Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Memorie di paura condizionata: formazione ed estinzione L’utilizzo di protocolli di condizionamento in cui uno stimolo inizialmente neutro (stimolo condizionato, SC) viene ripetutamente associato ad uno stimolo incondizionato (SI) doloroso provoca la formazione di una risposta di paura allo SC molto robusta e duratura. Esseri umani ed altri animali, sottoposti ad un protocollo di condizionamento come quello descritto, apprendono due associazioni: Primo, apprendono l’associazione fra lo stimolo incondizionato e l’ambiente in cui è avvenuto l’apprendimento. Una volta che questa associazione si è formata, la visita all’ambiente di condizionamento determinerà il manifestarsi della risposta di paura, anche in assenza dello stimolo incondizionato e dello stimolo condizionato. Questa associazione fra stimolo incondizionato e ambiente dà origine al condizionamento alla paura al contesto (contextual fear conditioning). La seconda associazione appresa è fra stimolo condizionato (suono, stimolo luminoso) e stimolo incondizionato: lo stimolo condizionato predice lo stimolo incondizionato Quindi, anche quando il suono o lo stimolo luminoso vengono presentati in un ambiente nuovo, il soggetto mostrerà una chiara risposta di paura allo stimolo condizionato. L’associazione suono (luce)-stimolo incondizionato dà origine al condizionamento alla paura allo stimolo (cued fear conditioning). In both context and cued fear conditioning, retention of the learned associations is assessed, in animal models, by measuring fear responses, such as freezing, following conditioning. It is important to use a novel environment when measuring retention of the tone-shock association in order to dissociate cued conditioning from context conditioning. Importantly, this simple tone-US pairing is able to elicit conditioned fear in many types of environments, and is thus context independent. That is, the animal learns the association between tone and shock, not that the tone predicts shock only in a certain time or place. (LeDoux, 2011) This is a form of elemental learning. The presence of the fear response during retention tests reflects the extent to which context-US associations have been consolidated. Once formed, these memories can persist for days, weeks, or even years (Golet et al 1986). Cued fear conditioning in rodents 45 days later, only CS (tone in new context) Baseline freezing Contextual fear conditioning in rodents 45 days later, only training context Baseline freezing Tali protocolli, detti di “acquisizione di risposte di paura condizionata”, o di “condizionamento alla paura”, sono utilizzati come modello per la formazione di memorie emotive negative associate a stimoli o eventi traumatici e per la loro estinzione non solo nell’animale ma anche nell’uomo. Primo passo: quali strutture sono coinvolte nell’acquisizione e nel recupero a lungo termine di memorie di paura condizionata? Studi classici di lesione e di attivazione hanno indicato l’amigdala come struttura cruciale per la formazione ed il richiamo di memorie di paura condizionata “cued”usando come uscita la risposta emotiva di paura (freezing, aumento pressione arteriosa, conduttanza cutanea, …). L’ amigdala fa parte del sistema limbico ed è costituita da diverse suddivisioni (laterale, basale, intercalata, centrale) Regioni dell'amigdala messe in evidenza con differenti metodi colorazione. Nelle immagini in alto troviamo la colorazione di Nissl, sinistra, e la colorazione per l'acetilcolinesterasi utile per mettere in evidenza i differenti nuclei a destra. Nelle immagini sottostanti a sinistra abbiamo le connessioni tra l'amigdala laterale (LA), basale (B), accessorio basale (AB) e centrale (CE), l'immagine di destra sottolinea il fatto che ogni nucleo può essere suddiviso in sotto-nuclei. PIR sta per corteccia piriforme, e CPU caudato-putamen (LeDoux 2000). PFC A Corteccia prefrontale Aree corticali associative Corteccia cingolata Nuclei talamici Ippocampo Ipotalamo Amigdala Fig. 2. Schema del circuito neurale proposto da Papez come base neurale per le emozioni con le successive integrazioni di Maclean. In grigio le strutture che oggi sappiamo essere maggiormente coinvolte nelle risposte emozionali. Le connessioni tracciate con le linee tratteggiate rosse fra l’amigdala, le aree corticali prefrontali e la corteccia cingolata non erano previste nel circuito originale. Le abbiamo aggiunte in base alle attuali conoscenze. L’amigdala è cruciale per il provare e manifestare emozioni, non solo per apprendere relazioni fra stimoli o contesti ed eventi emozionalmente “carichi”. La stimolazione elettrica dell’amigdala provoca nell’uomo un forte stato di paura. Lesioni dell’amigdala in animali producono docilità ed assenza di reazioni di paura in risposta a stimoli che normalmente le inducono. Pazienti con lesioni che includono l’amigdala (paziente SM) mostrano difficoltà a reagire e a giudicare le espressioni facciali di felicità, paura, disgusto o tristezza anche se non hanno alcuna difficoltà a riconoscere l’identità delle stesse facce (questa capacità è invece danneggiata da lesioni alle aree visive inferotemporali). Esperimenti di neuroimmagine hanno evidenziato come, in soggetti normali cui vengono mostrate facce con espressione impaurita o felice, l’attivazione dell’amigdala, ed in particolare dell’amigdala sinistra, è significativamente maggiore per le facce impaurite che per quelle felici L’attivazione dell’amigdala è correlata con la “paurosità” della foto Lesioni dell’amigdala impediscono la formazione di tracce di memoria di paura condizionata. Questo significa che soggetti con lesioni dell’amigdala non imparano a reagire in maniera appropriata ad uno stimolo la cui apparizione dovrebbe invece avere valore predittivo per l’accadere di un evento negativo. Differenze amigdala ippocampo (memoria implicita/esplicita) Papez attribuiva all’ippocampo un ruolo centrale nella regolazione delle emozioni, questo ruolo in realtà appartiene all’amigdala. Esempio nell’uomo Compito: Venivano mostrati campi colorati, uno dei colori era associato un fastidioso suono, intenso e di alta frequenza. Lesionati amigdala: ricordo dei colori mostrati e del fatto che alcuni fossero seguiti dal suono, ma nessuna associazione del colore con risposte emotive sistemiche (es. frequenza cardiaca). Lesionati ippocampo: mostravano risposte emotive sistemiche in risposta ai colori associati al suono, ma non si ricordavano che era avvenuta una associazione colore-suono L’ippocampo è importante per le memorie di paura condizionata al contesto Cued fear conditioning ASSOCIAZIONE Amigdala Contextual Fear Conditioning ASSOCIAZIONE Amigdala e ippocampo L’accoppiamento CS-US è necessario e, una volta appresa, la risposta di paura si manifesta anche in contesti diversi In a typical auditory fear conditioning procedure, rats are habituated to the conditioning chamber but given no stimuli. During the conditioning session, the electric shock (US) is paired with the auditory-conditioned stimulus (CS) several times (usually 1–5). The effects of conditioning are then assessed in a test session during which the conditioned stimulus is presented alone. Most studies measure “freezing” behavior, which is an innate defensive response elicited by the conditioned stimulus after conditioning. An unpaired control group in which the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus are presented in a nonoverlapping manner is often used. The conditioned stimulus elicits little or no freezing prior to conditioning (not shown). Both the paired and unpaired group freeze during the training session due to the shock presentation. In the test session, the paired group exhibits considerably more conditioned stimulus-elicited freezing than the unpaired. Differences between the paired and unpaired group reflect the association that is learned as a result of conditioned-unconditioned stimuli pairing. Corteccia acustica primaria Cortecce associative polimodali Corteccia acustica associativa Nucleo colinergico del prosencefalo basale Nucleo genicolato mediale (parte ventrale) Stimolo acustico emozionale Nucleo genicolato mediale (parte mediale) NC Amigdala NL Sistemi di controllo delle risposte emozionali (risposte comportamentali, risposte del SNA, risposte del sistema endocrino Schema semplificato delle vie coinvolte nell’elaborazione di uno stimolo sensoriale (in questo caso acustico) con significato emozionale. Notare che l’informazione sensoriale è inviata al nucleo laterale (NL) dell’amigdala attraverso due vie, una rapida e diretta dai nuclei sensoriali talamici ed una più lenta, ma accompagnata dalla cosciente percezione dello stimolo, che arriva dalle aree corticali acustiche. Particolarmente per le risposte a stimoli paurosi l’importanza dell’informazione che arriva all’amigdala dal talamo sta nel fatto che innesca risposte rapide che possono essere importanti in situazioni di pericolo. Il NBL proietta al nucleo centrale (NC), che costituisce l’uscita dell’amigdala e proietta all’ipotalamo ed a diversi nuclei del tronco dell’encefalo che controllano le risposte emozionali. Esso proietta anche ai nuclei colinergici del prosencefalo basale le cui proiezioni alla corteccia determinano una attivazione corticale generalizzata, detta arousal, e facilitano la modificazione dell’efficacia sinaptica (plasticità sinaptica). Adattata da LeDoux, 1992. Quindi, l’amigdala è in grado di mediare la componente corporea delle emozioni tramite le proiezioni del nucleo centrale: la maggior attività nel nucleo centrale si può pensare che determini, nel condizionamento alla paura, la maggior risposta allo stimolo condizionato. L’amigdala, attraverso le sue proiezioni all’ipotalamo ed al tronco dell’encefalo media le reazioni corporee, la parte inconscia di uno stato emozionale. L’amigdala è anche importante per l’esperienza cosciente delle emozioni: essa proietta infatti alle aree corticali associative, ed in particolare alla corteccia cingolata anteriore ed alla corteccia