction by Sherman
(1980), this self-prophecy effect has been documented
across a wide variety of normative contexts, with demonstrated increases in performing socially desirable actions
such as recycling aluminum cans and voting in an election
and decreases in undesirable behaviors such as cheating on
a take-home exam and implicit gender stereotyping (for a
review, see Spangenberg et al. 2003). The wide variety of
effects and consistently moderate effect sizes (Spangenberg
and Greenwald 1999) reported in prior research clearly suggest promise for the consistent, wide-scale application of
self-prediction to the benefit of society.
While demonstrations of the self-prophecy phenomenon
have been reported in the literature during the past several
years, only recently has evidence begun to accrue regarding
theoretical explication for the effect and under what conditions it manifests. Various theoretical mechanisms have
been proposed for the effects of behavioral self-prediction,
including script evocation and impression management
(Sherman 1980), cognitive dissonance (Spangenberg 1997;
Spangenberg and Greenwald 1999), attitude accessibility
(cf. Morwitz and Fitzsimons 2004), consistency (Cialdini
and Trost 1998), and norm salience (Sprott, Spangenberg,
and Fisher 2003). Given empirical elimination of a number
of these explanations and recent experimental support for
one (Spangenberg et al. 2003), it is reasonably concluded
that cognitive dissonance—as viewed through Aronson’s
self-concept-based conceptualization (1968, 1992)—underlies the self-prophecy effect.
The dissonance-based view of self-prophecy (see Spangenberg et al. 2003) holds that behavioral self-prediction
makes salient social norms associated with the behavior in
addition to a person’s prior failings to perform the behavior
in a socially normative manner. That is, at the time of prediction request, people become simultaneously aware of
what they should do as well as what they have done (or
have not done) in the past. The typically inconsistent nature
of these cognitions (e.g., “I should exercise, I joined a fitness
club, but I have not attended it recently”) confronts the
person’s self-concept as a moral, competent, and good person (Aronson 1968, 1992). Given that a moral, competent,
and good person should behave in concert with social norms
but likely has not always in the past, cognitive dissonance
is thereby elicited through self-prediction. Dissonance then
serves as an aversive state, motivating behavior change in
concert with social norms (also see Fried and Aronson
1995).
Spangenberg et al. (2003) provided wide-ranging experimental evidence consistent with the dissonance-based explanation of self-prophecy. In two studies, they demonstrated that making a prediction about other people’s
behavior (a dissonance-reduction mechanism based on social comparison theory) reduced psychological discomfort
associated with dissonance (Elliot and Devine 1994) after
making a self-prediction. A more elegant, third study found
that a traditional means of reducing dissonance through selfaffirmation (Sherman, Nelson, and Steele 2000) had a sim-
*The authors contributed equally to this article. Eric R. Spangenberg is
dean, Maughmer Chair, and professor of marketing, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164-4750 ([email protected]). David E. Sprott is Gardner O. Hart Distinguished Professor and associate professor of marketing,
Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164-4730 (dsprott@wsu
.edu). The authors thank the editor, associate editor, and two reviewers for
input and guidance on this article. They also thank Ronn J. Smith for his
assistance in data collection. Correspondence concerning this article may
be addressed to either author.
550
䉷 2006 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH, Inc. ● Vol. 32 ● March 2006
All rights reserved. 0093-5301/2006/3204-0008$10.00
SELF-PROPHECY AND SELF-MONITORING
ilar effect on psychological discomfort. A final study demonstrated that the self-affirmation manipulation also reduced
the effect of self-prediction on people’s willingness to donate time to the American Cancer Society. Proposed accounts of self-prophecy such as script evocation and attitude
accessibility cannot account for the diverse pattern of results;
Spangenberg et al. therefore concluded that dissonance underlies self-prophecy. Relatedly, design characteristics of
Spangenberg et al.’s research help to further eliminate attitude accessibility for the effects they observed; specifically,
conditions in four of their experiments were such that attitudes should have been accessible, but they did not produce
effects similar to self-prediction conditions.
