Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
The Contingency Model: Criticism and Suggestions Author(s): Terence R. Mitchell, Anthony Biglan, Gerald R. Oncken and Fred E. Fiedler Source: The Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 13, No. 3 (Sep., 1970), pp. 253-267 Published by: Academy of Management Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/254963 . Accessed: 14/06/2014 21:22 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . Academy of Management is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Academy of Management Journal. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions The ContingencyModel: Criticismand Suggestions TERENCE R. MITCHELL, ANTHONY BIGLAN, GERALD R. ONCKEN, FRED E. FIEDLER" University of Washington The authors present a critical analysis of Fiedler's Contingency Model of LeadThese criticisms ership Effectiveness. are discussed and various solutions are suggested. It became apparent many years ago" that leadership effectiveness theories must include information about the situation in which leaders worked. One such theory, the so-called "Contingency Model," was presented by Fiedler.3 The basic premise of the theory is that the performance of interacting groups is contingent upon the interaction of leadership styles and the favorability of the situation for the leader. The most important variable underlying much of this research has been the leader's "esteem for his least preferred co-worker" (LPC) score. To obtain this score, the individual rates his LPC on 17 bipolar adjective scales (e.g., pleasant . . . unpleasant), and the sum of these ratings is treated as his LPC score. In general, this score has been viewed as a 1Preparation of this paper was supported by Office of Naval Research Contract NR 177-473, N00014-67-A-0103-0013, Advanced Research Projects Agency (Fred E. Fiedler, Principal Investigator). *The authors are the senior members of the Organizational Research Group, Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington. Dr. Mitchell is Assistant Professor of Psychology and Business. Mr. Biglan is an acting lecturer in the Department of Psychology, as is Mr. Oncken. Dr. Fiedler is Professor of Psychology and of Management and Organization. He is also Director of the Group. 2R. M. Stogdill, "Personal Factors Associated with Leadership: A Survey of the Literature,"Journal of Psychology, 25 (1948), pp. 35-71; C. A. Gibb, "Leadership," Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol II, ed., G. Lindzey (Cambridge, Massachusetts: AddisonWesley, 1954). 3Fred E. Fiedler, "A Contingency Model of Leadership Effectiveness," Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 1, ed., L. Berkowitz (New York: Academic Press, 1964), pp. 149-190, F. E. Fiedler, "Validation and Extension of the Contingency Model of Leadership Effectiveness: A Review of Empirical Findings," Psychologcal Bulletin (in press). 253 This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Academy of Management Journal 254 September measure of leadership style with the high LPC leader seen as being interoriented, and the low LPC leader being seen as personal-relations task-oriented.4 To describe the situation, Fiedler has utilized three major dimensions cf the group-task situation: (1) leader-member relations, (2) task structure, and (3) the leader's position power. By dichotomizing each of these dimensions, eight types of group-task situations have been identified. Situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Leader-Member Relations Good Good Good Good Poor Poor Poor Poor Task Structure Structured Structured Unstructured Unstructured Structured Structured Unstructured Unstructured Position Power High Low High Low High Low High Low These situations are postulated to differ with respect to the degree to which the leader can influence and control his members. More specifically, the leader is seen as having maximum influence in situation 1 and relatively little influence in situation 8. A considerable amount of evidence indicates that a leader with a low LPC score tends to perform well in situations that are very easy or very difficult (situations 1, 2, and 8). In contrast, leaders with a high LPC score perform well in situations that are of medium difficulty (situations 4, 5, and 6). It seems, then, that the LPC score appears to differentiate between effective and ineffective leaders in a variety of situations. The publication of the model has led to extensive research, both by Fiedler and his associates, as well as other social scientists, in an attempt to examine the validity of the model. We will begin this paper by summarizing the validation studies that have been carried out since the model was introduced. Then we will discuss in turn methodological problems with the model, the meaning of the LPC score, and an analysis of the favorability dimension. SUMMARY OF EXPERIMENTAL RESULTS Previous Results Groups from 15 studies, antedating 1963, were classified according to their situational favorableness and the correlation between leader LPC, and performance was then computed for each set of groups. The correlations between the leader's LPC score and the group's effectiveness measgenerated a bowures, when plotted against situational favorableness, leaders low the LPC that distribution performed more indicating shaped favorable and unfavorable in than LPC leaders very very high effectively situations; high LPC leaders performed more effectively in situations intermediate in favorableness (see Figure 1). 