Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
The Auk 111(4):945-952, 1994 EFFECTS OF OLFACTORY ARTIFICIAL-NEST CUES ON EXPERIMENTS CHRISTOPHERJ. WHELAN, MICHAEL L. DILGER,DAVE RONSON, NATHALIE HALLYN, AND STEVEN DILGER The Morton Arboretum, Lisle, Illinois 60532, USA ABSTRACT.--We examined the effectsof two potential olfactory cueson the outcome of experimentsusing artificial neststo assaypredationon open-cupnestingsongbirds.In two experimentslasting 15 dayseachand replicatedtwo timesat three sites,bamboonestsbaited with Japanese Quail (Corturnixjaponica) eggswere placedon the groundin a 5 x 4 grid array with 50 m between adjacentnests.Egg survivorshipwas monitored every five days, and remote cameraswere used to take photographsof animalsremoving bait eggsfrom nests. One experimentconsistedof four treatmentsthat varied the type and amount of olfactory informationprovided by the investigator:(1) human scent;(2) "no scent";(3) "deer scent"; and (4) perfume. Artificial scentswere applied to shoes,clothing, and skin during the experiment set-upand monitoring.For the first experiment,ratesof nestlosswere greaterfor the human-scent treatment than deer-scent and/or no-scent treatments in one-half of the spatial/temporalreplicates.Rain appearsto have complicatedthe effectsof the scenttreatments in some of the spatial/temporal replicates.Final levels of nest loss, however, were greater for perfume and human-scenttreatmentsthan deer-scentand no-scenttreatments regardlessof spatial/temporalreplicate.The secondexperimentconsistedof two treatments, one in which eggswere replacedperiodically,and the otherin which eggswere not replaced for the duration of the experiment.Both treatmentshad similar ratesand final levels of nest loss.Approximately90%of 51 photographs were of olfactory-searching, predominantlynocturnal mammals.We concludethat olfactory-searching predatorscan cue on human odors left in the areaof artificialnests,but that either rain or scents(e.g. the deer scent)canbe usedto decreasethis potentialbias.In contrast,changingegg qualitiesover the time scale of our experiments do notprovideadditionalcuesto predators. Received 20April1992,accepted 25 November 1992. ALTHOUGH NESTPREDATION haslong been recognized as a major sourceof reproductive failure in bird species(e.g. Ricklefs 1969), only recently hasnestpredation been hypothesizedto be an important determinant of bird community structure (Martin 1988a, b). Predation on eggsand nestlingsmay regulate bird populations (George 1987),influence habitat selection (Blancherand Robertson1985), and affect patternsof nestdispersionand speciescoexistence (Martin 1988a,b). Perhapsmore importantly, nest predation also may be important for the conservationof many bird species(Ambuel and Temple 1983,Wilcove 1985).In the increasingly fragmentedlandscapesof North America, nest predation may be an important contribution to population declinesof many open-nestingbird species,particularly long-distanceNeotropical migrants(e.g. Terborgh 1989). One major methodologicalproblem in studies of nest predation is finding enough nestsof a given bird speciesto have meaningful sample sizesand to control possiblyconfoundingvari- ables (e.g. see Reitsmaet al. 1990). Largely becauseof these methodologicaldifficulties, investigatorshave been using artificial nestsbaited with quail eggsand have measuredegg survivorship over someperiod of time simulating a normal incubationperiod (e.g.Anderssonand Wiklund 1978, Loiselle and Hoppes 1983, Wilcove 1985, Martin 1987, Yahner 1989, Reitsma et al. 1990).Despite the growing useof artificial nests and the admission that some results from using thesenestsdo not mimic resultsfrom real nests(e.g.Reitsmaet al. 1990,S.A. Temple pers. comm.), few studies have examined in detail methodologicalissuesinvolving the use of artificial nests. Martin (1987), for instance, reported that artificial wicker-basket nests had lower rates of nest predation than artificially baited real nests when both nest types were placedoff the ground,but that artificial and real nestshad similarratesof predationwhen placed on the ground.He concludedthat the off-ground nestsare found primarily by visual-searching predatorsandthatthesepredatorspossiblyavoid 945 946 WHELAN ETAL. artificial nests(T. E. Martin