Further support for a dissonance-based account of selfprophecy is provided by prior research demonstrating three
moderators of self-prediction. Early research by Greenwald
and colleagues (reported in Spangenberg and Greenwald
1999) found that moderate (as compared with high or low)
prior voting experience produced greater self-prophecy effects on voting behavior. Although these results may reflect
ceiling and floor effects, the lack of self-prophecy effects
for those high and low in prior turnout can also be interpreted within a dissonance paradigm. Voters with high prior
turnout may have been less likely to experience dissonance
due to their norm-consistent past behavior, while voters with
low prior turnout may have had no established normative
beliefs about voting; for both, the important dissonancegenerating mechanisms (i.e., social norms and contrary prior
behavior) may have not existed. Another moderator is the
nature of the self-prediction itself. Sprott et al. (2004) demonstrated stronger self-prophecy effects for specific (compared with more general) self-predictions and suggested a
dissonance-based process such that the discomfort evoked
by a specific (vs. general) self-prediction may be less easily
attributable to sources other than not performing the criterion behavior. Finally, research by Sprott et al. (2003)
showed that self-prophecy effects are greater for those holding stronger, as opposed to weaker, social norms regarding
the predicted behavior. This finding also is interpretable from
a dissonance perspective, given that the greatest dissonance
should manifest for people with the strongest normative
beliefs who have not previously lived up to normative standards. Although these moderators are consistent with a dissonance explanation for self-prophecy, identification of additional theory-relevant moderators is important in order to
further our understanding regarding theoretical underpinnings for the effect.
SELF-PROPHECY AND SELFMONITORING
An underlying premise of this article is that the efficacy
of self-prophecy depends on individual differences among
those making self-predictions. Given that self-monitoring
has been shown to moderate the effects of dissonance (e.g.,
DeBono and Edmonds 1989) and influence the relationships
among attitudes, intentions, and behaviors (e.g., Ajzen, Tim-
551
ko, and White 1982; Zanna, Olson, and Fazio 1980), we
focus our attention on this individual difference. The underlying notion of self-monitoring is that a person’s behavior
is differentially influenced by dispositional and situational
factors (e.g., Snyder 1974). For high self-monitors, behavior
is more often influenced by situational factors, while the
behavior of low self-monitors is influenced most often by
dispositional factors. A premise of much self-monitoring
research is that attitudes serve different functions for high
and low self-monitors, each group being more persuaded
by messages that match the function served by their attitudes. Low self-monitors have consistently been shown to
be influenced by messages making appeals to values (i.e.,
attitudes serving a value-expressive function), while high
self-monitors are influenced by appeals to status (i.e., attitudes serving a social-adjustive function; see Lavine and
Snyder 1996), with evidence that attitudes of low self-monitors are more available relative to those of high self-monitors (Kardes et al. 1986).
A dissonance-based explanation of self-prophecy holds
that a values-action discrepancy is made salient by the act
of self-prediction regarding a socially normative behavior.
In other words, a person completing a prediction is made
aware of, and considers, the social norms surrounding the
predicted behavior and prior failures to act normatively regarding the behavior. This combination of normative consideration and previous (in)action is argued to confront the
self-concept of the person, thereby inducing dissonance.
This confrontation of the self-concept should be more effective for low self-monitors because their behaviors and
attitudes are generally based on values, in contrast to high
self-monitors, who base behaviors and attitudes on situational factors (e.g., DeBono and Edmonds 1989). Further
support for this proposed process is provided by research
demonstrating that the effects of self-discrepancies (Higgins
1987) are related to self-monitoring such that affective states
of low (vs. high) self-monitors are negatively influenced by
self-discrepancies related to an own (vs. other) viewpoint
(Gonnerman et al. 2000). Our fundamental hypothesis therefore is that the self-prophecy technique is more likely to
influence low versus high self-monitors.
High and low self-monitors have been shown to differ
regarding cognitive dissonance, a finding bearing upon theoretical explanation of self-prophecy. Snyder and Tanke
(1976) showed that low self-monitors were more likely to
change their attitudes after writing a counterattitudinal essay
than were high self-monitors, reflecting the fact that internal
beliefs are more important to low self-monitors and therefore
are more susceptible to dissonance. Both low and high selfmonitors may experience dissonance, albeit from different
sources. In particular, DeBono and Edmonds (1989) provided evidence that attitudes of high (vs. low) self-monitors
were more affected after writing a counterattitudinal essay
in opposition to peers and that attitudes of low (vs. high)
self-monitors were more influenced after writing an essay
opposing their values.