orbitofrontale E’ possibile evidenziare nell’uomo risposte emozionali a stimoli “non percepiti”? In un lavoro su soggetti normali, (Morris et al., 1999) è stata usata una procedura di rapida presentazione di due stimoli visivi in successione. In questo modo, la presentazione del secondo stimolo previene la percezione cosciente del primo (procedura di masking). Gli stimoli erano costituiti da facce, alcune delle quali (quelle tristi) erano state accoppiate con un forte rumore per provocare una risposta di paura condizionata. Ebbene, la presentazione di queste facce evocava sudorazione delle mani (risposta emozionale) sia quando esse venivano coscientemente percepite sia quando la loro percezione cosciente era prevenuta dal masking. Presentation Presentation Perception Perception Presentation Perception SCR CS+ Presentation CS+ Perception SCR Quindi, è possibile evocare una risposta emotiva con uno stimolo “non visto”. In questo caso si attivava l’amigdala destra in correlazione con l’attivazione del collicolo superiore e del pulvinar (nucleo talamico) ed in correlazione negativa con la corteccia orbitofrontale e con l’area delle facce nella corteccia inferotemporale. Quando invece lo stimolo condizionato veniva coscientemente percepito, l’attivazione dell’amigdala destra correlava positivamente con l’attivazione dell’ippocampo e del cervelletto e negativamente con quella del pulvinar. L’amigdala sinistra non mostrava correlazione con l’attività del pulvinar. Cosa accade nell’amigdala quando si forma una memoria di paura condizionata? Convergence of the auditoryconditioned stimulus and nociceptive unconditioned stimulus in the amygdala is essential for fear conditioning. Convergence of conditioned and unconditioned stimuli occurs in lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA), especially in the dorsal subnucleus (LAd), leading to synaptic plasticity in LA. Plasticity may also occur in the central nucleus (CE) and in the auditory thalamus. LA connects with CE directly and indirectly by way of connections in the basal (B), accessory basal (AB), and intercalated cell masses (ICM). CE connects with hypothalamic and brainstem areas that control the expression of conditioned fear responses, including freezing and autonomic (ANS) and hormonal responses. LeDoux, 2011 In a Hebbian model of fear conditioning, strong depolarization of LA pyramidal cells evoked by the aversive unconditioned stimulus leads to strengthening of coactive conditioned stimulus inputs onto the same neurons (remember inception of false memories, Tonegawa’s paper 2014). Existing data support the idea that LA associative plasticity and fear memory formation are triggered by unconditioned stimulusinduced activation of LA neurons. Thus, unconditioned stimulus-evoked depolarization is necessary for the enhancement of conditioned stimulus-elicited neural responses in LA after conditioned-unconditioned stimuli pairing (Rosenkranz and Grace, 2002), and pairing a conditioned stimulus with direct depolarization of LA pyramidal neurons as an unconditioned stimulus supports fear conditioning (Johansen et al., 2010). Though there is evidence that Hebbian plasticity in LA may not entirely explain fear conditioning, it is clear that synaptic plasticity at conditioned stimulus input pathways to the LA does occur with fear conditioning. Supporting this, in vivo studies demonstrate an enhancement of auditory stimulus-evoked responses in LA neurons after fear conditioning. Potenziali d’azione/sec Il condizionamento alla paura comporta variazioni della efficacia trasmissione sinaptica fra i neuroni uditivi ed I loro bersagli nell’amigdala Working Model of Molecular Processes in the amygdala Mediating Acquisition and Consolidation of Fear Memories All dotted lines denote hypothetical pathways. Molecules and processes in blue are known to be involved in the acquisition of fear conditioning. Molecules and process in black are known to be involved specifically in the consolidation or maintenance of fear conditioning. Purple labels denote molecules or elements whose role is not established for fear conditioning but are part of an established intracellular signaling pathway. Abbreviations: AC, adenyl cyclase; AKAP, A-kinase anchoring protein; Arc, activity-regulated cytoskeletal-associated protein; β-AR, βadrenergic receptor; BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; Ca2+, calcium; CaMKII, Ca2+/calmodulin (Cam)-dependent protein kinase II; CREB, cAMP response element (CRE) binding protein; GluA1, glutamate AMPA receptor subunit 1; GluA2/3, glutamate AMPA receptor subunit 2 and 3 heteromer; IP3, inositol 1,4,5-triphosphate; MAPK, mitogen-activated protein kinase; mGluR, metabotropic glutamate receptor; mTOR, mammalian target of rapamycin; NF-kB, nuclear factor κ light-chain enhancer of activated B cells; NMDAR, N-methyl-d-aspartate glutamate receptor; NO, nitric oxide; NOS, nitric oxide synthase; PI3-K, phosphatidylinositol-3 kinase; PKA, protein kinase A; PKC, protein kinase C; PKG, cGMP-dependent protein kinase; PKMζ, protein kinase M ζ; TrkB, tyrosine kinase B; VGCC, voltage-gated calcium channel. Monoamine Neuromodulatory-Dependent Mechanisms Involved in Fear Learning Though Hebbian plasticity may indeed occur during learning, it does not fully explain learning — especially learning in highly charged emotional situations. It is generally thought that monoamine transmitters such as norepinepherine (NE) and dopamine (DA) that are released in emotional situations regulate glutamatergic transmission and Hebbian plasticity (for review, see [Bailey et al., 2000] and [McGaugh, 2000] , and Tully and Bolshakov, 2010). The modulation of Hebbian (or activity-dependent) plasticity by neuromodulators (such as monoamines) or plasticity that is independent of postsynaptic activity is called heterosynaptic plasticity. This is in contrast with purely activity-dependent Hebbian plasticity, which is referred to as homosynaptic plasticity (Bailey et al., 2000). Indeed, in a variety of model systems, it has been shown that monoamines modulate plasticity underlying memory formation (Carew et al., 1984] and [Bailey et al., 2000] , and Glanzman, 2010 for review). Neuromodulators also contribute to fear conditioning. Tono noradrenergico nell’amigdala alto: maggior probabilità che si inneschi e consolidi un potenziamento delle risposte dell’amigdala allo stimolo condizionato appaiato allo stimolo incondizionato. Tono noradrenergico basso: si verifica l’opposto. A quale stato comportamentale corrisponde un tono noradrenergico alto? L’apprendimento di risposte di paura condizionata coinvolge quindi cambiamenti duraturi dell’efficacia sinaptica di tipo potenziamento a lungo termine (LTP) nel nucleo laterale. L’ipotesi è che l’attività evocata nel nucleo laterale dalla presentazione dello stimolo condizionato, quando appaiata con l’attività evocata dallo stimolo incondizionato, provochi un potenziamento della risposta allo stimolo condizionato (proprietà associativa di LTP). Il nucleo laterale dell’amigdala proietta al nucleo centrale, che costituisce la principale uscita dell’amigdala. La maggior risposta del nucleo laterale allo stimolo condizionato determinerà una maggior attivazione del nucleo centrale. Le proiezioni del nucleo centrale sono dirette all’ipotalamo e alle strutture del tronco dell’encefalo coinvolte nel controllo del sistema nervoso autonomo: in questo modo l’amigdala controlla sia il sistema nervoso autonomo che il sistema endocrino che l’asse ipotalamo-ipofisisurrene. Indurre risposte di paura in assenza di stimoli condizionati (o incondizionati) attraverso la semplice stimolazione optogenetica dell’amigdala centrale mediale Coronal section of the mouse brain indicating the location of the central amygdala (CEA). CEl/CEm, lateral/medial subdivisions of CEA. Numbers indicate the antero-posterior coordinates caudal to bregma. b, Red fluorescent neurons in CEm infected with AAV-ChR22A-tdimer. Scale bar, 100 μm. c, Left: example experiment illustrating rapid and reversible freezing induced by bilateral stimulation of ChR2-expressing CEm neurons with 10 s of blue light (inter-stimulation intervals, 30–60 s). Right: summary data demonstrating significant light-induced freezing responses in AAV-ChR22A-tdimer infected animals, but not in sham-operated controls. Circuiti nei quali si manifesta plasticità nell’acquisizione e nell’estinzione del “fear conditioning” PKC-d+ Fear CS presentation induced an increase (in CElon neurons) or a decrease (in CEloff neurons) in firing. S Ciocchi et al. Nature 468, 277-282 (2010) Is freezing a suitable behavioural parameter to assess contextual fear conditioning? Fin qui 11 novembre 2015 Studies from many laboratories have reported that in rats, lesions or inactivation of the basolateral amygdala complex (BLC; the set of lateral, basal, and accessory basal nuclei of the amygdala) decrease the immobility or “freezing” (defined as lack of movement except for respiration) displayed in the presence of specific cues or contexts previously paired with footshock Such findings have suggested that the BLC may be critical for storing cue– footshock or context–footshock learned associations and that the BLC may be a locus of the neuroplasticity that mediates fear conditioning. A major difficulty with these interpretations is that lesions and other treatments that disrupt amygdala functioning also decrease unlearned fear, or anxiety. (Vazdarijanova e McGaugh, 1998) I comportamenti rintracciabili dopo un training di apprendimento che possono essere presi in considerazione come indici di un apprendimento nel paradigma del contextual fear condition sono (Vazdarjanova McGaugh 1998): 1.La latenza della prima entrata nella zona dell’apparato in cui è avvenuto l’apprendimento (memoria esplicita); 2.Il numero di entrate nei singoli bracci (memoria esplicita); 3.Il freezing definito come l’assenza totale di movimenti eccetto quelli respiratori (memoria implicita); La componente esplicita ed implicita della memoria