pets. comm.);also, he judgedthat groundnestsare found primarily by olfactory-searchingpredators who do not respond to nest appearance. Other investigatorshave questioned the extent to which olfactory-searchingpredatorsfind artificial nests,thus questioning the biological relevance of the results of experiments using artificial nests(Willebrand and Marcstr6m 1988). If artificial nestsare seldomfound by olfactorysearchingpredators,their reflection of predation pressureson real nestsmay be minimal. When a large olfactory-searching predator(e.g. a raccoon,Procyonlotor) finds a songbird nest, there is little possibilityfor the parentsto drive [Auk, Vol. 111 the Elburn ForestPreserveis surroundedby agricultural fields (mostly corn or soybeans).Olfactorysearchingpredatorsat these sitesinclude the southern flying squirrel (Glaucomys volans),Virginia opossum (Didelphismarsupialis), raccoon,coyote (Canislatrans),red fox (Vulpesvulpes),and stripedskunk (Mephitismephitis), as well asdomesticdogs(Canisfamiliaris) and cats (Feliscattus). Effectof humanscenttrails (experiment/).--Human scentassociated with artificial nestsand eggscould serveasan olfactorycuethat couldlead to extremely biasedestimatesof nestpredation(Reitsmaet al. 1990). This experiment consisted of four treatments de- signedto differ in the extentof olfactoryinformation supplied to potential predators. Each treatment con- sistedof 20 bamboo,wicker nestsarrayedon a 5 x 4 grid system,with 50 m betweenadjacentnests(den- offthepredator(seePettingill 1976).Evensmall sity 6.67 nests/ha). Eachnestwas lined with leaf litter songbirds,however, can sometimesdrive away small, visual-searchingpredatorslike Blue Jays (Cyanocittacristata) and eastern chipmunks (Tamiasstriatus;C. J. Whelan pers. obs., R. T. Holmes pets. comm.). Artificial nestsprovide an attractive, nondestructive test system for examining nest pre- and placed in a hole in the ground with the top of the nest flush with the soil surface. Nests were baited with two JapaneseQuail (Corturnixjaponica)eggs,and were examinedfor eggsurvivorshipat days5, 10,and 15 following placementin the field, conformingto a monitoring scheduleused by Reitsma et al. (1990). The experiment was conducted twice, once in late May and once in early July. Scenttreatmentswere dation. While use of artificial nests remains conswitched among grid locations at each site for each troversial, they do allow experimental control time replicate. A single replicate of each treatment over numerous confounding factors like nest was conductedsimultaneouslyat each experimental shape,height,density,positionwithin a plant, site, for a total of three replicatesof each treatment and number of eggs.Therefore,the usefulness both times the experiment was conducted. of artificial nests in understanding the imporTreatments consistedof: (1) human scent; (2) "deer tance of nest predation needs to be evaluated. scent" (Tink's # 69 Doe-in-Rut Lure); (3) "no scent" Here we report the resultsof experimentsde- (ScentShield and Scent Walker• Boot Pads);and (4) signedto test specificallythe potentialfor olfactory-searchingnest predatorsto find artifi- perfume (Primo). The human-scenttreatment served (human-scenttrails and egg age/odor). decrease as a basisfor comparisonwith previousstudiesand was establishedby putting nestsout on the grid in a cial nestswhen thesenestsare carefully hidden on the ground under vegetation.We used two standardfashion, walking from grid intersectionto grid intersection,digging a slight depressionin the complementaryexperimentaltestsystemsthat: ground, placing the nest in the hole, lining the nest (1) clearly indicatedwhether or not olfactory- with leaves,and baiting the nestwith two quail eggs. searchingpredatorsfind artificial nests;and (2) Following Reitstoaet al. (1990), we rubbed our hands examinedtwo possiblemethodologicalbiases with leaf litter prior to handling nests or eggs to STUDY SITES AND METHODS Experimentstook place from May through August 1991at three separatesitesabout 25 to 65 km west of Chicago, Illinois: (1) the Morton Arboretum (Lisle, Illinois); (2) Hawthorn Hill (WoodridgePark District, Woodridge, Illinois); and (3) the Elburn ForestPreserve (Elburn, Illinois). All three sites consistedof the amount of human scent left on or near the