'Fred E. Fiedler, A Theory of Leadership Effectiveness (New York: McGraw-Hill,1967). This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions The Contingency 1970 Model: Criticism And Suggestions 255 FIGURE I Correlations Between High LPC: Permissive Considerate J t- 1.00 - 0.80 - 0.60 - 0.40 - Leaders' LPC Scores and Group Effectiveness Plotted for Each Cell x x 0 0 CD . O _ 0.20 </- I -o 4x X- -0. 20 - -0.40 - -0.60 - \ / \ x 4 x X Eta Low LPC: Control Ing Managing -0.80 - -1 .00 '.,r . Favorable for leader Leader-member Task relations Leader position power = 0.43 x t I good str. structure x \ strong II good lit good str. urstr. weak strong -_ I ...i I 1 . IV V VI VIt VIII good mod. mod. mod. mod. poor poor poor poor str. str. unstr. unstr. strong weak strong weak unstr. weak Unfavorable for leader It should be pointed out that one octant was not represented (No. 6) in these studies and that the points on the figure represent studies from a variety of organizations. The locations of these points in a given octant vary widely as do the sample sizes. Validation Evidence Approximately 25 studies were conducted since 1964 which attempted to test the model or various aspects of it. A recent review of the validation evidence indicates that the situational favorableness moderates the correlation between leadership style and group performance.5 An analysis of studies representing a fairly exact test of the Contingency Model, based on the three component dimensions of the situational favorableness classification, generally support the model. Specifically, field studies testing the model indicate, on the whole, a very close relationship between the predicted curve and the obtained curve. Moreover, a significantly large proportion of correlations were in the predicted direction. Laboratory experiments did not support the model in Octant II, and the support for 5Fred E. Fiedler, "Validation and Extension." This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Academy of Management Journal 256 September certain other octants was weak. On the other hand, here, too, the binomial test of correlations supporting the predictions of the model as against those not supporting the model was significant. Altogether, 35 of 44 correlations were in the predicted direction. Nevertheless, the variation from predicted correlations in Octant II suggests caution in assuming either that the model is readily generalizable to laboratory experiments, or that certain conditions existing in field situations can be adequately reproduced in laboratory studies. The data from these field studies and laboratory studies are presented in Table 1. TABLE 1 Summary of Field and Laboratory Studies Testing the Contingency Model Octants Field Studies I -0.64 -0.51 Hunt Hill Fiedler, et al. O'Brien, et al. Laboratory Experiments -0.72 Belgian Navy -0.77 Shima Mitchell II -0.10 Ifl -0.80 0.60 -0.29 IV -0.24 -0.21 0.00 -0.46 0.47 0.37 0.50 -0.26 0.24 0.17 0.34 -0.16 -0.54 VI V 0.21 0.08 0.13 0.71* 0.43 0.38 0.51 Vl/ 0.30 -0.30 0.62 -0.51 0.67* 0.14 -0.45 0.16 0.03 0.07 0.14 Fiedler Executive West Point 0.13 0.35 0.28 0.10 -0.43 -0.32 (Shzypek) 0.10 0.22 0.17 -0.22 0.38 Median, all studies -0.64 0.23 0.21 -0.24 -0.21 -0.29 Median, field studies -0.57 Median, laboratory 0.13 0.16 0.38 -0.72 0.24 -0.16 experiments Medians in original 0.47 0.42 -0.52 -0.58 -0.33 studies Number of correlations in the expected direction ........ Number of correlations opposite to expected direction .... ......... ......... p by binominal test VIIl 0.26 -0.27 -0.37 0.60 0.08 -0.33 0.26 0.30 -0.35 -0.33 0.08 -0.33 0.05 -0.43 .... 34** 11 0.01 *p <05 '*Exclusive of Octant VI, for which no prediction had been made. The remainder of the paper will present criticisms of the model and its component parts. There will also be an attempt to provide solutions to those problems which are discussed. METHODOLOGICAL CRITIQUE OF THE CONTINGENCY MODEL Methodological considerations pertaining to the Contingency Model will be discussed in two sections. First, the measurement of various parameters and variables relevant to the model will be discussed. Then, we will critically examine the general strategy which Fiedler and others have employed to test predictions of the Contingency Model. This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1970 The Contingency Model: Criticism And Suggestions 257 Measure of Parameters As prescribed in the description of the original model, the situational favorability dimension is composed of three components: (1) task structure, (2) leader position power, and (3) leader-member relations. Each component is