emotiva hanno un decorso di ritenzione diverso LATENZA D'INGRESSO "braccio shock" GRUPPO ST. SHOCK GRUPPO ST.CONTROLLO 140,00 latenza d'ingresso sec. 120,00 100,00 80,00 60,00 40,00 20,00 0,00 Abituazione Test 7° giorno 14°giorno 21°giorno 28°giorno 35°giorno TEMPO SPESO IN FREEZING"braccio Shock" GRUPPO STANDARD SHOCK GRUPPO STANDARD CONTROLLO 8,00 7,00 6,00 freezing sec. 5,00 4,00 3,00 2,00 1,00 0,00 -1,00 -2,00 Abituazione Test 7°giorno 14°giorno 21°giorno 28°giorno 35°gionro Recall of remote fear memories Visual, acoustic, and olfactory stimuli associated with a highly charged emotional situation take on the affective qualities of that situation. Where the emotional meaning of a given sensory experience is stored is a matter of debate. Secondary auditory cortex Remote Sacchetti 2010 Secondary auditory cortex and fear memory Secondary cortices that perform high-level sensory analysis combine sensory processing and memory plasticity to encode the behavioral salience of perceiving stimuli. Such information becomes widely distributed throughout the cortex, each secondary sensory cortex coding the valence of stimuli of a specific modality. Such a memory storage mechanism results in a synaptic strengthening of corticocortical connections that may provide the integrated view of the whole emotional experience during memory recall. Dysregulation of the fear system is at the core of many disorders. Despite recent advances, the question of how to persistently weaken aversive CSUS associations, or dampen traumatic memories in pathological cases, remains a major dilemma. The term ‘emotion regulation’ refers to the different types of regulatory processes that can control the physiological, behavioral, and experiential components of our affective responses (Gross and Thompson, 2007). These include automatic forms of regulation that flexibly alter our emotional responses as we learn about changing stimulus outcome contingencies in our environment, as well as intentionally deployed techniques. We may change how we think about an emotion-evoking stimulus, or shift our focus of attention to diminish an undesired emotion. We may also take action to avoid or cope with a distressing situation or to bring about a positive outcome. Effective regulation of emotion through these various processes is essential for both our mental and physical well being. Cancellare o ridurre l’impatto di memorie traumatiche intrusive Strategie utilizzate: 1. Sfruttare il riconsolidamento 2. Usare protocolli di estinzione 3. Cognitive regulation 4. Active coping During extinction, fear is diminished through learning that a previously threatening stimulus no longer signals danger. Cognitive emotion regulation involves using various mental strategies to modify a fear response. In active coping, fear is regulated through the performance of behaviors that reduce exposure to a fear-evoking stimulus. Finally, a fear memory can be disrupted after it is recalled through pharmacological or behavioral manipulations that block its reconsolidation. Our understanding of the neurocircuitry underlying the control of fear stems from research across species clarifying the mechanisms by which we learn and modify emotional associations, as well as studies exploring forms of cognitive emotion regulation that are uniquely human. Failure to properly regulate fear responses has been associated with various forms of psychopathology. For example, some forms of anxiety disorders are thought to involve dysfunction in the neural systems underlying the extinction of fear learning (see Rauch et al, 2006) and are treated with extinction-based therapies. “Facilitating emotion regulation through intentional cognitive mechanisms is a primary aim of cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy, a successful approach to the treatment of depression and other psychological disorders.” Hartley and Phelps, 2010 “Active coping may be used to attenuate learned fear responses and mitigate the functional impairments engendered by a fear evoking stimulus.” Hartley and Phelps, 2010 Finally, reconsolidation may permit the permanent modification of the pathological traumatic memories. Hartley and Phelps, 2010 “An improved understanding of the neurocircuitry of normal emotion regulation sheds light on the potential mechanisms underlying specific disorders, and may aid in the development of more effective treatments for these conditions.” Hartley and Phelps, 2010 Il riconsolidamento Il riconsolidamento The classic view of memory suggests that immediately after learning, there is a period of time during which the memory is fragile and labile, but after sufficient time has passed the memory is, more or less, permanent. During this consolidation period, it is possible to disrupt the formation of the memory, but once this time window has passed, the memory may be modified or inhibited, but not eliminated. Il riconsolidamento However, recent studies support an alternative view of memory in which every time a memory is retrieved, the underlying memory trace is once again labile and fragile, requiring another consolidation period called reconsolidation. Given that fear memories can, at times, be maladaptive, contributing to fear or anxiety disorders, the possibility of disrupting an earlier acquired fear memory by blocking reconsolidation could have significant clinical implications. The interest in reconsolidation was sparked by a finding by Nader et al (2000), showing that conditioned fear can be eliminated by blocking reconsolidation in the amygdala of a reactivated fear memory. Fear memories undergo reconsolidation only when they are retrieved and this reconsolidation process can be disrupted, essentially eliminating the earlier learned fear. Blockade of reconsolidation is specific to the fear memory reactivated, leaving other memories intact (Doyere et al, 2007). Retrieval and reconsolidation alter memories via synaptic plasticity in lateral amygdala (LA). Changes in short-latency AEFPs in lateral amygdala. A selective additional potentiation was observed during PR-STM (left panel, P < 0.05, contrast analysis) after reactivation with the pure tone conditioned stimulus. U0126 produced a depotentiation of lateral amygdala responses to the reactivated conditioned stimulus (CSr), regardless of tone type (right panel, P < 0.05, two-way ANOVA). (c) Changes in AEFPs in the auditory thalamus. No differential effect of reactivation or intra–lateral amygdala infusion of U0126 on either memory test was observed. Fear memories erased by disrupting reconsolidation processes do not return with passage of time, or the alteration of contextual cues, or additional stress (Duvarci and Nader, 2004). Owing to the lack of return of fear, blocking reconsolidation provides an important advantage over other techniques that might be used to regulate emotion. If, as suggested, the memory trace is permanently altered, the inhibition of the fear memory through extinction, regulation, or adaptive action is not necessary. Research on the blockade of the reconsolidation of fear memories in humans has been slow to emerge for a few reasons. One primary reason is that the initial findings showing the blockade of reconsolidation relied on the administration of protein synthesis inhibitors, which is not a viable technique in humans. Recently, however, two techniques that can be translated to humans have been identified. The first uses a beta-adrenergic antagonist, propranolol, which is safe to administer to humans, to block reconsolidation. Debiec and LeDoux (2004) showed that either intraamygdala or systemic administration of propranolol immediately after reactivation of the CS blocked the reconsolidation of conditioned fear and prevented the return of fear. A recent finding by Kindt et al (2009) found that the administration of propranolol can block the return of fear in human participants. “From an evolutionary perspective, it is extremely functional to never forget the most important events in life. However, the putative indelibility of emotional memory can also be harmful and maladaptive, such as in some trauma victims who suffer from dreadful memories and anxiety. If emotional memory could be weakened or even erased, then we might be able to eliminate the root of many disorders, such as post-traumatic stress disorder.” Kindt et al., 2009 Testing included different phases across 3 d: fear acquisition (day 1), memory reactivation (day 2), and extinction followed by a reinstatement procedure and a test phase (day 3) The conditioned fear response was measured as potentiation of the eyeblink startle reflex to a loud noise (40 ms, 104 dB) by electromyography of the right orbicularis oculi muscle. Stronger startle responses to the loud noise during the fearconditioned stimulus (CS1+) as compared with the control stimulus (CS2-) reflects the fearful state of the participant elicited by CS1+. Startle potentiation taps directly into the amygdala, and fearconditioning procedures yield highly reliable and robust startle potentiation Mean startle potentiation to the fear-conditioned stimulus (CS1), the control stimulus (CS2) and noise alone (NA) trials during acquisition (trial 1–8), extinction (trial 1–10) and test (trial 1–5) for the placebo (n = 20, a,b), propranolol reactivation (n = 20, c,d) and propranolol without reactivation (n = 20, e,f) group. CS1+ refers to the fear conditioned stimulus during acquisition, CS1- refers to the fear conditioned stimulus during extinction and test, CS1-R refers to the reactivation of the fear conditioned stimulus and CS2- refers to the control stimulus during all phases of the experiment. Error bars represent s.e.m. In the procedure of Kindt et al (2009), propranolol was given before reactivation. If propranolol blocks the expression and return of fear memories, this suggests a potentially promising clinical intervention. Earlier research has suggested that propranolol might be clinically useful if administered immediately after a traumatic event. It is suggested