nest.Deer scentsupposedlyconsistsof white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus) doe hormones,bodily secretions, and urine (but see Trost 1989); it can be simulatedusingammoniaand water (Trost1989).Scent Shield and Boot Pads (used in the no-scent treatment) are designed to eliminate or to mask human scent. For each nonhuman-scent treatment, shoes, clothes, and skin were treatedwith the animalscent,masking scent,or perfume. Otherwise, nestswere treated simisolated tracts of eastern deciduous forest dominated ilarly to the human-scent treatment. Becauseof seby variousoaks(Quercus spp.),sugarmaple(Acersac- verely degraded habitat, part of the Hawthorn Hill charurn),white ash (Fraxinusamericana),and a variety site was unusable. Therefore, at Hawthorn Hill we of shrubsand understorytree species(e.g. ironwood, used only three scent treatments:human scent, no Ostryavirginiana). The Morton Arboretumand Haw- scent,and perfume. Two types of complementarydata were collected thorn Hill are surrounded by suburban homes,but October 1994] Olfactory Cues andArtificial Nests and analyzed:ratesof egg survivorship(i.e. speedat which eggsdisappeared)over the 15-daysimulated 947 electricallyactivateda solenoidmountedon the camera. When a bait egg was disturbed,the shutterwas "incubation period," and the final level of overall released.Cameraswere placedalongtransects at the predation(i.e. numberof eggsremainingat lastcen- Arboretum and at Elburn, but not at Hawthorn Hill. sus). Rates of predation at each site for each time replicatewere analyzed with survival analysis(Be- The cameraswere not placed on any of the experimentalgrids.Instead,transects were establishedaway nedetti et al. 1990). Final levels of predation (number fromthe experimental gridsusingoneof threescent of nestssurvivingatendof 15-dayincubationperiod) were analyzedwith a log-linearmodel (Brown 1990) treatments(human scent,no scent,or perfume), and five cameraswere placed along each of these tran- that examinedsimultaneouslythe effectsof time rep- sects.Cameraswere moved5 m or morefollowing a licate, site, and scent treatment on the number of nests depredation event. surviving(for an exampleof thesestatisticalanalyses, see Whelan et al. 1991). The expectationfor this experimentwas that if viRESULTS sual-searching predatorsare the major predatorson artificialnests,then ratesand final levelsof predation Experiment1.--In the first time replicate there shouldnot differ amongtreatments.We assumedthat were significantdifferencesin ratesof nest loss olfactory-searching predatorslike raccoons are more amongscenttreatmentsonly at the Elburn Forlikely to associatethe presenceof food with humans est Preserve. At this site, rates of nest loss inthan with deer. Consequently,if thesepredatorsare more important, we assumedthat the human-scent andperfumetreatments shouldhavegreateramounts of predation than the deer-scentand no-scenttreatments. Effectof egg age and odor(experiment2).--Unrefrigerated eggs that are not incubated can develop a rather strongodor with increasingage.Therefore,it is possiblethat the longer baited eggs survive, the greaterwill be their susceptibilityto olfactory-searching predators(a concoctionincluding rotten chicken eggsis a frequent bait usedby mammaltrappers;Joel S. Brown pers.comm.).Egg susceptibilityshould not changewith egg age for visual-searchingpredators. Experiment2 testedwhether egg susceptibilityin- creasedfrom deer scent,to perfume, to no scent, and to human scent (Gehan-Wilcoxon test, X 2 = 8.79, df = 3, P = 0.032;Fig. 1). In the second time replicate, ratesof nest lossdiffered significantly amongthe scenttreatmentsat all three sites. Differences the Arboretum in rates of nest loss at both and Hawthorn Hill were close to our expectationif olfactory-searching predatorswere primarily responsiblefor nest losses. At the Arboretum, rates of nest loss increased from deer scent, to no scent, to human scent, and to perfume(Gehan-WilcoxonX 2= 12.52,df = 3, P < 0.006;Fig. 2). At Hawthorn Hill, rates creases asa functionof eggage.The experimentcon- increased from no scent, to human scent, and sisted of two treatments, each of which had the same to perfume (Gehan-WilcoxonX 2 = 7.415, df = 2, P < 0.03; Fig. 2). At the Elburn Forest Preserve, rates of nest loss