Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
So I Found A Paper – now what do I do? How to quickly read a paper and critically (ap)praise it Dr Ben Mitchell MBBS BSc, FRACGP Medical Educator GPTQ Lecturer The University of Queensland Session Outline • SHOULD I CARE? (How to read a paper in 5 minutes) • SHOULD I BELIEVE THE FINDINGS? (Approach to critically appraising the paper and interpreting the results) • SO WHAT? (are the results clinically meaningful) • Some online resources to help Online critical appraisal tools Helpful statistical articles Who’s confident? 1 = Not confident 2 = Done a little before 3 = Fairly confident 4 = Pretty confident actually 5 = Very confident Why “how to read a paper?” • It can be difficult • Its not like reading a book • You want to be able to decide quickly if it will answer your question (and thus help you decide how you will spend the next 30 mins of your life) • It gets easier with practice A full process might involve: • a one or two sentence summary of the paper. • a deeper, more extensive outline of the main points of the paper, including for example assumptions made, arguments presented, data analyzed, and conclusions drawn. • any limitations or extensions you see for the ideas in the paper. • your opinion of the paper; primarily, the quality of the ideas and its potential impact Step 1: Should I care? • Don’t read it the whole way through first time • Make notes as you read the paper • After the first read-through, try to summarize the paper in one or two sentences. What is this about? What did they actually do? – Usually just title and abstract Have a go with your paper • Question 1: – Why was the study done, – What research question were the authors addressing? Population Intervention Comparator Outcome (Time) Don’t worry about results just yet Don’t worry about critical appraisal just yet Step 2: SHOULD I BELIEVE THE FINDINGS? (Critical Appraisal) • Be suspicious • Read Critically • Not all that is claimed may be true Step 2: SHOULD I BELIEVE THE FINDINGS? 3 things that contribute to a result? Results • Three reasons for getting the results found: 1. Bias Critical appraisal helps here 2. Chance Stats helps here 3. True result (or placebo) (but is it clinically meaningful?) Experience/clinical knowledge helps here Results • Three reasons for getting the results found: 1. Bias Critical appraisal helps here 2. Chance Stats helps here 3. True result (or placebo) (but is it clinically meaningful?) Experience/clinical knowledge & study design helps here If you are deciding whether a paper is worth reading, you should do so on the design of the methods section and not: – the interest of the hypothesis – the nature or potential impact of the results, or – the speculation in the discussion. • Most papers come in the IMRaD format Question 2: Correct study design? • • • • • Cohort? Case Report? Case control? RCT? Systematic review? Have a go with your paper • Question 1: – Why was the study done, – What research question were the authors addressing? • Question 2: What type of study was done? – Is it the correct one for that question? – First, decide whether the paper describes a primary study, which reports research first hand, or a secondary one, which attempts to summarise and draw conclusions from primary studies. Don’t worry about results just yet Use the provided document to help you Questions to do with drug treatment or other medical interventions should be addressed by double blind, randomised controlled trials Questions about prognosis require longitudinal cohort studies, and those about causation require either cohort or case control studies Case reports, though methodologically weak, can be produced rapidly and have a place in alerting practitioners to adverse drug reactions Was the study design appropriate to the research? Rapid critical appraisal using an appraisal tool • Different study designs call for different steps to reduce systematic bias Use the tool provided to help you look for possible sources of bias Most of the information you will need will be in the methods section • Smith, Jill P Pell Results • Three reasons for getting the results found: 1. Bias Critical appraisal helps here 2. Chance Stats helps here 3. True result (or placebo) (but is it clinically meaningful?) Experience/clinical knowledge helps here Your turn • Take a look at the results of your paper • Write down the main findings and note the statistics used (p value, Confidence intervals) Messerli (NEJM 2012) • What was the research question? • What study design was it? Correlation vs causation A correlation between X and Y does not prove causation • but indicates that either X influences Y, • Y influences X, or • X and Y are influenced by a common underlying mechanism. Consider: • Sleeping in shoes is associated with morning headaches • Bigger foot size correlates with better reading ability in children • Sitting at the front of lecture theatres is associated with better marks in assessment RCT - Swayden et al (Patient