dichotomized, and the components are combined in the prescribed configural fashion to define the eight octants of the situational favorability dimension. Task structure is operationally dimensions: 1. Goal clarity, 2. Decision verifiability, 3. Solution specificity, and 4. Goal path multiplicity. defined by four of Shaw's6 task A task is rated on each of these four dimensions on an eight-point scale, and a mean scale score of 5.0 is used as a cutting point for dichotomizing task structure. These specifications provide a well-defined standard method of dichotomizing task structure. The model provides an objective measure of leader position power. This measure consists of an 18-item checklist designed to determine the reward, coercive, and legitimate degree to which the leader possesses power, as well as having special knowledge or skills not available to the group members. However, no cutting point is specified for dichotomizing position power, and the split was originally made at the median. This procedure is open to the obvious criticism that the median is likely to shift from study to study, so that a situation of high position power in one study could possibly be considered low position power in another study. A more important problem with the position power dimension, however, is that the scale proposed by Fiedler is omitted in some studies testing the Contingency Model. Thus, in the Belgian Navy Study,7 position power was manipulated by using petty officers (high position power) versus recruits (low position power) as leaders. The manipulation was further strengthened by giving the petty officers the final decision on points of disagreement, while requiring that group decisions be unanimous in the low position power condition. This manipulation of position power has good face validity, but it would have been desirable to follow the guidelines of the proposed checklist. Other studies8 have, however, applied the scale. 6M. E. Shaw, "Scaling Group Tasks: A Method for Dimensional Analysis" (Gainesville, Florida: University of Florida, 1963) (mimeographed). TFred E. Fiedler, "The Effect of Leadership and Cultural Heterogeneity on Group Performance: A Test of the Contingency Model," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2 (1966), pp. 237-264. 8J. G. Hunt, "Fiedler's Leadership Contingency Model: An empirical test in three organizations," Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 2 (1967), p. 290-308. This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 258 Academy of Management Journal September Leader-member relations are typically measured by the leader's score on the group atmosphere scale, on which the leader rates the group situation on items such as friendly-unfriendly, tense-relaxed, etc. This scale is supposed to measure the degree to which the leader feels accepted by the group. There are two problems with the technique of assessing leader-member relations. First, once again no cutting score is specified for dichotomizing the scale, and the split is typically made at the median. There is very little reason to doubt that the median group atmosphere score would vary widely from setting to setting. It would be desirable to have normative data on group atmosphere in a variety of settings to provide cutting scores not dependent on the particular situation. A report by Posthuma'. now provides such information. A second problem with the group atmosphere scale is that it is usually administered after the group session. This is not a fatal flaw, but the investigator should be aware that the group atmosphere score may at least in part be determined by the leader's perception of the group's performance on the task. One possible statistical method for dealing with this problem would be to partial group productivity scores out of the group atmosphere score and then make the split. Leadership style is measured by the leader's responses to the LPC.10 The highest test-retest reliability coefficient reported by Fiedler for the LPC scale is 0.70, indicating that under the best circumstances, the LPC scale has about 50 percent reliable variance. In one study, with an 8week intertest period characterized by military training for the respondents, the test-retest correlation dropped as low as 0.31.1l Assuming that these two coefficients are upper and lower limits, there is some question as to whether the LPC scale is a sufficiently reliable measure of what is presumed to be a relatively enduring trait, and more research on the measure is required. STRATEGY FOR TESTS OF THE MODEL Antecedent Probability model is based on The antecedent evidence for the contingency research in a wide variety of settings with a number of different types of actors. Furthermore, the prior evidence is based on a number of different measures of group productivity. Based on the wide sampling of behaviors, actors, and settings, we can conclude that the model has fairly good external validity. 