that propranolol might impair the initial consolidation of fear memories, and thus prevent the development of PTSD in the future (Pitman and Delahanty, 2005). However, this treatment is not applicable to someone who already suffers from PTSD. To explore whether the administration of propranolol during the reconsolidation period might be useful in the treatment of clinical disorders, Brunet et al (2008) tested whether it could prevent the return of older fear memories in patients suffering from PTSD. The patients were asked to describe their traumatic events to reactivate their traumatic memories. Immediately after reactivation, patients were given either propranolol or placebo. A week later during a script-driven memory task, physiological responses to these memories were assessed. The patients who received propranolol showed diminished physiological fear responses for most of the measures assessed. Although this initial clinical study had a small sample, moderate effects, and lacked some important control conditions, it provides preliminary evidence that propranolol could be an important tool in the treatment of anxiety disorders by targeting the reconsolidation process. The studies examining means to block the reconsolidation of fear memories to date have primarily used pharmacological manipulations. However, reconsolidation is not a process that evolved via pharmacological manipulation in the service of treating clinical disorders. Rather reconsolidation is a natural memory process that allows older memories to be updated to incorporate new information each time they are retrieved. On account of this, it is possible that presenting specific new information during the reconsolidation window can provide another means to alter or block older fear memories. To explore whether the reconsolidation of fear memories can be influenced without pharmacological manipulation, Monfils et al (2009) developed a behavioral intervention aimed at blocking reconsolidation. To understand this work, we need to examine first the other approach, extintion, because the Monfils paper uses the reconsolidation approach coupled with extintion. Fin qui 16 novembre 2015 L’estinzione consiste nella riduzione della risposta emotiva condizionata, determinata dall’apprendimento dell’associazione tra uno stimolo condizionato (CS) ed uno incondizionato (US), in seguito alla ripetuta esposizione allo CS in assenza dello US. L’estinzione non ha caratteristiche costanti durante l’ontogenesi. Apprendimento in contesto A, estinzione in contesto B Extinction learning involves the formation of a novel stimulus-outcome association. The CS that earlier predicted danger now predicts safety. This new extinction memory does not erase or overwrite the memory for the original CS–US association. This is evidenced by the re-emergence of the CR in certain circumstances including a shift in context (renewal), unsignaled presentation of the US (reinstatement), or the mere passage of time (spontaneous recovery) (context B) Livello di freezing al termine dell’estinzione 10 giorni prima (context A) Mechanisms of fear extintion When the CS is present, the LA excites the central nucleus (CE), which controls passive forms of expression of the CR through descending projections to the brainstem and hypothalamus. The LA also has indirect projections to the CE, through the basal nucleus (B) and the intercalated (ITC) cell masses, clusters of inhibitory GABAergic neurons. The B itself also projects directly to the ITC. These pathways provide multiple potential circuits for gating fear expression. Fin qui 24 novembre sviluppo Knowledge of the fear conditioning circuitry has allowed researchers to investigate functional changes that occur during extinction. Research in rats using lesions, pharmacological manipulations, and electrophysiology are providing an increasingly detailed model of the neural circuitry of fear extinction. Recent studies in human beings have been consistent with the animal literature. This body of research suggests that interaction between the amygdala, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and the hippocampus supports the acquisition, storage, retrieval, and contextual modulation of fear extinction Acquisition and consolidation of extintion Amygdala Recent studies have shown that blockade of NMDA (SotresBayon et al, 2007) or glutamate (Kim et al, 2007) receptors within the basolateral amygdala complex (BLA) impaired extinction learning and that the blockade of mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPk) activity in the basolateral nucleus entirely prevented the acquisition of extinction (Herry et al, 2006). Furthermore, several studies suggest that morphological changes in the BLA synapses after extinction training support the consolidation of extinction learning (Lin et al, 2003; Chatwal et al, 2005, 2006; Markram et al, 2007). Consistent with the idea that extinction constitutes new learning, not erasure of the original fear memory, Repa et al (2001) identified a population of neurons in the lateral amygdala whose response to the CS decreased during extinction training, as well as a second population in which the CS response remained high despite a decrease in the expression of conditioned fear. This finding provides further evidence that the amygdala supports the maintenance of the original fear memory when simultaneously facilitating extinction learning. Although the amygdala seems to be critical for the acquisition of extinction learning, convergent evidence suggests that the vmPFC is necessary for the retention and recall of extinction. In line with the well-documented observation in human beings and primates that damage to the PFC leads to perseverative behavior (see Sotres-Bayon et al, 2006 for a review), Morgan et al (1993) observed that rats with vmPFC lesions required many more unreinforced presentations of the CS to extinguish conditioned responding. A subsequent study pointed to the infralimbic (IL) region of the vmPFC as a potential site of extinction consolidation, reporting that pre-training IL lesions left within-session acquisition of extinction intact, but impaired extinction retrieval on the following day (Quirk et al, 2000). Infusion studies showing that impairing long term plasticity (disruption of protein synthesis (Santini et al, 2004), MAPk blockade (Hugues et al, 2006), and administration of an NMDA antagonist (Burgos-Robles et al, 2007)) within the vmPFC all impair retrieval of extinction suggest that the plasticity in this region supports extinction consolidation mPFC could act on inhibitory lateral amygdala neurons or on intercalated (inhibitory) neurons. The results is a decrease of central amygdala output. Infusion Burgos-Robles et a., 2007 Bursting activity may reflect hippocampal input to mPFC Paré found that fear extinction in rats was associated with a potentiation of fear input synapses from BLA to GABAergic intercalated amygdala neurons that project to the CEA. Enhancement of inputs to intercalated cells required prefrontal activity during extinction training. a) Experimental procedure. (b) Intensity-dependence of BLA-evoked responses in ITC cells from the vehicle (n = 15) and muscimol (n = 11) groups (average ± s.e.m.). Dashed line indicates data from unpaired group Inset, extent of fluorophore-conjugated muscimol diffusion in the infralimbic cortex. LA Extintion Acquisition BLA CEA ITC mPFC Fear Relapse (spontaneous retrieval of fear memory, reinstatement, renewal) Since extintion does not erase the original fear memory, the CS has an ambiguous valence, it could be fear eliciting or not, depending on: 1. the level of activation of the US representation during non reinforced trials, 2. the decreases in attention, which can influence conditioned performance during extinction, 3. contexts. La risposta emotiva può perciò riapparire a causa del recupero spontaneo, della ripresentazione dello SI o del rinnovo (se lo SC è presentato in un contesto differente da quello dell’estinzione). Da qui 4 dicembre After extinction, contextual information has a critical function in determining whether the original fear memory or the new extinction memory should control fear expression. Evidence suggests that hippocampal projections to the vmPFC and the amygdala mediate the context-dependent expression of extinction (Fanselow, 2000; Ji and Maren, 2005). Conditioning day Extintion day Test day Effects of Dorsal Hippocampus lesions on AAA/ABA renewal. (A) Mean (±SEM) percentage of freezing exhibited on the conditioning day, with 3 min prior to tone CS onset (baseline) and 1 min post-CS for five trials. All rats were fear conditioned in context A. (B) Extinction to the tone CS. Mean (±SEM) percentage of freezing for the first four CS presentations across the 3 d of extinction in contexts A and B. (C) Mean (±SEM) percentage of freezing for the first 4 min after CS onset during test. Rats were tested for fear of CS in the conditioning context, either in the extinction context (SAME; AAA; open bars) or outside of the extinction context (DIFF; ABA; filled bars). Electrolytic DH (DH-Pre) or sham (SH) lesions were made before conditioning. Ji and Maren, 2005 Furthermore, inactivation of the hippocampus before extinction learning impairs extinction recall on the subsequent day (Corcoran et al, 2005), suggesting that the hippocampus regulates fear expression both outside and within the extinction context. One suggestion is that the hippocampus controls the contextspecific retrieval of extinction through projections to the vmPFC (Corcoran and Quirk, 2007). Another possibility is that the hippocampus gates fear expression directly through projections to the LA. Although clarification of the precise circuitry requires further investigation, there is strong evidence that the hippocampus, through communication with the vmPFC and the amygdala, regulates the contextual modulation of fear expression during extinction retrieval. Ruolo della neurogenesi ippocampale nell’espressione di comportamenti