differed among scent treatments,but not in the way predicted if olfactory-searchingmammalsare the important predatorson these nests.Rates of nest loss at basic design as experiment 1 and was replicated at each experimental site. In the no-replacementtreatment, nests were baited on the first day of the experiment, and surviving eggswere not replacedfor the entire 15-day incubation period. In the replacement treatment, surviving eggswere replaced with fresheggson eachcensusday.Experiment2 wasconductedtwice in a staggeredfashionwith experiment 1. Statisticalanalyseswere the sameasfor experiment 1. In experiment2, if visual-searchingpredatorsare the most important predatorson artificial nests,we expectedno differencein ratesand levels of predation between the two treatments.If olfactory-searching predators are the most important predators on artificial nests,we expectedgreater rates and levels of predationon the no-replacementtreatmentneststhan the replacementtreatment nests. Remote-camera system.--In addition to the aboveexperiments, remote cameras modified from Picman (1987; M. L. Dilger et al. unpubl. manuscript)were usedto obtainpicturesof predatorstaking eggsfrom the artificialnests.Eachcamerajnestsetupconsisted of a microswitch attached to an artificial nest that Elburn increased from human scent, to deer scent and perfume, and to no scent (GehanWilcoxon X 2 = 8.789, df = 3, P < 0.04; Fig. 2). We found that on average49.5%of nests(range 31-75%) in all treatments were chewed, were moved, or disappeared. The log-linear model that best fits the multidimensionalcontingencytable of time replicate, experimental site, scent treatment, and number of nestssurviving included two interactions of main effects (G = 32.41, df = 26, P = 0.180). The interaction of scent treatment and number nestssurviving (G = 12.12,df = 3, P = 0.007) indicates that final survivorship decreased from deer scent, to no scent, to human scentand perfume (Fig. 3). The three-way in- 948 WH•LANEl'AL. lOO [Auk, Vol. 111 teractionof time replicate,experimentalsite and number nestssurviving (G = 43.72, df = 2, P < Arboretum, Rep 1 0.001) indicates that the levels of overall mor- 8o Deer Scent No Scent HumanScent 8o 4o 2o i i • 5 lO 15 lOO 8O 8O 4O 2O Elburn, Rep I tality differed amongthe sitesdepending upon time replicate (Fig. 4). For instance,at the Arboretum, survivorship decreasedmoderately in the secondtime replicate.At Elburn, survivorship decreaseddramaticallyin the secondtime replicateand, at Hawthorn Hill, there was virtually no difference in survivorship between time replicates. Experiment2.--There were no differences in rates of nest lossbetween the no-egg-replacement and egg-replacement treatments at any experimental site in either time replicate. The log-linear analysis of the multidimensional contingency table of experimental site, time replicate, replacement treatment, and number of nestssurviving showed no significantmain effects,nor any significantinteractionsof main effects.Indeed, virtually no nests survived at each site for each time replicate. On average 46.24%of nests(range43-80%) in all treatments were chewed, were moved, or disappeared. Remote-camera photographs.--Fifty-onephotographswere taken showing eight different animal species.Most photographs (36) were of raccoons,regardlessof scenttransect.In several of thesephotographs,up to three different raccoons could be seen in the frame. The next most 0 I I 5 10 15 IO0 8O 8O 4O 2O i i ß 5 10 15 Day abundantanimal photographed(7) was the Virginia opossum.Photographsof two other mammals,the easternchipmunk and the white-tailed deer, were taken only on the perfume transect. Although the eastern chipmunk is primarily a visual-searchingpredator,it is alsopossiblethat it sometimeslocatesfood using olfaction (D. Jedlicka pers. comm.). Whether the deer was actuallypreying uponthe egg,or simplytripped the cameraaccidentally,is not evident from the photograph. The three photographsof a nonolfactorypredator,the Blue Jay,were associated with the no-scenttransects.Three of the photographs are of animals that probably tripped the cameras accidentally, an American Robin (Turdusmigratorius),a Black-cappedchickadee (Parusatricapillus),and an unidentified bird. Fig. 1. Percent nestssurviving over 15-day simulated incubationperiod for nestsin four scenttreatments for first-time replicate of experiment (n = 20 nests/treatmen0. October1994] Olfactory CuesandArtificial Nests lOO o• Arboretum, Rep 2 6o 3O ;> 8o • --m--o-+ 949 Deer Scent No Soent Human Scent Perfume • 20 c lO a. o I 4o 1 Deer r • No Scent I- Human 1 [--] Perfume Fig. 3. For different treatments,percentnestssurviving at completionof eachtime replicatefor both time replicatescombined(n = 60 nests/treatment,except for deer-scenttreatment in which n = 40 nests). 