Education & Counseling 2012) • What was the research question? • What study design was it? RCT - Swayden et al (Patient Education & Counseling 2012) • Difference in perception of time spent P = 0.01 What does a p value mean? What does a p value mean? • P values are a statement about statistical significance. • Hypothesis tests (also known as significance tests) and P values, are used to assess the strength of the evidence against the null hypothesis that there is no true association in the population that the sample was drawn. • A P value, is the probability of getting a difference between the two groups, at least as large, than that was observed, if the null hypothesis was true. This value is the P value. • Thus, if the probability of getting such a difference is very, very small, then we can say because this is so unlikely, we can reject the null hypothesis, and accept our hypothesis. • Korownyk et al (BMJ 2014) • What was the research question? • What study design was it? • What were the results? Results from Korownyk et al • “We could find at least a case study or better evidence to support 54% (95% confidence interval 47% to 62%) of the 160 recommendations (80 from each show). • For recommendations in The Dr Oz Show, evidence supported 46%, contradicted 15%, and was not found for 39%. • For recommendations in The Doctors, evidence supported 63%, contradicted 14%, and was not found for 24%. • Believable or somewhat believable evidence supported 33% of the recommendations on The Dr Oz Show and 53% on The Doctors.” • Korownyk et al (BMJ 2014) • What does a confidence interval mean? • Wide CI vs narrow CI Results • Three reasons for getting the results found: 1. Chance – Stats helps here 2. Bias – Critical appraisal helps here 3. True result – But is it clinically meaningful? – Experience/clinical knowledge helps here RCT • Furyk et al (MJA 2009) • We know the p value. • What is the clinical significance of the results? RCT • Furyk et al (MJA 2009) • We know the p value. • What is the clinical significance of the results? • Statistical difference is not the same as Clinical Difference Evidenced Based Practice Patient values Best Research evidence Clinical experience Remember Thankyou • Testing treatments.org (great book) • www.jameslindlibrary.org • https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=z1b2hFzX srU • http://www.bmj.com/about-bmj/resourcesreaders/publications/how-read-paper – Trisha Greenhalgh BMJ Journal articles • http://www.youtube.com/watch?NR=1&v=XvnUojPCft k&feature=endscreen – Short you tube video on introduction to this topic • http://www.cebm.net/index.aspx?o=1157 – Appraisal tools, statistics calulators and more • http://www.cebm.net/index.aspx?o=2320 – Richard Lehman’s Journal Review – Light hearted GP perspective Arroll et al (BMJ 2014) • What study type? • Who were the participants? Arroll et al (BMJ 2014) • What study type? • Who were the participants? • What were the results? Arroll et al • “Using the hazard ratio, on any one day the gossipy magazines disappeared 14.51 times (95% confidence interval 6.69 to 33.32) faster than the non-gossipy ones” • What does this mean? Hazard Ratio • The hazard ratio equals a weighted relative risk over the entire duration of a study and is derived from a time-to-event curve or Kaplan-Meier curve. This curve describes the status of both patient groups at different time points after a defined starting point Hazard Ratio • Hazard Ratio is a ratio of hazards HR = Hazard in treatment arm/hazard in control arm • A Hazard = instantaneous event rate = probability that an individual at time t has an event at that time • HR = 1 (event rates are the same in both arms) • HR = 2 (at any time twice as many patients in treatment group are having an event proportionally to the comparator group • HR = 0.5 (half as many….) • A hazard ratio of 14 means the a magazine in the gossipy group, is has 14x the odds of reaching the outcome (disappearance) compared to a non-gossipy one Illustrated For interest.. “Gossipy magazines had a significantly higher mortality rate than the non-gossipy ones (P<0.001), whereas age had no significant impact on survival (P=0.41).” “Magazines that disappeared were also significantly cheaper than those that remained.” Hazard Ratio of 2 A hazard ratio of 2 does means that treatment will cause the patient to heal faster, but in a very specific sense. In the context of hazard ratio, “fast” means that a treated patient who has not yet healed by a certain time has twice the chance of being healed at the next point in time compared to someone in the control group. The hazard ratio is equivalent to the odds that an individual in the group with the higher hazard reaches the endpoint first. In a clinical trial examining time to disease resolution, it represents the odds that a treated patient will resolve symptoms before a control patient A hazard ratio of 14 means the a magazine in the gossipy group, is has 14x the odds of reaching the outcome (disappearance) compared to a non-gossipy one