ansiosi e nel pattern separation Ipotesi sul ruolo della neurogenesi ippocampale: Formazione di cluster temporali di memorie (retrieval episodico) (Aimone e Gage) Separazione di contesti diversi (Sahai et al., 2011) Meno neurogenesi, più generalizzazione (FC in contesto A, test in contesto B, mancanza di discriminazione, come nel sistema olfattivo) Ippocampo ventrale e dorsale: ruoli diversi Ippocampo dorsale cognition of context; ippocampo ventrale connesso con il sistema limbico, espressione di ansietà (blocco con chRh dei neuroni dell’ippocampo ventrale, animali esplorano i bracci aperti dell’EPM) Legame con PTSD: eccessiva generalizzazione è un evento maladattivo, un singolo reminder del contesto originale può provocare il richiamo dell’intero contesto traumatico anche in un contesto “sicuro” B A Menta Contesto del condizionamento Menta Contesto sicuro Research examining extinction learning in humans has been largely consistent with the findings from rodent studies. Initial fMRI studies of extinction learning in humans reported an increase in amygdala activation during early extinction after the shift in the CS–US contingency (LaBar et al, 1998; Gottfried and Dolan, 2004; Knight et al, 2004; Phelps et al, 2004). Amygdala activation decreased as extinction progressed and remained low during extinction The magnitude of the decrease in amygdala activation during extinction learning correlated with the degree of participants’ extinction retrieval (Phelps et al, 2004). These findings further support a function for the amygdala in extinction learning and confirm that amygdala activity is reduced during extinction retrieval. The first fMRI study examining both extinction learning and subsequent retrieval in humans supported the function of the vmPFC in extinction retrieval (Phelps et al, 2004). Fear conditioning fMRI studies have typically reported decreases in the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal in the vmPFC below the resting baseline when the CS is presented during fear acquisition (Gottfried and Dolan, 2004; Phelps et al, 2004). BOLD signal in this region increases toward baseline during initial extinction learning, and increases further during extinction retrieval (Phelps et al, 2004). Subsequent studies have also observed increased vmPFC activation during extinction retrieval (Kalisch et al, 2006; Milad et al, 2007). The degree of increase in activity in the vmPFC as well as the thickness of the cortex in this region are correlated with the degree of extinction success during recall (Milad et al, 2005, 2007). Anterior cingulate region has been proposed as a potential human homologue of the rodent IL region (Kim et al, 2003), and may inhibit conditioned fear expression during extinction retrieval, as suggested by the animal literature. Extinction-based therapies have been widely explored as potential avenues of treatment for anxiety disorders (Barlow, 2002; Garakani et al, 2006). Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), in which patients display spontaneous or cue induced re-experiencing of a traumatic event, may result from failure to consolidate and retrieve extinction learning (see Rauch et al, 2006 for a review). Consistent with this theory, PTSD patients exhibit deficits in extinction retention (Orr et al, 2000), along with reduced vmPFC and hippocampal volume and activity and increased amygdala activity (Gilbertson et al, 2002; Bremner, 2006; Liberzon and Martis, 2006; Shin et al, 2006). fMRI studies examining the context-dependent recall of extinction in humans point to an important function for the hippocampus (Kalisch et al, 2006; Milad et al, 2007). In such studies, context is manipulated through changes in the background color or the visual scene in which the CS is presented. Extinction learning takes place only in one of the contexts, allowing activation correlated with context dependent retrieval to be examined. Increased hippocampal activation was observed during extinction retrieval (Kalisch et al, 2006; Milad et al, 2007). Activation in the hippocampus was positively correlated with vmPFC activation, providing further support for the notion that the hippocampus may mediate context-dependent extinction recall through connections with the vmPFC. Finally, confirming the function of the hippocampus in contextual reinstatement, a human lesion study found that contextual reinstatement of the CR was impaired in individuals with hippocampal lesions (LaBar and Phelps, 2005), similar to the findings observed in rodents (Wilson et al, 1995). Consistent with studies in animal models, extinction learning in humans seems therefore to depend on the integrated functioning of a neural circuit that includes the amygdala, the vmPFC, and the hippocampus. This convergent evidence across species suggests that the neural mechanisms supporting fear extinction are highly phylogenetically conserved. Hippocampus, amygdala and mPFC are often disfunctional in PTSD L’esperire eventi traumatici e paurosi può determinare nell’uomo l’insorgenza di un disturbo d’ansia, la cui cura è ostacolata dal riaffiorare delle memorie a valenza negativa legate all’evento. Alcuni disturbi, come il disturbo post traumatico da stress (PTSD), sono associati ad un deficit di estinzione (Milad et al., 2009). A clinical characteristic of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is persistently elevated fear responses to stimuli associated with the traumatic event. The objective of the Milad paper is to determine whether extinction of fear responses is impaired in PTSD and whether such impairment is related to dysfunctional activation of brain regions known to be involved in fear extinction, e.g., amygdala, hippocampus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). Individuals with PTSD typically show exaggerated amygdala and diminished hippocampal activation relative to controls. The dACC has emerged as another brain region that appears hyperactive in PTSD. Most studies have shown that vmPFC is hypoactive in this disorder, but a few have reported hyperactivity The objective of the Milad study was to examine the neurobiological basis of deficient extinction recall in PTSD with a focus on the above-mentioned brain regions. While in a 3T fMRI scanner, PTSD and trauma-exposed, nonPTSD control (TENC) subjects underwent a two-day Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction procedure previously used in healthy and PTSD subjects. Skin conductance response (SCR), a commonly used measure in human fear studies served as the dependent measure of conditioned responding. On day 1, subjects underwent fear conditioning to two pictures of differently colored lamps, followed by extinction for one of them. Day 2 tested recall of the extinction that had been learned the previous day by contrasting responses to the previously extinguished and unextinguished stimuli. A total of 19 PTSD patients and 20 trauma-exposed non-PTSD control (TENC) subjects were recruited from the community. Late extinction learning (day 1) Early extinction recall (day 2) These findings support the hypothesis that fear extinction is impaired in PTSD. They further suggest that dysfunctional activation in brain structures that mediate fear extinction learning, and especially its recall, underlie this impairment. Disturbi d’ansia e persistenza di memorie emotive hanno una prevalenza familiare, suggerendo la presenza di componenti genetiche come fattori di rischio nell’insorgenza di disturbi. A Genetic Variant BDNF Polymorphism Alters Extinction Learning in Both Mouse and Human (Soliman et al., 2010) BDNF mediates synaptic plasticity associated with learning and memory specifically in fear learning and extinction. BDNF-dependent forms of fear learning have known biological substrates, and lie at the core of a number of clinical disorders associated with the variant BDNF. Fear learning paradigms require the ability to recognize and remember cues that signal safety or threat and to extinguish these associations when they no longer exist. As we have seen, these abilities are impaired in anxiety disorders such as posttraumatic stress disorder and phobias and behavioral treatments for these disorders rely on basic principles of extinction learning. Understanding the effect of the BDNF Met allele on these forms of learning can provide insight into the mechanism of risk for anxiety disorders. The objective of this study was to test if the Val66Met genotype could impact extinction learning in a mouse model, and if such findings could be generalized to human populations. Fin qui 24 The neuroimaging findings of diminished ventromedial prefrontal activity and elevated amygdala activity during extinction are reminiscent of those reported in patients with anxiety disorders and depression when presented with empty threat or aversive stimuli (e.g., fearful faces). Understanding the effect of the BDNF Met allele on specific components of a simple form of learning provides insight into risk for anxiety disorders and has important implications for the efficacy of treatments for these disorders that rely on extinction mechanisms. These findings suggest that the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism may play a key role in the efficacy of such treatments and may ultimately guide personalized medicine for related clinical disorders. BDNF agisce dove? Induction of Fear Extinction with Hippocampal-Infralimbic BDNF (Peters et al., 2010) As we have seen, the extinction of conditioned fear memories requires plasticity in the infralimbic medial prefrontal cortex (IL mPFC), but little is known about the molecular mechanisms involved. Epigenetic regulation within the IL mPFC of the gene encoding BDNF correlates with fear extinction. Because BDNF is a major molecular mediator of memory consolidation, the authors hypothesized that BDNF is responsible for consolidating extinction memory within the ILmPFC. If true, it should be possible to enhance extinction via direct application of BDNF to the ILmPFC Research