2o o ,5 lO 15 DISCUSSION lOO Our resultsindicatethat olfactory-searching Elburn, Rep 2 mammalswere the primary predatorson arti- ficial nestsbaitedwith quail eggs.In experiment1 for one-halfof the site/timereplicates, m 60 •' 40 • the ratesof predationwere greatestfor nests associated with human or perfume scent,and least for nests associated with no scent or deer scent. Nests in the deer-scent treatment had al- mosttwicethe final survivorshipasthosein the humanand perfumetreatments.Finally,about 20 90%of the photographs of animalsattacking artificial nestswere of olfactory-searching mammals.Of these,the raccoonis clearlythe 0 5 10 15 lOO most important. In contrast,therewere no differencesamong treatmentsin experiment2. Accordingto original expectations,this would have been inter- pretedasevidenceagainstolfactory-searching predatorsplaying a majorrole in depredation of artificialnests.However,giventhe resultsof the remote cameras,coupledwith the large 8o number of neststhat were chewed, were moved, 6o or disappeared in bothexperiments 1 and2, our originalexpectationfor this experimentseems mistaken. If changingodorhasnoeffecton egg susceptibility, or if egg odor doesnot change duringthe 15-daysimulated incubation period, then there should have been no great differences between the two experimentaltreat- 4o 2o 0 • [ ß 5 10 15 Day Fig. 2. Percentnestssurvivingover 1S-daysimulatedincubationperiodfor nestsin four scenttreat- mentsfor second-time replicateof experiment.Symbol for perfumescentis hidden by symbolfor no scentin that their survival curveswere identical (n = 20 nests/treatment). 950 W•EL*NL•r^L. 60 I 4O [Auk,Vol. 111 Thus, it is plausiblethat the confusingresults of the first time replicateand the Elburn study site in the secondtime replicatecan be attributed to confounding effectsof rain. If our resultsare interpreted to indicatethat olfactory-searching predatorsare morelikely to find artificial neststhan visual-searchingpredators, it may be argued that these nests adequately assayfor effectsof olfactory-searching -1 predators.However, our findings also suggest that olfactory-searchingpredatorscan use olfactoryinformation suppliedby the investiga1 2 1 2 1 2 tor and, thus, results from these experiments Arboretum Elburn Hawthorn Hill mayactuallyoverestimatethe importanceof olpredators.There may be two Fig. 4. Percentnestssurviving accordingto both factory-searching experimentalsite and time replicate(n = 80 nests. ways to prevent suchoverestimation.First, be[time replicate]-•.[site]-z exceptHawthorn Hill, for which n = 60 nests.[time replicate]-•.[site]-•). cause rain obscures or confounds whatever ol- factoryinformationis left in the neighborhood of the artificial nest by the investigator,one possibleway to decreasethe ability of olfactoryments, regardlessof whether predation is by searchingpredatorsto cue in to that informaolfactory-searchingor visual-searchingpreda- tion is to set up the experimentduring a raintots. storm. Second,in dry-weather conditions,anOne striking result is the amount of vari- imal or shielding scents(asused in our experability in the rate of nest depredationamong iments)couldeffectivelymaskthe odor left by the sites and between time replicates for ex- the investigator. periment 1. A likely explanation for this variPerhapsthe most important implication of ability seemsto be related to weather condi- our findings is that uniform methodology is tions, especiallythose conditionsat the times essential for results of different studies to be the experimentswere setup. At the time when comparable(e.g.Knight and Temple 1986).Difthe first time replicatewassetup, northeastern ferent methodologies(e.g. frequency of nest Illinois (including each of the three sites) ex- monitoring) are likely to introduce different perienceda large, regional rain storm.The veg- suitesof biases,making comparisonsproblemetation and ground at eachsite were extremely atic. In the future, if someinvestigatorsfollow wet both when the nestswere placed on the our lead in using animal or shielding scentsto grid and, again, when they were baited with reducethe effectsof investigatorodor,they will eggs.It seemspossiblethat the rain decreased need to use cautionwhen comparingtheir rethe effectivenessof the scentsapplied to shoes, suits with studies that used no scents. clothing, and skin. Justhow olfactory-searching predatorsarereThis interpretationis supportedby the results spondingto the scenttreatmentsof experiment of the secondtime replicate.Two of the sites 1 is not clear,but two alternatives(not mutually (the Arboretum and Hawthorn Hill) remained exclusive) seem likely possibilities.First, the dry during the courseof the experiment. The predatormay detecta trail of scentleft by the Elburn ForestPreserve,however, experienced investigator, learn to associatethis scent with localizedbut extremelysevererainstorms,both an egg reward and, thus, "trapline" from nest when the nestswere placed on the grids and to nest. Second, the scent treatments we used when they were baited with eggs.At the two may simply increasethe "radius of detection" siteswith dry-weather conditions,ratesof nest of a nest (i.e. the extent of the area around a loss were least for the deer-scent and no-scent nest associatedwith a particular scentcue). If treatments, and greatest for the human-scent predatorslike raccoonsare attractedto unusual and perfumetreatments.In contrast,at Elburn, scents(neophilia rather than neophobia),the which was deluged by rain, there were signif- greater the radius of detection, the higher the icant differencesamong the scent treatments, likelihood of nest depredation.Casualinspecbut not in a way that is easily interpretable. tionsof the temporaland spatialpatternsof nest October 1994] Olfactory Cues andArtificial Nests lossesin theseexperimentssuggestthat, in our systems,the secondalternative is the more like- ly. Determiningwhich, if either,of thesealternativesis correctmaybe an importantnext step 951 ing scents,and RonaldDilger for donatingcameras fordocumenting nestpredators. Quaileggsweresupplied by Henry MarksandVivian Pattenof the SoutheastPoultryResearch Laboratoryof the Agricultural Research Service,SouthAtlanticArea, U.S. Department of Agriculture.We alsothank the Woodridge in understandingmethodologicalintricaciesof the experimentaltechnique. Park District and the Forest Preserve District of Kane Our experimentsindicatethat it maybe pos- Countyfor permissionto work on their grounds.This sibleto vary experimentallythe typesof olfac- work wassupportedby a FrankM. ChapmanGrant tory cuesassociated with nestsand, thus,begin from the AmericanMuseumof Natural History to to determine which cuesvarious predators are C.J.W.,a Steenbock Grantfromthe WisconsinSociety at least potentially able to use when finding for Ornithologyto M.L.D., and the Emily Rodgers nests.Experimentalmanipulationof visualcues, Davis Endowment to the Morton Arboretum. as well as various combinations of visual/olfac- tory cues,alsomay be possible.Suchmanipulationscould reveal much about the sensory LITERATURE CITED stimuli importantto the success of nestpredators,andthisinformationcouldin turn suggest AMBUEL,B.,ANDS. A. TEMPLE.1983. Area-dependent changesin the bird communitiesand vegetation habitatmanipulationsthat coulddecreasetheir of southernWisconsinforests.Ecology64:1057impacton cup-nestingbird species. 1068. Investigators studyingaviannestingsuccess ANDERSSON, M., AND C. G. WIKLUND. 1978. Clumphavelong been concernedthat their presence ing versusspacingout: Experimentson nest preand monitoringof nestscould have negative dation in Fieldfares(Turduspilaris).Anim. Behav. 26:1207-1212. consequences for individualbirdsunderstudy (..e.g. Bart1977,Lenington1979,G6tmarkand Ahlund 1984). Predators could be attracted to BART,J. 1977. Impact of human visitationson avian nesting success.Living Bird 16:187-192. agitatedparents,follow investigatorsvisually, BENEDETTI,J., K. YUEN, AND L. YOUNG. 1990. Life cue in to markers left near nests, or follow scent trailsto the nest.Our resultssupportthe suggestionthatolfactorypredators potentiallycould use human scent to locate nests. Various ideas tables and survival functions. Pages739-768 in BMDP statisticalsoftware manual, vol. 2. (W. J. Dixon, Ed.). Univ. California Press,Berkeley. BLANCHER, P. J., AND R. J. ROBERTSON.1985. Site con- sistencyin kingbird breeding performance:Im- have been offeredto counteractthe potential plicationsfor sitefidelity. J.Anim. Ecol.54:1017biascausedby the presenceof humans,includ1027. ing repellents (e.g. mustardoil and kerosene BROWN,M. B. 1990. Part 3. Analysis of multiway [Hammond and Forward 1956]; naphthalene tables:Log-linearmodels.Pages277-310in BMDP statisticalsoftware,vol. 2. (W. J. Dixon, Ed.). Univ. mothballs[Hamerstrom1970,Gawlik et al. 1988] and mechanical devices [Post and Greenlaw 1989]).As suggestedabovefor artificial nosts, the useof animalor shieldingscentsto mask humanodor couldbe an importanttool in stud- iesof actualnestingsuccess. Our findingsin- California Press,Berkeley. GAWLIK, D. E., M. E. HOSTETLER,AND K. L. BILDSTEIN. 1988. Naphthalenemoth ballsdo not deter mammalian predatorsat Red-wingedBlackbirdnest. J. Field. Ornithol. 59:189-191. GEORGE,T. L. 1987. Greater land bird densities on dicate that any steps taken to decreasethe island vs. mainland:Relation to nest predation amountof humanscentin theproximityof nests level. Ecology68:1393-1400. shouldhelp to decrease the potentialnegative CdSTM.a•RK, F., ANDM. J(HLUND.1984. Do field obinfluenceof observerpresence. serversattract nest predatorsand influence nesting success of CommonEiders?J.Wildl. Manage. 48:381-387. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS HAMERSTROM, F. 1970. Think with a goodnosenear a nest. Raptor Res. News 4:79-80. We thank Joel Brown, Marcy DeMauro, Richard HAMMOND, M. C., AND W. F. FORWARD. 1956. ExHolmes, BobMorgan, Len Reitsma,ScottRobinson, periments on causesof duck nest predation. J. and StanleyTemple for helpful consultationon exWildl. Manage. 20:243-247. perimental design and/or interpretation.We also KNIGHT,R. L., ANDS. A. TEMPLE.1986. Methodologthank three reviewerswhosecomments greatlyimical problems in studiesof avian nest defence. provedthe manuscript. We areespeciallygratefulto JoeRothfor suggesting the useof animalandshield- Anim. Behav. 34:561-566. LENINGTON, S. 1979. Predators and blackbirds: The 952 wrm•u "uncertaintyprinciple" in field biology.Auk 96: 190-192. LOISELLE, B. A., AND W. G. HOPEES.1983. Nest predation in insular and mainland est in Panama. Condor lowland rain for- 85:93-95. MARTIN,T. E. 1987. Artificial nest experiments:Effects of nest appearanceand type of predator. Condor 89:925-928. MARTIN, T. E. 1988a. Processesorganizing opennesting bird assemblages:Competition or nest predation?Evol. Ecol.2:37-50. MARTIN,T. E. 1988b. On the advantageof being different:Nest predationand the coexistence of bird species.Proc.Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 85:21962199. [Auk, Vol. 111 Effectsof removal of red squirrels, Tamiasciurus hudsonicus, and easternchipmunks, Tamiasstria- tus,on nest predationin a northernhardwoods forest: An artificial nest experiment. Oikos 57: 375-380. RICKL•S,R.E. 1969. Ananalysisofnestingmortality in birds. Smithson. Contr. Zool. 9:1-48. TERBORGH, J. 1989. Where have all the birds gone? Princeton Univ. Press,Princeton, New Jersey. TROST, F. 1989. Sexscentshowdown. Outdoor Digest September/October:16-17. Wh•L•N, C. J., M. F. WILLSON, C. A. TUMA,ANDI. SOUZA-PINTO.1991. Spatial and temporal patternsof postdispersal seedpredation.Can.J.Bot. 69:428-436. PETTINGILL, O. S., JR. 1976. Observedactsof predation on birds in northern lower Michigan. Living Bird ET AI,. 15:33-41. PICM•N, J. 1987. An inexpensivecamerasetup for the study of predation at artificial nests.J. Field Ornithol. POST,W., •D 58:372-382. J. S. GREENLAW.1989. Metal barriers WILCOVE, D. S. 1985. Nest predationin foresttracts and the declineof migratorysongbirds.Ecology 66:1211-1214. WILLEBRAND, T., AND V. MARCSTR•M.1988. On the dangerofusingdummyneststostudypredation. Auk 105:378-379. YAHNER,R. H. 1989. Effectsof nest-siteselectionon protectnear-groundnestsfrom predators.J.Field depredationof artificialnests.J. Wildl. Manage. Ornithol. 53:21-25. 60:102-103. RErrSMA,L. R., R. T. HOLMES,•ND T. W. SHERRY. 1990.