9A. Posthuma, "Normative Data on the Least-Preferred Co-Worker and Group Atmosphere Questionnaires" (Seattle, Washington: Oreganizational Research, Technical Report No. 70-6). "'Fred E. Fiedler, Leadership. "Ibid, p. 48. This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1970 The Contingency Model: Criticism And Suggestions 259 The predicted correlations between leader LPC and group productivity for each octant are defined as the median correlation within each octant across all antecedent studies. Examination of the correlations used to derive the model12 reveals that at least in some of the octants, the correlations vary over a wide range. Thus, in Octant III, the correlations range from -0.72 to one isolated case of +0.84. This broad range of correlations leaves us with very little confidence in the predicted correlation of -0.33 for Octant ilI and suggests that the classification of group situations needs improvement. The range of correlations in the other octants is less extreme. Evidential Probability The predictions of the Contingency Model are typically tested in field studies and field experiments, although laboratory tests of the model have been conducted. In the field studies,13 the usual procedure has been to study intact groups engaging in their routine activities. Task structure and leader position power were decided either by ratings of judges or by the judgment of the experimenter: group productivity was assessed where possible by objective scores. Leader LPC was obtained where possible either before the study, and Group Atmosphere scores were assessed concurrently or as in the Fiedler, et al.14 study at the end of the field experiment. The groups were then classified into the appropriate octants of situational favorability, and rank-order correlations are computed between leader LPC and group performance within each octant. Because of the partitioning, there were usually no more than three or four octants represented in any study. The field experiments differed from the field studies in that the subjects were assigned tasks especially designed for the study, and in some cases were assigned to groups assembled for purposes of this study.l5 Several methodological problems with these tests of the Contingency Model need to be pointed out. First, only two studies16 tested the predictions of all eight octants of the model, and the results of one 17 were not totally in support of the model as predicted. 12lbid, p. 146. "3Hunt,"A Test of Leadership"; Fiedler, "Validation and Extension"; and Fred E. Fiedler. G. E. O'Brien, and D. R. Ilgen, "The Effect of Leadership Style upon the Performance and Adjustment of Volunteer Teams Operating in a Stressful Foreign Environment," Human Relations (in press). "lbid. "Fred E. Fiedler, "The Effect of Leadership." "Ibid; and G. J. Skrzypek, "The Relationship of Leadership Style to Task Structure Position Power, and Leader-Member Relations" (West Point, New York: U. S. Military Academy, Technical Report No. 34, 1969). "1Fred E. Fiedler, "The Effect of Leadership." This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Academy of Management Journal 260 September Second, as Graen, Alvares, Orris, & Martella18have pointed out, the usual tests of statistical significance were frequently not applied to the correlations used to test the model. Since the number of cases per octant in any one study tends to be very small, the more lenient criterion of being in the hypothesized direction was used. Based on a null hypothesis of a zero correlation, this results in an alpha level of 0.50 for tests of the hypothesis. Graen et al. argue further that a consequence of this strategy is that it is necessary to generate almost the entire sampling distribution of correlations in an octant before the null hypothesis can be rejected. The evidential impact of any one study is therefore relatively small. Nevertheless, the validation studies shown in Table 1 have provided a large number of correlation coefficients in each octant, and while few of the individual correlations differ significantly from zero, the total pattern of correlations are significantly in support of the predictions of the model.19 A final methodological difficulty with the Contingency Model has to do with the partitioning of the situational favorability dimension into homogeneous subsets of groups.20 Fiedler states, The basic hypothesis of the Contingency Model . . . is not tied to the definition of the favorableness dimension in terms of group climate, position power, and task structure. . . . The main question is always how to order situations in terms of their favorableness.21 This implies a research strategy involving partitioning the groups on a number of variables until meaningful results (i.e., supporting the model) are obtained. Just this type of approach was used originally in the Belgian Navy Study,22 although it was not used when the material was analyzed again23 nor was it used in other validation studies shown in Table 1. It is important to guard against this type of research strategy. This type of research strategy has two consequences. First, with continued partitioning, the number of groups within any cell is reduced to a level where meaningful statistical tests cannot be performed. Second, this approach will inevitably lead to a rejection of the null hypothesis over a series of studies. The rejection of the null hypothesis of no systematic relationship would in this case be due not to any true relationship, but rather to the systematic treatment of the data.24 "SG.Graen, D. Alvares, J. Orris, and J. Martella, "The Contingency Model of leadership effectiveness: Antecedent and Evidential Results," Psychological Bulletin (in press). g9Fiedler,"Validation and Extension," p. 16. 