showing the facilitation of extinction learning through pharmacological means (Walker et al, 2002; Ressler et al, 2004; see Anderson and Insel, 2006; Quirk and Mueller, 2008 for reviews) suggests that drug administration may enhance the efficacy of such extinction based therapies. Finally, a recent study reported that mice lacking the serotonin transporter gene show marked deficits in extinction retention (Wellman et al, 2007). As human beings with the low-expressing short allele variant of this gene exhibit decreased functional connectivity between the vmPFC and amygdala (Pezawas et al, 2005), this suggests a possible genetic basis for individual differences in extinction learning, as well as a potential risk factor for the development of anxiety disorders. NORADRENERGIC ENHANCEMENT OF RECONSOLIDATION IN THE AMYGDALA IMPAIRS EXTINCTION OF CONDITIONED FEAR IN RATS—A POSSIBLE MECHANISM FOR THE PERSISTENCE OF TRAUMATIC MEMORIES IN PTSD Debiec et al., 2011 Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is associated with enhanced noradrenergic activity. Animal and human studies demonstrate that noradrenergic stimulation augments consolidation of fear learning. Retrieval of well-established memories by presenting a learned fear cue triggers reconsolidation processes during which memories may be updated, weakened, or strengthened. Noradrenergic blockade in the rat amygdala impairs reconsolidation of fear memories. Noradrenergic enhancement on reconsolidation of learned fear? Clinical research indicates that the persistence and severity of PTSD symptoms is associated with increased noradrenergic activity long after the traumatic event. It is possible that norepinephrine is involved not only in the original encoding but also in the maintenance and exacerbation of symptoms associated with traumatic memories. The major finding of this study was that beta-adrenergic receptor agonist isoproterenol infused into the lateral amygdala following retrieval of a conditioned fear impaired its extinction 48 hr later Noradrenergic enhancement of reconsolidation may also serve as a model for delayed-onset PTSD, whereas PTSD symptoms emerge and exacerbate over time. Hippocampal dysfunction effects on context memory: Possible etiology for posttraumatic stress disorder. Acheson DT, Gresack JE, Risbrough VB. Hippocampal volume reductions and functional impairments are reliable findings in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) imaging studies. However, it is not clear if and how hippocampal dysfunction contributes to the etiology and maintenance of PTSD. Individuals with PTSD are often described as showing fear responses to trauma reminders outside of contexts in which these cues would reasonably predict danger. Animal studies suggest that the hippocampus is required to form and recall associations between contextual stimuli and aversive events. Conversely, the hippocampus is not required for associations with discrete cues. In animal studies, if configural memory is disrupted, learning strategies using discrete cue associations predominate. These data suggest poor hippocampal function could bias the organism toward forming multiple simple cue associations during trauma, thus increasing the chances of fear responses in multiple environments (or contexts) in which these simple cues may be present. 2. Neural circuit abnormalities reported in PTSD 2.1. Cortical and subcortical findings Structural MRI and functional MRI studies in PTSD report differences in volume and activation of limbic circuitry mediating stress responding, fear memory, and emotional modulation ( [Garfinkel and Liberzon, 2009] and [Rauch et al., 2006] ). A recent meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies in anxiety disorders supports the notion of dysfunction in prefrontal-limbic areas in PTSD, showing modest amygdala hyperactivation along with more pronounced hypoactivation in the medial prefrontal cortex as well as the rostral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Etkin and Wager, 2007). Another robust finding in neuroimaging research is reduced gray matter volume in the ACC in patients with PTSD ( [Yamasue et al., 2003] , [Corbo et al., 2005] and [Kitayama et al., 2006] ). Kasai et al. (2008) found that combat-exposed twins with PTSD had lower gray matter volumes in the subgenual ACC compared to their trauma-exposed non-PTSD co-twins. This finding suggests that subgenual ACC volume reductions are an acquired feature of PTSD, rather than a vulnerability factor. However, in another recent study, Shin et al. (2009) used positron emission tomography (PET) to assess resting metabolic activity in the dorsal ACC in veterans with PTSD and their non-combat-exposed co-twins. They found that veterans with PTSD and their co-twin both exhibited higher resting metabolic rates than veterans without PTSD and their co-twins. These results suggest that metabolic abnormalities in the dorsal ACC represent a preexisting vulnerability factor for the development of PTSD subsequent to trauma. More research is needed to clarify the nature of prefrontal/ACC abnormalities in PTSD, and how they may contribute to the development or maintenance of the disorder. In order to understand how hippocampal abnormalities contribute to development and/or maintenance of PTSD, an important question is whether this reduced volume is a trait in vulnerable individuals or, rather, a manifestation of the disease state after experiencing trauma. Recent research is beginning to support a conceptualization of these abnormalities as a pre-existing vulnerability factor for development of the disorder. [Gilbertson et al., 2002] and [Gilbertson et al., 2007] examined hippocampal volume and function in a sample of monozygotic twins discordant for trauma exposure. In the first study, the authors compared hippocampal volume, as assessed by structural MRI, across twins with and without PTSD, and also correlated volume with PTSD severity in the exposed twin (Gilbertson et al., 2002). Findings supported smaller, predominately right, hippocampal volume in severe PTSD vs. traumaexposed non-PTSD subjects. Interestingly, twins of severe PTSD subjects showed hippocampal volumes comparable to their brothers and significantly lower than both trauma-exposed non-PTSD patients as well as their non-traumaexposed twins. Disorder severity in PTSD subjects was negatively correlated with both their own hippocampal volumes and with those of their trauma-discordant twin. Thus, hippocampal volume may be a vulnerability trait that contributes to the development of PTSD upon trauma exposure. Support for the alternative idea that hippocampal reductions manifest only after trauma (i.e. “state”) is predominantly based on evidence for stress- and glucocorticoid signaling-induced reductions of hippocampal volume It has been hypothesized that that trauma exposure induces HPA axis dysregulation, resulting in hippocampal atrophy in vulnerable individuals (Elzinga and Bremner, 2002). Measures of circulating cortisol levels in PTSD patients have been inconsistent however, predominantly with reports of normal or low levels of cortisol and increased negative feedback (for review see Yehuda, 2009). Recent findings of alterations in second messenger systems involved in glucocorticoid signaling (Binder, 2009) suggest that, although glucocorticoid levels may be relatively normal, patient’s ability to regulate signaling at the cellular level may be impaired. It remains to be determined if these downstream modifiers of glucocorticoid signaling are linked to the hippocampal abnormalities reported in PTSD patients. A recent human structural MRI study, while replicating the finding of smaller hippocampal volume in chronic PTSD patients, found no differences between Gulf War veterans without PTSD and those who had successfully recovered from the disorder (Apfel et al., 2011). The authors suggest that this pattern of findings may indicate that low hippocampal volume is a state factor. However, the cross-sectional design of the study precludes any causal inference in this regard. It is also possible, as the authors mention, that measurable structural abnormalities are a trait factor predisposing individuals toward a more severe, treatment resistant form of the disorder, or that hippocampal neurogenesis is a mediating factor in successful recovery. Longitudinal imaging studies are necessary to disentangle these competing hypotheses. Remember the effects of poor hippocampal functioning Normal hippocampal function A conjunctive representation comprised of many individual elements present in the environment is encoded as a whole. In this case the individual elements comprising the environment are bound into one representation that defines a place (i.e. context) where the event occurs (Rudy et al., 2004) and a conditioned response would only occur in the presence of an environment with stimuli that matches the full representation, with no single cue sufficient to induce a conditioned response. Conjunctive representation of the context is also referred to as a cognitive map (O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978) Poor hippocampal function leads to poor cognitive mapping, generalization and shift to elemental representation (each element of the traumatic context is associated with the aversive event and can evoke a fear response even in a safe context) Ontogenesi ed estinzione Come già visto, la risposta emotiva può riapparire dopo estinzione a causa del recupero spontaneo, della ripresentazione dello SI o del rinnovo (se lo SC è presentato in un contesto differente). Nell’età giovanile l’estinzione sembra invece avere un effetto permanente che porta alla cancellazione della traccia associativa precedentemente acquisita (Kim e Richardson, 2008) Il cambiamento molecolare che porta alla mancanza di effetto duraturo dell’estinzione potrebbe essere costituito dalla maturazione della matrice extracellulare (ECM) nell’amigdala (Gogolla et al., 2009). Non è escluso un ruolo della