Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Kristina Reiss and Aiso Heinze, University of Munich (Germany)1 Research Paradigms on Teaching and Learning Proof Modeling proof competency Summary In the following, we describe the crucial aspects of our research program on proof and argumentation. This research focuses on geometrical proof competencies of students at the lower secondary level (grades 7–9). We have already formulated a preliminary model of geometrical proof and intend to develop a more detailed competency model on proof that describes • the structure of the individual’s competency when creating proofs, and • the development of proof competency including continuous learning processes as well as sudden insights. The competency model will be based on theoretical assumptions and has to be confirmed by empirical data. The model can be applied for a more profound development of proof items in assessments at the system level (i.e., classrooms or larger units). Moreover, it should help teachers and researchers to develop curricula and teaching material and to evaluate learning environments for the mathematics classroom. Our research is clearly embedded in mathematics education and performed in the mathematics classroom with a specific emphasize on proof and argumentation. However, it is strongly interdisciplinary in its nature. We particularly take into account cognitive as well as motivational aspects of educational psychology and cooperate with researchers from this field. 1. Proof competency In the center of our research paradigm is a specific conception of proof competency. According to Weinert (2001), competencies are defined as cognitive abilities and skills, which individuals have or which can be learned by them. These abilities and skills enable them to solve particular problems and encompass the motivational, volitional, and social readiness and capacity to utilize the solutions successfully and responsibly in variable situations. However, competencies cannot be seen independent of domains, in particular, in the case of competency acquisition. In this sense, a description of proof competency is fundamentally based on the domain of mathematical proof and proving. More precisely, proof competency encompasses cognitive and non-cognitive personal dispositions as prerequisites for successfully dealing with problem situations related to mathematical proof. Based on these assumptions the concept of proof certainly is important for the definition of proof competency. Our research interest is not to describe the students’ understanding of mathematical proof explicitly but to describe their competency to solve proof problems. Therefore we take a normative perspective on mathematical proof. This view is supported by 1 Department of Mathematics, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Theresienstr. 39, 80333 München, Germany, [email protected] . the curriculum for secondary mathematics classroom and also provides criteria for the acceptance of students’ proof as mathematically correct. 2. Structure of proof competency One important application of the concept of “proof competency” probably is to assess whether an individual is more competent in solving proof problems than another individual. For this purpose we have to find out what the structure of proof competency is. Therefore it is important to analyze proof competency with respect to the factors by which it is influenced (in the sense of subsidiary competencies). Since proof competency is characterized by the competency to solve proof problems, it goes without saying to start with a theoretical cognitively oriented investigation of proving processes. This means, that in addition to the normative perspective on proof there is a second analytical perspective on the process of generating a proof. An analysis of the proving process is necessary for identifying essential competencies that are part of the proof competency. A starting point might be an expert model of the proving process as suggested by Boero (1999). He distinguished six phases: (1) exploration of the problem situation, (2) formulation of a hypothesis, (3) exploration of the hypothesis (including the identification of possible arguments), (4) constructon of a logical chain of arguments, (5) extension of this chain of arguments to a proof (according to given standards), and optionally (6) approaching a formal proof. Based on such a process model one may identify knowledge, abilities and skills related to mathematics, which are necessarily part of proof competency. However, the expert’s steps while performing a proof do not give detailed information on the process. Accordingly, aspects of the process should be taken into account. According to the problem-solving competencies formulated by Schoenfeld (1992) it is possible to describe cognitive factors like knowledge on facts, strategic knowledge, meta-cognition and non-cognitive factors like beliefs and motivation as important subelements of proof competency. These factors have to be analyzed in detail (e.g. Weber, 2001, for aspects of the strategic knowledge). Moreover, there is a competency on a meta-level concerning the knowledge on characteristics of deductive reasoning in mathematical proofs and the competency to perform multiple-step deductive reasoning. The latter includes the ability to understand that statements can have different status (as premise and as conclusion) and to use this fact for creating deductive chains of arguments (cf. Duval, 1991). We will call this the “dynamic status” of statements. Regarding the classification of different epistemologies of mathematical proof by Balacheff (2002) this view is connected to the one of Healy and Hoyles (1998). Balacheff (2002) describes this view as “related to the technical understanding of what it [proof] is in the mathematical activity”. On a statistical level one can find evidence that factors identified theoretically are related to proof competency. A quantitative analysis of empirical data like linear regression models can provide insight about the significance of different