20Graen,et al., "The Contingency Model." 2'Fred E. Fiedler, "Leadership." 2Fred E. Fiedler, "The Effect of Leadership." '3Fred E. Fiedler, "Validation and Extension." 2Graen, et al., "The Contingency Model." This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1970 The Contingency Model: Criticism And Suggestions 261 Some Suggestions In the original statement of the Contingency Model, an explicit set of predictions is made concerning the correlation between leader LPC and group performance within each octant of situational favorability. These octants are well defined by a specified configuration of dichotomized scores on leader-member relations, task structure, and leader position power. An empirical test of the Contingency Model requires that groups be classified into octants in the prescribed way, and that the correlations between leader LPC and group productivity be computed. Statistical tests of these correlations should be applied to test their difference from zero and from the predicted correlation in each octant. If a different partitioning of the situational favorability dimension is used, then a different model is implied and must run the gamut of evidential test. If the predictions of the Contingency Model need not be tied to the prescribed definition of situational favorability, then a theory of situational favorability must be developed stating the rules of combination of various parameters of the situation in determining the favorability of the situation for the leader in a variety of settings. Then the more general hypothesis of a curvilinear relationship between situational favorability and the LPCproductivity correlation could be tested in a number of settings. We will discuss such an approach later in this paper. MEANING OF THE LPC SCORE The LPC score has been related to group performance in a large number of studies. The direction of the relationship depends, however, upon the type of situation in which the leader finds himself. The problem has been the interpretation of this score: Assumed Similarity and LPC scores have been the subject of intensive research since 1950. However, these scores have been extremely resistant to meaningful interpretations which related them to personality traits and to consistent behavior patterns. For many years we were in the peculiar position of having a score which seemed to correlate with nothing but group performance.25 The evidence, taken as a whole, suggests that the low LPC person is more concerned with the task, and the high LPC person is more concerned with establishing good interpersonal relations. These interpretations rest upon a wide variety of data. Most important, perhaps, are studies showing that high LPC leaders become more active in the interpersonal relations area, while low LPC leaders become more active in the task area as the leadership situation increases in difficulty; i.e., as the leader has less and less control and influence over the situation.'2 2"FredE. Fiedler, "Leadership," p. 46. 26lbid;D. W. Bishop, "Relations between Task and Interpersonal Success and Group Member Adjustment" (Urbana, Illinois: Group Effectiveness Research Laboratory, University of Illinois, 1964) (mimeographed); and Fred E. Fiedler, 'The Effect of Leadership." This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 262 Academy of Management Journal September In addition, studies by Mitchell'2 have demonstrated a consistent but moderate relationship between LPC and Scott's28 differentiation measure. High LPC persons tend to categorize more finely when asked to classify a set of different groups in as many ways as possible. Also, Bieri29 has pointed out that the high LPC person differentiates more among traits of the individual he describes, a finding substantiated in a number of studies which show that the variance of item scores of high LPC persons is higher than that of low LPC persons. It was also found in a series of lab and questionnaire studies that high LPC people are able to discriminate more finely among the behaviors used by others than were low LPC people. These studies are described in more detail elsewhere.30 A similar result was found for the ability to differentiate among the demands of the task setting,31 again supporting the idea that the LPC scale is a measure of cognitive complexity. It may well be that the greater cognitive complexity of the high LPC person in his perception of co-workers as well as groups, reflects his concern for interpersonal relations in order to achieve need satisfaction. This