maturazione della corteccia prefrontale, che prenderebbe, con il procedere della maturazione, il ruolo di guida nel processo di estinzione, conducendo al processo di inibizione, ma non di cancellazione, della traccia di memoria di paura condizionata. I topi link (Crl1 KO) mancano delle reti perineuronali della matrice extracellulare Fin qui 25 novembre Attivazione dell’amigdala dopo il primo giorno di estinzione Range of Zif positive cells in pseudoconditioned subjects (unpaired CS-US) Attivazione dell’amigdala dopo il primo giorno di estinzione Range of Zif positive cells in pseudoconditioned subjects (unpaired CS-US) Attivazione dell’amigdala dopo il primo giorno di estinzione Range of Zif positive cells in pseudoconditioned subjects (unpaired CS-US) La riduzione dell’attivazione è a livello dell’ingresso all’amigdala, ovvero ad LA Estinzione duratura nei topi Crtl1 KO Estinzione duratura nei topi Crtl1 KO Effetto arricchimento ambientale sull’estinzione?? Latenze Braccio Shock GRUPPO ST. SHOCK GRUP. AA SHOCK 160,00 140,00 Latenza sec. 120,00 100,00 80,00 60,00 40,00 20,00 0,00 Abituazione Test 7°giorno 14°giorno 21°giorno 28°giorno 35°giorno Tempo medio speso in freezing "braccio shock" GRUPPO STAN. SHOCK GRUP. AA SHOCK 12,00 Tempo Freezing. Sec. 10,00 8,00 6,00 4,00 2,00 0,00 Abituazione Test 7°giorno 14°giorno 21°giorno 28°giorno 35°giorno Tempo speso in freezing durante la fase di apprendimento. Gruppo Standard Gruppo Arricchito 5,00 Tempo freezing sec. 4,00 3,00 2,00 1,00 0,00 -1,00 -2,00 entrata 15 sec 30 sec. 75 sec. 90.sec Considerazioni finali sull’estinzione Combinare estinzione e riconsolidamento Science. 2009 May 15;324(5929):951-5. Epub 2009 Apr 2. Extinction-reconsolidation boundaries: key to persistent attenuation of fear memories. Monfils MH, Cowansage KK, Klann E, LeDoux JE. Extinction-Reconsolidation Boundaries: Key to Persistent Attenuation of Fear Memories The possibility that reactivated memories may be modifiable was proposed many years ago, and since then, numerous studies have demonstrated that blockade of the updating process engaged during retrieval—usually via pharmacological intervention within the reconsolidation window—prevents memory restorage and produces amnesia (loss of the specific memory that was reactivated in the presence of the drug or access to it). Thus, in the case of aversive memories, blocking reconsolidation weakens the emotional impact of a once fear-inducing stimulus by altering the molecular composition of the memory trace. This process generally requires the use of drugs that often cannot be readily administered to humans. (Monfils et al., 2009) In contrast, fear extinction—a paradigm in which the conditioned stimulus (CS) is repeatedly presented in the absence of the unconditioned stimulus (US)—leads to progressive reduction in the expression of fear, but is not permanent because extinction does not directly modify the existing memory but instead leads to the formation of a new memory that suppresses activation of the initial trace. The efficacy of this inhibition, however, is strongly contingent on spatial, sensory, and temporal variables. Specifically, the reemergence of a previously extinguished fear is known to occur, in rodents and humans alike, under three general conditions: (i) renewal, when the CS is presented outside of the extinction context; (ii) reinstatement, when the original US is given unexpectedly; (iii) spontaneous recovery, when a substantial amount of time has passed Monfils et al devised an effective, drug-free paradigm for the persistent reduction of learned fear, capitalizing on differences between reconsolidation and extinction. Given that extinction training reduces the threatening value of the CS, they reasoned that when applied within the reconsolidation window (after the memory is rendered unstable by presenting an isolated retrieval trial), extinction training would result in the storage of the new nonthreatening meaning of the CS and prevent renewal, reinstatement, and spontaneous recovery, thus resulting in a more enduring reduction in fear relative to extinction training conducted outside the reconsolidation window. Specifically, they predicted that an extinction session presented after an isolated retrieval trial would lead to a persistent revaluation of the CS as less threatening, and/or a weakening of the stored trace or access to it, and thus would prevent the return of fear in the three aforementioned tests. Six experiments were conducted. 1) Test whether their behavioral paradigm could prevent the return of fear on a spontaneous recovery test, and if so, whether the observed effect was the result of an update during reconsolidation. They specifically designed this experiment on the basis of the premise that the lability window engaged at the time of retrieval is temporary—in rat fear conditioning, it closes within 6 hours—at which time the memory is thought to be reconsolidated. They posited that if the interval between the isolated retrieval cue and extinction training was brief enough to enable the repeated unreinforced CSs to be presented within the lability window, then the new interpretation of the CS as no longer threatening should be incorporated during reconsolidation. Rats were fear-conditioned using three tone-shock pairings, and were then divided into five experimental groups. Two groups had a retrieval-extinction interval within the reconsolidation window [10 min (n = 8) and 1 hour (n = 8)] and two groups outside the reconsolidation window [6 hours (n = 8) and 24 hours (n = 8)]. In addition to these four retrieval (Ret) groups, a fifth group (No Ret) was exposed to context but did not receive a CS retrieval (n = 12). All procedures were conducted in context A (grid floor). All groups showed equivalent freezing for the last four trials of extinction Finite lability window to prevent return of fear via post-retrieval extinction. (A) Rats were fear-conditioned (Fear Cond) with three tone-shock pairings. After 24 hours, they were exposed either to an isolated cue retrieval trial (Ret) or context only (No Ret) followed by extinction training. The time interval between the retrieval trial (or context exposure, n = 12) and the extinction was either within (10 min, n = 8; 1 hour, n = 8) or outside (6 hours, n = 8; 24 hours, n = 8) the reconsolidation window. Twenty-four hours after extinction, all groups were tested for LTM, and 1 month later for spontaneous recovery. The gray shading represents context A. (B) All groups were equivalent for the last four trials of extinction and at the 24-hour LTM test. One month later, the Ret groups with an interval outside the reconsolidation window as well as the No Ret group showed increased freezing (spontaneous recovery) relative to the 24-hour test Attenuation of fear memory by presenting a single isolated retrieval trial followed by an extinction session prevents renewal. (A) Rats were fear-conditioned in context A. Twenty-four hours later, they were exposed either to an isolated cue retrieval trial (Ret, n = 8) or context only (No Ret, n = 8) in context B, followed 1 hour later by extinction training in context B. Twenty-four hours after extinction, they were tested for LTM in context B. The gray shading represents context A; the blue shading represents context B (smooth black floor and peppermint scent) (28). (B) Rats from both experimental groups froze equivalently during the LTM test (all ANOVAs, P > 0.1). When they were placed back in the acquisition context, the No Ret group (black) showed fear renewal (P = 0.012), but the Ret group (red) did not (P > 0.1), relative to their respective LTM tests Presenting a single isolated retrieval trial before an extinction session prevents reinstatement. (A) Rats were fear-conditioned. The next day, they were exposed either to an isolated cue retrieval trial (Ret, n = 8) or context only (No Ret, n = 8), followed 1 hour later by extinction training. Twenty-four hours after extinction, they received five unsignaled footshocks, and the next day were tested for reinstatement. The gray shading represents context A. (B) The No Ret and Ret groups froze equivalently to the last four CSs of extinction; however, 24 hours after the unsignaled footshocks, the No Ret group (black) showed increased freezing (reinstatement) (P < 0.05), but the Ret group (red) did not (P > 0.05). Nature 463, 49-53 (7 January 2010) Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms Schiller et al., 2010 “Research on changing fears has highlighted several techniques, most of which rely on the inhibition of the learned fear response. An inherent problem with these inhibition techniques is that the fear may return, for example with stress. Recent research on changing fears targeting the reconsolidation process overcomes this challenge to some extent. During reconsolidation, stored information is rendered labile after being retrieved, and pharmacological manipulations at this stage result in an inability to retrieve the memories at later times, suggesting that they are either erased or persistently inhibited.” Remember that the hypothesis is that reconsolidation is an adaptive update mechanism by which new information is incorporated into old memories. By introducing new information during the reconsolidation period, it may be possible to permanently change the fear memory. In the present study, Schiller et al provide evidence in humans that old fear memories can be updated with non-fearful information provided during the reconsolidation window. As a consequence, fear responses are no longer expressed. If an old fear memory could be restored while incorporating neutral or more positive information provided at the time of retrieval, it may be possible to permanently modify the fearful properties of this memory. Although this approach captures the very essence of reconsolidation, it has been surprisingly neglected in emotion research in humans and other animals. The authors designed two experiments examining whether extinction training