factors (see the example below). It is important to notice that research in this paradigm aims at explaining important factors of individual proof competency. However, it is not possible and we do not have the ambition to completely explain individual proof competency. Human beings are too different that their behavior and their actions might be fully explained by a small set of factors. However, quantitative methods as used in our research (see examples below) might be appropriate for providing evidence at the system level of a school or a classroom. Example 1: A study on geometrical proof competency was conducted with 576 grade 9 students (350 from Germany and 226 from Taiwan). Data was collected by different tests for (1) proof competency in geometry, (2) competency on calculating angles sizes and side lengths in geometric figures, (3) knowledge on geometrical concepts, and (4) problem-solving competency regarding unfamiliar problems related to mathematics. A linear regression analysis showed that there was a significant influence of knowledge on geometric concepts, competency in performing geometric calculations and problem solving competency on the proof competency in geometry. This result differed between German and Taiwanese students. For German students, the three influential factors explained 32.5% of variance of the proof competence, for the Taiwanese sample even 57.7% (recent results of a joint project with Ying-Hao Cheng, Fou-Lai Lin, and Stefan Ufer). Example 2: In a three-year longitudinal study on geometrical proof with 194 German students from grade 7 to 9 data were collected each year for (1) proof competency in geometry, (2) competency on calculating angles sizes and side lengths in geometric figures and, only in grade 9, knowledge of geometrical concepts. A linear regression analysis to explain proof competency in grade 9 showed that only proof competency in grades 7 and 8 and the knowledge on concepts in grade 9 had significant influence (46% explained variance). Particularly, the competency of calculate angles and sides in geometrical figures in grade 7, 8 and 9 had no significant influence. 3. Modeling proof competency Factors which influence proof competency can be identified by a theoretical analysis of proving activities and evaluated by empirical findings. They have to be integrated into a competency model. The naive approach of putting together all the important factors is not consistent with the idea of modeling a competency. In particular, the aim to use a competency model for the description of the development of proof competency implies that the preparation and validation of such a model is a complex challenge. Since proof competency is described by the competency of an individual to solve proof problems, the main point is to identify characterizations of the difficulty of proof problems. In the following an approach for a competency model on proof is given for geometric proof competency of students in grades 7–9. Based on the elaboration of the previous section we can assume that • creating a proof consisting of one deductive step is more difficult than solving a simple geometrical calculation problem, because the competency of deductive reasoning combined with explicit conceptual knowledge is necessary for the first task but not for the second. Solving simple geometrical calculation problems means to apply rules in concrete situations. The underlying principles do not have to be given explicitly, because for the solutions reasoning is not required. This means that one-step reasoning presupposes a different kind of competency. • creating a proof consisting of multiple deductive steps is more difficult than creating a proof consisting of one deductive step, because combining multiple steps requires strategic knowledge as well as the understanding and application of the dynamic status of statements. • distinguishing between two-step proofs, three-step proofs etc. does not seem to be essential. In fact, there is a general tendency of an increase in complexity and difficulty going along with the number of steps of a proof. However, it is assumed that there is no new level of quality in the sense of a non-continuous increase of complexity. This first analysis revealed three levels of competency: Level 1 applying rules in concrete situations Level 2 performing single step proofs Level 3 performing multiple step proofs Although the competency model was developed in a research program on geometric proof, the description of the competency levels are quite general and independent of the actual content. If we apply this model to geometry, for example in order to develop test items, it turns out that it is not sufficient to consider the domain-specific content only from a mathematical perspective. The mathematical perspective gives a first orientation about the structure of a proof, the concepts involved and the number of deductive steps. However, with respect to the competency model it remains to be shown that during the cognitive process the individual really performs all these steps separately. Particularly at this point it is evident, that this type of research is based on a research paradigm integrating aspects of mathematics education and of educational psychology. We assume that the domain-specific content plays two different roles. On the one hand, the difficulty of proof problems will increase with the complexity of the concepts involved. For example, a proof step using the vertical angles property is less difficult than a proof step based on the congruence rule SAS, because for the latter it is more complex to check whether the conditions of the rule are fulfilled. On the other hand, the quality of the individual conceptual knowledge plays a role. There may be an individual who retrieves a “schema-congruent rule SAS” including all conditions and consequences from his or her mind (in the sense of a chunk, cf. Anderson, 2004). And there may be an individual who needs a lot of cognitive capacity to reconstruct the congruence rule before being able to apply it. The former is probably more