hypothesis is currently being tested. On the other hand, the evidence is far from clear. A number of studies have not supported the interpretation of the LPC score as relating to task-versus Thus, Steiner32 found low LPC relationship-orientation. more on three social distance measures, be to expansive socially subjects and less extrapunitive than high LPC persons. Nealey (unpublished) found low LPC subjects to state that when they were faced with a hypothetical work situation, they would prefer good interpersonal relations, while high LPC subjects stated that they wished to have an efficient group. Mitchell's33 study showed that high LPC subjects were more cognizant of position power and task structure than were low LPC subjects in judging leadership situations, while the latter relied to a greater extent on the interpersonal relationship between leader and group members. 2T. R. Mitchell, "Cognitive Complexity and Leadership Style," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (in press). 29W.A. Scott, "Cognitive Complexity and Cognitive Flexibility," Sociometry, 25 (1962), p. 405-414; and Idem, "Conceptualizing and Measuring Structural Properties of Cognition," Motivation and Social Interaction: Cognitive Determinants, in 0. J. Harvey (ed.) (New York: Ronald, 1963), p. 266-288. as a Personality Variable in Cognitive and Prefer29J. Bieri, "Complexity-Simplicity ential Behavor," Functons of Varied Experience, D. W. Fiske and S. R. Maddi (eds.), (Homewood, Illinois: Dorsey, 1961), p. 355-379. 30T. R. Mitchell, "Leader Complexity, Leadership Style, and Group Performance" (Ph.D. diss., Urbana, Illinois: University of Illinois, 1969). 31U. G. Foa, T. R. Mitchell, and F. E. Fiedler, "Differentiation Matching," Behavioral Science (in press). 321. D. Steiner, "Interpersonal Orientation and Assumed Similarity between Opposites" (Urbana, Illinois: Group Effectiveness Research Laboratory, 1959) (mimeographed). ST. R. Mitchell, "Leader Complexity, Leadership Style." This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1970 The Contingency Model: Criticism And Suggestions A reanalysis of research on LPC suggests that these findings can be integrated into the framework of a goal preparation). 263 contradictory hierarchy (in THE SITUATIONAL FAVORABLENESS DIMENSION Validity Recall that the situational favorableness dimension is composed of three variables; i.e., leader-member relations, task structure, and position power, and that the leader-member relations are deemed to be the most important. Three different measures have been used by Fiedler to assess these relations: 1. The leader's rating of the group atmosphere (the sum of 10 bi-polar scales describing the situation), 2. The members' ratings of group atmosphere, and 3. The degree to which the leader is sociometricaily chosen by group members. The first of these has been used most frequently. Ratings of group atmosphere may well reflect aspects of group climate other than the relations between leader and members. For example, low group atmosphere ratings could result from tension between members of the group and not reflect poor relations between the leader and members. The leader's rating of group atmosphere depends upon the assumption that the leader can accurately judge the climate of the group, but it is his position which seems most likely to create the possibility of inaccurate perception. In fact, studies by Mitchell, Fiedler ,and O'Brien34 indicate low intercorrelations between these measures. These findings suggest that sociometric measures and group atmosphere ratings by different participants do not compose a single dimension. The sociometric measure appears to have the best face validity as a measure of the quality of leader-member relations. Unfortunately, it has been used less frequently than the leader's rating of group atmosphere. We need, also, to ask about the construct validity of the favorability dimension. The question here is, "Is the dimension related to other variables in the theory in the way that the theory suggests?" A first and obvious as situational prediction is that perceived favorableness will decrease favorableness decreases. At least two studies support this prediction. Mitchell35 found that leaders did perceive favorability to be lower in 3"T.R. Mitchell, "The Construct Validity of Three Dimensions of Leadership Research," Journal of Social Psychology, 80 (1970), p. 89-94; Fred E. Fiedler, "Leadership": G. E. O'Brien, "Methods of Analyzing Group Tasks" (Urbana, Illinois: Group Effective Research Laboratory, Technical Report No. 46 [67-2] 1967). 