Thus, we propose that low self-monitors should be more
JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
552
likely than high self-monitors to attribute prior failure to
behave normatively to their own disposition. By attributing
prior failures to themselves, low self-monitors are more
likely to have their self-concept threatened by having made
a self-prediction and thereby more likely to experience cognitive dissonance. In contrast, high self-monitors are more
likely to consider their past failures to perform normatively
as having been due to situational (not dispositional) factors;
there is little or no threat to their self-concept, and dissonance is therefore less likely to be evoked. These differing
sources and resultant levels of dissonance should produce
larger self-prophecy effects for low (vs. high) self-monitors.
EXPERIMENT 1
Method
Design, Sample, and Procedure. The experiment employed a 2 (self-monitoring: low vs. high) # 2 (self-prophecy: control vs. self-prediction) between-participants design.
The study was conducted in a behavioral laboratory in
groups of up to 20 U.S. undergraduate participants (N p
125) receiving course credit for participation. Upon entering
the lab, participants completed the self-monitoring scale and
the prediction survey, followed by an unrelated 20–30 min.
filler task. Participants then completed the dependent measure—commitment to participate in a health and fitness assessment offered by their university (Sprott et al. 2003).
Independent Variables. Participants completed the 13item Lennox and Wolfe (1984) revised (i.e., of Snyder’s 1974)
self-monitoring scale (a p .83 ) and were classified based on
a median split into low (M p 3.04, n p 64) and high
(M p 3.85, n p 59) self-monitoring groups (t(121) p
14.47, p ! .001). The self-prophecy manipulation followed
procedures used in multiple published studies (e.g., Sprott
et al. 2003), with participants randomly assigned to control
(n p 59) and self-prediction (n p 64) conditions. In both
conditions, participants completed a series of predictions,
including the choice of transportation from an airport, selection of a restaurant in Europe, and response to observing
shoplifting. The self-prophecy (or control) manipulation always appeared as the second prediction on the survey. In
the self-prophecy condition, participants were provided with
the following:
A health and fitness assessment is locally available to you.
The assessment will evaluate your overall physical fitness
and health and is offered free of charge to you as a student
at the university you attend.
Q. Do you predict that:
a. You will not participate in the health and fitness assessment.
b. You will participate in the health and fitness assessment.
Response options were counterbalanced. In the control condition, the fitness assessment prediction was replaced by
a prediction regarding tipping at a restaurant after poor
service.
Dependent Measure. Following Sprott et al. (2003,
2004), participants were informed of the opportunity to take
part in a free health and fitness assessment via a memo
provided at the end of the experimental session. The memo,
on university health and wellness services letterhead, described the nature of the assessment: “The health and fitness
assessment provides individuals with a clear picture of their
overall physical well-being and is based on a variety of
measures of healthfulness. The assessment takes approximately 15 minutes. The assessment program is free of charge
to members of [the university] including students, faculty
and staff.” After this description, participants were asked to
indicate their willingness to participate in the assessment.
The primary dependent variable appearing on the memo
asked participants, “Are you interested in participating in
the health and fitness assessment?” Response options of
“yes” and “no” were counterbalanced. Scheduling and contact information was requested; participants committing to
the assessment had to complete this information in order to
be considered valid respondents (two participants indicating
a commitment were dropped for failure to provide contact
information). It is noteworthy that, while the current research
employs a behavioral intention measure, prior research has
documented self-prediction effects on actual behavior at two
different health clubs (Spangenberg 1997; Spangenberg et
al. 2003).
Results
There was an overall influence of self-prediction on participants’ commitment to the health and fitness assessment
(x 2 (123) p 6.09, p p .01; r p .22). There was higher
commitment among those making a self-prediction (46.9%)
compared with those making a control prediction (25.4%).
As hypothesized and depicted in figure 1, the effect of selfprophecy remained (and actually strengthened) for participants low in self-monitoring (x 2 (64) p 9.23, p p .002;
r p .38), and there was no effect of self-prediction for participants high in self-monitoring (x 2 (59) p 0.12, p p .73;
r p .05).
Discussion
Experiment 1 provides support for our proposition that
self-monitoring moderates the effectiveness of self-prophecy. In particular, people low in self-monitoring were more
susceptible to the effects of self-prediction regarding a socially normative behavior than were those high in self-monitoring. This effect is replicated in experiment 2, using a
different context with a new sample.
SELF-PROPHECY AND SELF-MONITORING
FIGURE 1
MODERATING INFLUENCE OF SELF-MONITORING ON
PREDICTION REQUESTS REGARDING COMMITMENT
TO HEALTH AND FITNESS ASSESSMENT
553
Dependent Measure. The focal variable was participants’ willingness to make a donation of their time to the
American Cancer Society. After a filler task, participants
were presented with a memo from the American Cancer
Society describing the nature of the organization and work
associated with those donating their time: “Volunteers provide a variety of valuable services to the American Cancer
Society including: staff of local offices; coordinating Relay
for Life activities; operate informational booths at events.”
At the end of the memo, participants were asked, “Will you
donate a few hours of your time to assist the American
Cancer Society in your community?” Response options of
“yes” and “no” were counterbalanced. Scheduling and contact information (phone and e-mail) were requested from
those indicating that they were interested in donating time
(lacking contact information, one participant was dropped).
Results
EXPERIMENT 2
Method
Design, Sample, and Procedure. The 2 (self-monitoring: low vs. high) # 2 (self-prophecy: control vs. selfprediction) between-participants design and procedures of
experiment 1 were replicated in the context of influencing
time donations to the American Cancer Society (cf. Sherman 1980; Spangenberg et al. 2003). The sample included
U.S. undergraduates (N p 87) receiving course credit for
participation.
Independent Variables. As in experiment 1, participants were classified using the Lennox and Wolfe (1984)
scale (a p .77) based on a median split as low (M p
3.16, n p 37) or high (M p 3.79, n p 49) self-monitors
(t(84) p 11.52, p ! .001). Participants were also randomly
assigned to either a control (n p 42) or a self-prediction
(n p 44) condition; the only difference in materials from
experiment 1 was the prediction in the self-prophecy condition, which read:
Nonprofit organizations often rely upon donations from people in order to serve society. You receive a request from the
American Cancer Society and you are asked if you would
donate a few hours of your time to the organization.
Q. Do you predict that:
a. You will donate a few hours of your time to the
American Cancer Society.
b. You will not donate a few hours of your time to the
American Cancer Society.
Response options were counterbalanced.
There was no significant influence of self-prediction on
participants’ willingness to donate time to the American
Cancer Society (x 2 (86) p 1.99, p p .16; r p .15), nor was
there an effect of self-prophecy for those scoring high in
self-monitoring (x 2 (49) p .00, p p .99; r p .00). As theoretically predicted, however, there was a significant effect
of self-prediction for participants low in self-monitoring
(x 2 (37) p 4.79, p p .03; r p .36). Figure 2 shows this
interaction.
FIGURE 2
MODERATING INFLUENCE OF SELF-MONITORING ON
PREDICTION REQUESTS REGARDING DONATING
TIME TO THE AMERICAN CANCER SOCIETY
JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
554
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Our experiments demonstrate, across two distinct normative behaviors, that the level of a person’s self-monitoring
moderates the efficacy of self-prophecy as an influence technique. In particular, low self-monitors are more likely to
manifest a self-prophecy effect than are high self-monitors.
Results of the current research are interpretable within a
dissonance paradigmatic understanding of self-prophecy (although process-level evidence of this interpretation should
be provided by future research). In particular, prior research
(Spangenberg et al. 2003) has shown that making a selfprediction regarding the performance of a socially normative
behavior invokes dissonance (in the form of cognitive discomfort), thereby motivating future behavior change that
would otherwise have been less likely to occur. Dissonance
is likely to arise from inconsistent cognitive elements, including the norm about what one should do regarding a
behavior and self-knowledge about conformity to the norm,
which may take the form of memories of specific episodes
or a generalization (e.g., “As a rule, I usually . . . ”). Given
that few behave in normatively appropriate ways 100% of
the time, people should often experience dissonance when
made aware of a discrepancy between self-knowledge and
normative prescription. When subsequently confronted with
a relevant behavioral opportunity, evoked dissonance motivates norm-appropriate behavior. The self-concept interpretation of dissonance (Aronson 1968, 1992) holds that
dissonance is motivational because inconsistency in actions
and thoughts confronts a person’s view of himself or herself
as a moral and competent person. Our interpretation of the
current findings is that the self-concept of low self-monitors
is confronted by the prediction request because it is these
people whose behavior is internally based or driven, in contrast to high self-monitors, whose behavior is externally
driven and who are therefore less likely to be threatened by
the prediction. Thus, our research adds new empirical evidence for a theory-relevant boundary condition to our understanding of self-prophecy.
Self-Prophecy and Related Research
Self-prophecy appears to share a domain with mere measurement, a phenomenon wherein behavioral shifts are elicited by measuring purchase intentions (e.g., Fitzsimons and
Morwitz 1996; Morwitz, Johnson, and Schmittlein 1993)
and recently customer satisfaction (Dholakia and Morwitz
2002). While these effects at first glance may appear similar
(i.e., behavior change occurs after asking a person to consider what s/he will do in the future), close examination of
published empirical findings reveals salient differences. An
obvious difference between the two streams of research is
the much larger effect size associated with self-prophecy
(cf. Spangenberg and Greenwald 1999) as compared with
mere measurement. Another notable difference is that selfprophecy has focused exclusively on socially normative behaviors, whereas mere measurement has examined various
(nonnormative) consumption-oriented behaviors such as the
purchase of consumer goods (e.g., Morwitz et al. 1993).
This contextual difference may be a potential explanation
for the larger effect sizes associated with self-prophecy.
Likely the most important distinction, however, is found in
the theoretical mechanisms thought to underlie the respective effects. Recent empirical evidence suggests different
theoretical explanations for the effects—namely, cognitive
dissonance for self-prophecy and attitude accessibility for
mere measurement. While mere-measurement researchers
have empirically supported an attitude accessibility explanation (Morwitz and Fitzsimons 2004), this account has
been eliminated as an explanation for self-prophecy in several studies by scholars providing evidence for a dissonancebased process (Spangenberg et al. 2003). Thus, although a
prima facie relationship is apparent, methodological differences and empirical evidence argue that self-prophecy is a
phenomenon perhaps distinct from mere measurement, with
clearly separate theoretical underpinnings.
Future Research
Future research initiatives are clearly motivated by the
present studies. Extensions of the current research might
include a study similar to those reported in this article that
assesses cognitive processes (e.g., cognitive discomfort; Elliot and Devine 1994) in the context of self-prophecy and
self-monitoring. This would eliminate a possible rival explanation for the current findings that low self-monitors may
simply hold stronger social norms than do high self-monitors
(cf. Sprott et al. 2003).
While research finds self-monitoring to moderate the effectiveness of a persuasive message (e.g., DeBono 1985),
our work is the first to demonstrate that self-monitoring
moderates the effectiveness of a social influence technique.
Considering prior research on self-monitoring and consumption may usefully extend our reported effects. In particular, it has been demonstrated that during product evaluation, high (vs. low) self-monitors are more likely to be
influenced by country-of-origin information (DeBono and
Rubin 1995), packaging (Debono, Leavitt, and Backus 2003),
and image-oriented advertisements (Snyder and DeBono
1985). Efficacy of self-prediction may therefore be enhanced
for high self-monitors by emphasizing the social-adjustive
aspects of the situation. That is, a self-prediction emphasizing the importance of the focal normative behavior for
peers may allow for a self-prophecy effect to emerge for
high self-monitors (DeBono and Edmonds 1989).
Although boundaries for self-prophecy are being defined,
more research is required to further delineate conditions
under which self-predictions are more or less effective. One
opportunity includes examination of conditions under which
self-predictions are made. In most published self-prophecy
research, people make anonymous self-predictions on a survey. If predictions were administered interpersonally, varying the credibility or expertise of the source could alter
normative aspects of particular behaviors and thereby alter
effectiveness of the technique. For example, a medical doctor asking a patient to make a self-prediction regarding fu-
SELF-PROPHECY AND SELF-MONITORING
ture plans to exercise may increase the likelihood of behavioral change relative to the prediction being administered
by someone with less expertise. Another likely moderator
relates to the nature of the focal behavior for which prediction is made. Behavior-based differences could relate to
people’s level of (perceived) control over the predicted behavior. Prior research has typically focused on behaviors
under the control (at least partially) of the person completing
the prediction; socially nonnormative behaviors under less
personal control (e.g., addictive or compulsive behaviors)
are worthy of investigation in order to determine the efficacy
of self-prediction in these contexts.
Future research aimed at clarifying and understanding the
theoretical (dis)similarities between self-prophecy and mere
measurement is also warranted. In addition to differences
between the paradigms with regard to focal behavior types
and prediction versus intention measures, there exists a clear
divergence regarding theoretical explanations and evidence
(or lack thereof) as to why these effects occur. As noted
previously, we currently believe that these two effects are
likely distinct; existing theoretical evidence and significant difference in the average observed effect sizes are
only two pieces of evidence justifying our belief. Only
research investigating both techniques in the same experimental design with similar behaviors (potentially with process measures) can hope to resolve the emerging, and potentially controversial, theoretical issues surrounding these
observed effects.
Conclusion
The social influence of requesting self-prediction has
important practical implications for profit and nonprofit
enterprises interested in affecting choice and behavior.
A substantial amount of research has clearly documented
real-world behavioral effects of this phenomenon. A clear
benefit of the self-prophecy technique is its simplicity: a
question followed by a simple “yes” or “no” response elicits behavioral change in a normative direction. When compared with more elaborate social influence strategies like
the theory of reasoned action (Fishbein and Ajzen 1975),
the benefits of self-prediction with regard to changing behavior seem self-evident. The current research provides
further evidence that normative behaviors are changed after
making a self-prediction, in this case with the strongest
effects for those who are low (as compared with high) in
self-monitoring. Our findings emphasize a fundamental
premise that should be kept in mind by influence agents
desiring to effect normative behavior via self-prophecy:
a self-prediction needs to confront the self-concept of
the person making the prediction, as it does with low selfmonitors.
[Dawn Iacobucci served as editor and Laura Peracchio
served as associate editor for this article.]
555
REFERENCES
Ajzen, Icek, Christine Timko, and John B. White (1982), “SelfMonitoring and the Attitude-Behavior Relation,” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 42 (March), 426–35.
Aronson, Elliot (1968), “Dissonance Theory: Progress and Problems,” in Theories of Cognitive Consistency: A Sourcebook,
ed. R. P. Abelson, E. Aronson, W. J. McGuire, T. M. Newcomb, M. J. Rosenberg, and P. H. Tannenbaum, Chicago:
Rand McNally, 5–27.
——— (1992), “The Return of the Repressed: Dissonance Theory
Makes a Comeback,” Psychological Inquiry, 3 (4), 303–11.
Cialdini, Robert B. and Melanie R. Trost (1998), “Social Influence:
Social Norms, Conformity, and Compliance,” in The Handbook of Social Psychology, ed. Daniel T. Gilbert, Susan T.
Fiske, and Gardner Lindzey, New York: McGraw-Hill, 151–92.
DeBono, Kenneth G. (1985), “Investigating the Social-Adjustive
and Value-Expressive Functions of Attitudes: Implications for
Persuasion Processes,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (February), 279–87.
DeBono, Kenneth G. and Ashley E. Edmonds (1989), “Cognitive
Dissonance and Self-Monitoring: A Matter of Context?” Motivation and Emotion, 13 (December), 259–70.
DeBono, Kenneth G., Amy Leavitt, and Jennifer Backus (2003),
“Product Packaging and Product Evaluation: An Individual
Difference Approach,” Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
33 (March), 513–21.
DeBono, Kenneth G. and Karen Rubin (1995), “Country of Origin
and Perceptions of Product Quality: An Individual Difference
Perspective,” Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17 (August), 239–47.
Dholakia, Utpal M. and Vicki G. Morwitz (2002), “The Scope and
Persistence of the Mere-Measurement Effect: Evidence from
a Field Study of Customer Satisfaction Measurement,” Journal of Consumer Research, 29 (September), 159–67.
Elliot, Andrew J. and Patricia G. Devine (1994), “On the Motivational Nature of Cognitive Dissonance: Dissonance as Psychological Discomfort,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 67 (September), 382–94.
Fishbein, Martin and Icek Ajzen (1975), Belief, Attitude, Intention,
and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Fitzsimons, Gavan J. and Vicki G. Morwitz (1996), “The Effect
of Measuring Intent on Brand-Level Purchase Behavior,”
Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (June), 1–11.
Fried, Carrie B. and Elliot Aronson (1995), “Hypocrisy, Misattribution, and Dissonance Reduction,” Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 21 (September), 925–33.
Gonnerman, Melvin E., Jr., Christopher P. Parker, Howard Lavine,
and Joseph Huff (2000), “The Relationship between SelfDiscrepancies and Affective States: The Moderating Roles of
Self-Monitoring and Standpoints on the Self,” Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 26 (July), 810–9.
Higgins, E. Tory (1987), “Self-Discrepancy: A Theory Relating
Self and Affect,” Psychological Review, 94 (July), 319– 40.
Kardes, Frank R., David M. Sanbonmatsu, Richard T. Voss, and
Russell H. Fazio (1986), “Self-Monitoring and Attitude Accessibility,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12
(December), 468–74.
Lavine, Howard and Mark Snyder (1996), “Cognitive Processing
and the Functional Matching Effect in Persuasion: The Mediating Role of Subjective Perceptions of Message Quality,”
556
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32 (November),
580–604.
Lennox, Richard D. and Raymond N. Wolfe (1984), “Revision of
the Self-Monitoring Scale,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 46 (June), 1349–64.
Morwitz, Vicki G. and Gavan J. Fitzsimons (2004), “The MereMeasurement Effect: Why Does Measuring Intentions Change
Actual Behavior?” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14 (1–2),
64–75.
Morwitz, Vicki G., Eric Johnson, and David Schmittlein (1993),
“Does Measuring Intent Change Behavior?” Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (June), 46–51.
Sherman, David A. K., Leif D. Nelson, and Claude M. Steele
(2000), “Do Messages about Health Risks Threaten the Self?
Increasing Acceptance of Health Messages via Self-Affirmation,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26 (September), 1046–58.
Sherman, Steven J. (1980), “On the Self-Erasing Nature of Errors
of Prediction,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
39 (August), 211–21.
Snyder, Mark (1974), “Self-Monitoring of Expressive Behavior,”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30 (October),
526–37.
Snyder, Mark and Kenneth G. DeBono (1985), “Appeals to Image
and Claims about Quality: Understanding the Psychology of
Advertising,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
49 (September), 586–97.
JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
Snyder, Mark and Elizabeth Decker Tanke (1976), “Behavior and
Attitude: Some People Are More Consistent than Others,”
Journal of Personality, 44 (September), 510–17.
Spangenberg, Eric R. (1997), “Increasing Health Club Attendance
through Self-Prophecy,” Marketing Letters, 8 (January), 23–31.
Spangenberg, Eric R. and Anthony G. Greenwald (1999), “Social
Influence by Requesting Self-Prophecy,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 8 (1), 61–89.
Spangenberg, Eric R., David E. Sprott, Bianca Grohmann, and
Ronn J. Smith (2003), “Mass-Communicated Prediction Requests: Practical Application and a Cognitive Dissonance Explanation for Self-Prophecy,” Journal of Marketing, 67 (July),
47–62.
Sprott, David E., Ronn J. Smith, Eric R. Spangenberg, and Timothy
S. Freson (2004), “Specificity of Prediction Requests: Evidence for the Differential Effects of Self-Prophecy on Commitment to a Health Assessment,” Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 34 (June), 1176–90.
Sprott, David E., Eric R. Spangenberg, and Robert J. Fisher (2003),
“The Importance of Normative Beliefs to the Self-Prophecy
Effect,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 88 (June), 423–31.
Zanna, Mark P., James M. Olson, and Russell H. Fazio (1980),
“Attitude-Behavior Consistency: An Individual Difference
Perspective,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
38 (March), 432– 40.