successful with multiple step proofs including the SAS rule than the latter. The quality of knowledge may be a key factor when classifying proof problems as one step or multiple step proofs. Mathematically, a proof may consist of two steps (competency level 3), but for an individual solving the proof problem the first proof step probably is “only” initial information in its mental representation of the problem, because he or she just retrieves this information from memory. In this case the proof problem requires only the competency of level 2 (performing a one step proof). This means that the same proof problem may be allocated in different competency levels for different individuals. In this sense, we hypothesize that the quality of individual knowledge has a moderating effect in the relation between proof problems and competency levels. Summarizing the previous aspects we may state: when applying the competency model we have to make assumptions about the geometrical knowledge of the sample. Generally, we will do this in a normative way based on the curriculum. Thus we can develop test items for proof tests for different grades reflecting the model of proof competency. Actually the same proof item may be allocated to competency level 3 for the test in grade 7 and to competency level 2 for the test in grade 9. Empirical results from different studies in Germany (N > 2000) have confirmed the competency model, particularly the organization of geometrical proof competency in levels (e.g. Reiss, Hellmich & Reiss, 2002). Data of German students from grade 7 through 9 have shown repeatedly that for each grade the corresponding test items could be modeled onedimensionally by the dichotomous Rasch model (Rasch, 1960). Recently this model was also confirmed in a cross-cultural study with German and Taiwanese students for grade 9 (cf. the study in example 1). The results are given in the following table: Percentages of correct solutions grade 9 Taiwan lower third average third upper third Germany lower third average third upper third Level 1 applying rules in concrete situations Level 2 performing single step proofs Level 3 performing multiple step proofs 54,4 25,0 6,2 69,3 63,6 39,7 76,0 83,2 84,3 60,2 15,7 2,9 83,2 39,6 8,1 88,9 70,0 33,7 4. Open questions and further research There are several possibilities to extend the present research. a) We need a deeper analysis of and more empirical evidence regarding the role of the quality of knowledge and its role as a moderator between the objective mathematical character of proof problems and the required individual competency when solving this problem. Here, a test concerning the quality of the individual knowledge should be developed. In particular, we have to determine which aspects of knowledge will play a role (only concepts or also knowledge on domain-specific strategies like the examples in Weber, 2001). b) It is an open question whether three levels of competency provide enough detailed information. It is possible that level 2 (one step proofs) can be differentiated in two further sublevels based on the complexity of the concepts involved (e.g., vertical angle argument versus congruence rule). Moreover, it is possible that there is an intermediate level between levels 1 and 2 consisting of geometrical calculation items that require complex problem-solving strategies (Hsu, 2007, used such items for a qualitative study). c) Presently, the competency model is restricted and evaluated for geometry. It should also be applied or adapted for algebra, calculus, and perhaps statistics/stochastics. First attempts may easily be conducted by re-analyses of existing data. d) Our experimental studies include questionnaires on interest and motivation. There is some evidence that interest and motivation play a significant role with respect to proving competency. Accordingly, these constructs should better be integrated into further research and dealt with in more detail. References Anderson, J. R. (2004). Cognitive psychology and its implications (6th ed.). New York: Palgrave Macmillan. Balacheff, N. (2002). The researcher epistemology: A deadlock for educational research on proof. In F. L. Lin (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2002 International Conference on Mathematics: Understanding proving and proving to understand (pp. 23–44). Taipei: NSC and NTNU. Boero, P. (1999). Argumentation and mathematical proof: A complex, productive, unavoidable relationship in mathematics and mathematics education. International Newsletter on the Teaching and Learning of Mathematical Proof 7/8. Duval R. (1991) Structure du raisonnement déductif et apprentissage de la démonstration. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 22(3), 233–263. Healy, L. & Hoyles, C. (1998). Justifying and proving in school mathematics. Technical report on the nationwide survey. Institute of Education, University of London. Hsu, H.-Y. (2007). Geometric calculations are more than just the application of procedural knowledge. In Woo, J. H., Lew, H. C., Park, K. S. & Seo, D. Y. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 57–64). Seoul: PME. Rasch, G. (1960). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Copenhagen: Nielsen & Lydiche. Reiss, K., Hellmich, F., & Reiss, M. (2002). Reasoning and proof in geometry: prerequisites of knowledge acquisition in secondary school students. In A.D. Cockburn & E. Nardi (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 4, pp. 113–120). Norwich: University of East Anglia. Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving, metacognition, and sense making in mathematics. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 334–370). New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. Weber, K. (2001). Student difficulty in constructing proofs: the need for strategic knowledge. Educational Studies in Mathematics 48, 101–119. Weinert, F. E. (2001). Concept of competence: a conceptual clarification. In D. S. Rychen & L. H. Salganik (Eds.), Defining and selecting key competencies (pp. 45–65). Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.