35T.R. Mitchell, "Leader Complexity, Leadership Style." This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 264 Academy of Management Journal September unfavorable situations. Chemers and Summers3'1 asked subjects to rate the favorability of hypothetical group situations for which member-leader and member-member relations were described as being either positive or negative. They found that both of these factors were associated with significant differences in the ratings of favorability of the situation. Fiedler;7 also found that leaders were more anxious in the less favorable situations than in the more favorable ones. A second prediction which follows from the definition of situation favorability is that actual influence of the leader will differ according to favorability. Actual influence could be measured by the degree of actual compliance to specific influence attempts of the leader. As yet no such direct tests of the interpretation of this dimension have been attempted. Finally, we need to consider the possibility that alternate interpretations of this dimension can be made. Foa, Mitchell, and Fiedler38 suggest as one alternative that the underlying dimension is situation complexity V11 , and VIII comprise situations which rather than favorability. Octants 1, 1, the than other situations (in that everything is may be seen as simpler between LPC and measures correlations The positive "good" or "bad"). that an of cognitive complexity suggest important component of the variance in LPC is cognitive complexity. Thus, the positive correlations between LPC and performance found in Octants IV and V may then be due to the greater ability of the cognitively more complex, high LPC leaders to deal with more complex situations. Negative correlations in the other octants are interpreted as resulting from the lower complexity of these situations. To summarize, a number of problems appear to exist with respect to the validity of the favorability dimension. The various measures of the quality of leader-member relations have face validity, but do not converge to a satisfactory degree. Construct validity for the dimension is not complete, and a plausible alternate explanation of the dimension exists. Other Criticisms of the Favorability Dimension A variety of other critcisms of the favorability dimension may be made. Many of the criticisms which follow have been made elsewhere by other authors (O'Brien and Fiedler:`). it should be noted before launching into criticism that the dimension was developed in 1963. At that time, the dimension represented a more sophisticated and complete treatment of the situation than could be found elsewhere. A search for a more satisfactory method of defining this dimension has continued. 36M. M. Chemers and D. A. Summers, "Group Atmosphere and the Perception of Research Laboratory, TechGroup Favorableness" (Urbana. Illinois: Group Effectiveness nical Report No. 71 [68-16] 1968). 37Fred E. Fiedler, "Leadership." '3U. G. Foa, T. R. Mitchell, and Fred E. Fiedler, "Differentiation Matching." "G. E. O'Brien, "Group Structure and the Measurement of Potential Leader Influence," Australian Journal of Psychology, 21 (1969), p. 277-289; and Fred E. Fiedler, "Leadership." This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions The Contingency Model: Criticism And Suggestions 1970 265 Absence of a Metric of Situation Favorability The Contingency Model treatment of favorability does not provide a way of quantifying the favorability of a particular group situation. The favorability of the group situation can only be obtained through comparison of the group with other groups. A procedure which provided quantification of favorability would alleviate some of the statistical obstructions to the test of the model: the interaction of the measure of favorability and LPC on performance could be examined. Problem of Adding Other Dimensions Other measures of situation favorability have been used. They include ratings of stress, tenure of leader, cultural heterogeneity, etc. No procedure has been specified to date as to how these measures should be combined with the usual factors in the favorability dimension. Moreover, the adequacy of these measures as measures of situation favorability typically was not established. This leaves the possibility open of generating supporting results for the Contingency Model by choosing from among the many measures, which are purported to be of situation favorability, those which produce the curvilinear relationship between LPC and performance. This is obviously not tenable from a methodological point of view. Important Aspects of the Situation are Neglected The influence of the leader in group situations may be a function of variables which are not included in the Contingency Model, Wearing and Bishop40 have shown the influence of member LPC and attitudes. In addition, O'Brien41 cited two such variables. First, in organizations, the authority relationships above the supervisor may affect his ability to influence group members. Second, aspects of the task, besides its structuredness, may affect favorability. For example, Vroom and Mann42 suggest that the cooperation requirements of the task may affect the relative importance of task- and relationship-oriented behaviors for performance. Favorableness is Leader-Centered The leader-member, relations focus on only the relationships between leaders and members and do not include consideration of the relations among group members. It is possible that these relationships are important for the ability of the leader to influence group members. Situational 40A.Wearing, and D. W. Bishop, "Leader and Member Attitudes toward Co-Workers, Intergroup Competition, and the Effectiveness and Adjustment of Military Squads" (Urbana, Illinois: Group Effectiveness Laboratory, Technical Report No. 21, 1967). 