conducted during the reconsolidation window would block the return of extinguished fear. In the first study, three groups of subjects underwent fear conditioning using a discrimination paradigm with partial reinforcement. Two coloured squares were used. One square (conditioned stimulus+, hereafter termed CS+) was paired with a mild shock to the wrist (unconditioned stimulus) on 38% of the trials, whereas the other square was never paired with shock (CS-). A day later, all three groups underwent extinction training in which the two conditioned stimuli were repeatedly presented without the unconditioned stimulus. In two groups the fear memory was reactivated before extinction using a single presentation of the CS+. One group (n = 20) received the reminder trial 10 min before extinction (within the reconsolidation window), whereas the second group (n = 23) was reminded 6 h before extinction (outside the reconsolidation window). The third group (n = 22) was not reminded of the fear memory before extinction training. Twenty-four hours later, all three groups were presented again with the conditioned stimuli without the unconditioned stimulus (re-extinction) to assess spontaneous fear recovery. The measure of fear was the skin conductance response (SCR). At each stage, the differential fear response was calculated by subtracting responses to the CS- from responses to the CS+. a, Experimental design and timeline. b, Mean differential SCRs (CS+ minus CS-) during acquisition (late phase), extinction (last trial) and reextinction (first trial) for each experimental group (10-min reminder, 6-h reminder and no reminder). The three groups showed equivalent fear acquisition and extinction. Spontaneous recovery (first trial of re-extinction versus the last trial of extinction) was found in the group that had not been reminded or that was reminded 6 h before extinction. In contrast, there was no spontaneous recovery in the group reminded 10 min before extinction. a, Experimental design and timeline. US, unconditioned stimulus. b, Mean SCRs (CSa+, CSb+ and CS-) during acquisition (late phase), extinction (last trial) and re-extinction (first trial). Subjects had equivalent levels of acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear to the two conditioned stimuli. The index of fear recovery was the first trial of re-extinction (after reinstatement) minus the last trial of extinction (before reinstatement). Fear reinstatement was found only to CSb+ (not reminded before extinction training), but not to CSa+ (reminded 10 min before extinction training). The present findings suggest a new technique to target specific fear memories and prevent the return of fear after extinction training. Using two recovery assays, the authors demonstrated that extinction conducted during the reconsolidation window of an old fear memory prevented the spontaneous recovery or the reinstatement of fear responses, an effect that was maintained a year later. Moreover, this manipulation selectively affected only the reactivated conditioned stimulus while leaving fear memory to the other non-reactivated conditioned stimulus intact. The current results also suggest that timing may have a more important role in the control of fear than previously appreciated. Standard extinction training, without previous memory reactivation, also triggers the fear memory. Given this, one might expect mere extinction training to have similar effects. That is, the first trial of extinction might serve as the reminder cue triggering the reconsolidation cascade, which is immediately followed by extinction. However, there is abundant evidence that during standard extinction training the nonreinforced presentations of the fear-eliciting cue induce new inhibitory learning, which competes for expression with the initial fear learning, resulting in the recovery of fear responses in some. Schiller et al findings indicate that the timing of extinction relative to the reactivation of the memory can capitalize on reconsolidation mechanisms. Two factors may be important determinants in this process: the timing of extinction training relative to retrieval, and/or the chunking of the conditioned stimulus presentations during extinction relative to reactivation (that is, the fact that they are massed relative to the single retrieval trial during the reconsolidation phase). Further studies are required to disentangle these possibilities. The present study proposes that invasive techniques are not necessary to reduce conditioned fear responses. Using a more natural intervention that captures the adaptive purpose of reconsolidation allows a safe and easily implemented way to prevent the return of fear. Let us discuss this Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013 Nov 25. Extinction during reconsolidation of threat memory diminishes prefrontal cortex involvement. Schiller D, Kanen JW, Ledoux JE, Monfils MH, Phelps EA. Abstract Controlling learned defensive responses through extinction does not alter the threat memory itself, but rather regulates its expression via inhibitory influence of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) over amygdala. Individual differences in amygdala-PFC circuitry function have been linked to trait anxiety and posttraumatic stress disorder. This finding suggests that exposure-based techniques may actually be least effective in those who suffer from anxiety disorders. A theoretical advantage of techniques influencing reconsolidation of threat memories is that the threat representation is altered, potentially diminishing reliance on this PFC circuitry, resulting in a more persistent reduction of defensive reactions. Schiller, Phelps and LeDoux hypothesized that timing extinction to coincide with threat memory reconsolidation would prevent the return of defensive reactions and diminish PFC involvement. Two conditioned stimuli (CS) were paired with shock and the third was not. A day later, one stimulus (reminded CS+) but not the other (nonreminded CS+) was presented 10 min before extinction to reactivate the threat memory, followed by extinction training for all CSs. The recovery of the threat memory was tested 24 h later. Extinction of the nonreminded CS+ (i.e., standard extinction) engaged the PFC, as previously shown, but extinction of the reminded CS+ (i.e., extinction during reconsolidation) did not. Moreover, only the nonreminded CS+ memory recovered on day 3. These results suggest that extinction during reconsolidation prevents the return of defensive reactions and diminishes PFC involvement. Reducing the necessity of the PFC-amygdala circuitry to control defensive reactions may help overcome a primary obstacle in the longterm efficacy of current treatments for anxiety disorders Cognitive regulation of emotions (testable in humans) Ochsner et al. (2002) had participants view emotionally negative pictures (e.g., woman crying on steps of church) and either “attend” (focus on your natural feelings) or “reappraise” (reinterpret the picture in a less negative context; e.g., the woman is crying at a wedding) the stimulus. Behaviorally, the reappraisal technique was successful in decreasing the negative affect as measured through subjective ratings. An examination of changes in the BOLD signal contrasting the reappraisal and attend conditions revealed enhanced BOLD signal in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and decreased BOLD signal in the amygdala. Several other neuroimaging studies have reported similar modulation of the amygdala response with emotion regulation, along with involvement of regions of the dlPFC as well as anterior cingulate and other mPFC regions (Ochsner and Gross, 2005). Cognitive regulation of emotions Using an emotion regulation strategy, Phelps examine the neural mechanisms of regulating conditioned fear using fMRI and compare the resulting activation pattern with that observed during classic extinction. The results suggest that the lateral PFC regions engaged by cognitive emotion regulation strategies may influence the amygdala, diminishing fear through similar vmPFC connections that are thought to inhibit the amygdala during extinction. These findings further suggest that humans may have developed complex cognition that can aid in regulating emotional responses while utilizing phylogenetically shared mechanisms of extinction. The dlPFC regions often observed in studies of emotion regulation have also been implicated in other higher cognitive functions, such as executive processing and working memory, or the active maintenance of on-line information (Smith and Jonides, 1999). The finding that the cognitive regulation of responses to emotional scenes alters functional activity in the dlPFC and amygdala could suggests that these brain regions are functionally interconnected. However, anatomical connectivity studies have failed to find direct connections between the amygdala and the dlPFC (Barbas, 2000; McDonald et al., 1996; Stefanacci and Amaral, 2002). Phelps proposes that the dlPFC regions linked with the cognitive regulation of emotion may take advantage of the evolutionarily shared mechanisms of extinction to diminish emotional responses. Although these dlPFC regions do not have direct projections to the amygdala, they do project to ventral and medial PFC regions that are more directly connected with the amygdala (Amaral, 2002; Groenewegen et al., 1997; McDonald et al., 1996). It is possible, therefore, that cognitive emotion regulation strategies, which recruit dlPFC regions, may diminish emotional response through connections with vmPFC, which has been shown to inhibit the amygdala in fear extinction (Milad and Quirk, 2002). Meno deattivata è la mPFC, più inibita è l’amigdala. La mPFC è meno deattivata nel caso della regolazione In other words, humans may have developed complex cognition that can aid in the regulation of emotional responses, but these processes utilize phylogenetically older mechanisms of extinction for diminishing fears that are no longer adaptive.