41G.E. O'Brien,"GroupStructure." 4'. H. Vroom and F. C. Mann, "Leader Authoritarianism and Employee Attitudes," Personnel Psychology, 13 (1960), pp. 125-240. This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 266 Academy of Management Journal September To summarize, an improved treatment of situation favorability should provide a quantitative index of favorability, a consistent system for weighing aspects of the situation, and it may need to include other aspects of the situation known to affect leader influence. Potential Influence O'Brien4: provides an important response to the problems which have just been discussed. He derived a measure of the potential influence of the leader which is quantitative and includes consideration of the structure of the group as a whole. The measure stems from the structural role theory framework for the treatment of organization structure.44 Structural role theory treats the structure of the organization in terms of the relationships among three elements: (1) persons, (2) positions, and (3) tasks. Six relationships are possible among these elements: 1. Person-to-person relations (e.g., liking, communication); 2. Assignment of persons to positions; 3. Power relations among positions; 4. Allocation of tasks to positions; 5. Precedence relations among tasks; and 6. Person-to-task relations (e.g., liking for task, time actually spent on task). The relation between two elements may be represented in a digraph by a directed line from the point representing the first element to the point representing the second element. Figure 11 presents an example of a Figure 2 Structural Role Theory Digraph Informal Persons Relationships h Assignment " Positions Power R1 P2 P1 Allocation Tasks tt Precedence R2 t2 Elements and Relationships of Organizational Structure (after Oeser, and Harary, 1962).* *hl and h, are persons; pi and p. are positions; and ti and t2 are tasks. Informal relationships (R,) such as liking and communication connect persons. Positions are connected by power relationships (R1). Tasks are connected by precedence relationships (R2), which are relationships that define the order in which subtasks must be completed. 3G. E. O'Brien, "Group Structure." A. Oeser, and F. A. Harary, "A Mathematical Model for Structural Role Theory, I," Human Relations, 15 (1962), pp. 89-109; Idem, "A Mathematical Model for Structural Role Theory, !I," Human Relations, 17 (1964), pp. 3-17; and 0. A. Oeser and G. O. O'Brien, "A Mathematical Model for Structural Role Theory, III,"Human Relations, 20 (1967), pp. 83-97. 440. This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1970 The Contingency Model: Criticism And Suggestions 267 simple role structure. A path is said to exist between two points if it is possible to trace from the first point to the second point via directed lines. Thus, in Figure II, a path exists between h, and P., via Pi. However, no paths exist between P1 and hl, since the directed lines do not go toward hi. O'Brien uses the notion of a path to define an index of potential influence. The leader's potential influence is defined by the "ratio between the number of paths connecting him to the task system and total number of paths between persons and tasks." Matrix algebra algorithms are provided for the calculation of the index. The value of this approach measuring situational favorableness is: (a) It allows quantification of group situations; (b) It presumably includes most of the important relationships making up group structure, and (c) It treats the entire structure of the group. O'Brien45 used this index as a measure of situation favorability in a test of the Contingency Model. He found that the correlations between LPC and performance were related in a curvilinear fashion to the index of potential influence. This suggests that the measure is tapping the same construct as Fiedler's dimension and, as pointed out, may avoid some of the problems with the dimension as it is presently operationalized. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The Contingency Model has become one of the major theories of leadership effectiveness. Validation studies have provided strong support for this theory which has led to important new insights into the leadership process. This critique pointed to various aspects of the model which are in particular need of further research. More specifically, we see the need for new interpretations of the LPC variable and an alternate method for assessing situational favorableness. We have also pointed out a number of methodological problems with the model and have attempted to suggest remedies for them. '5G. E. O'Brien, "Group Structure." This content downloaded from 62.122.73.34 on Sat, 14 Jun 2014 21:22:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions