Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
JOURNAL OF CRUSTACEAN BIOLOGY, 22(2): 439–448, 2002 DIFFERENCES IN MEMORY CAPABILITIES IN INVASIVE AND NATIVE CRAYFISH Brian A. Hazlett, Patrizia Acquistapace, and Francesca Gherardi (BAH, correspondence) Department of Biology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109, U.S.A. ([email protected]); (PA, FG) Dipartimento di Biologia Animale e Genetica ‘Leo Pardi’, Universitá di Firenze, Via Romana 17, 50125, Firenze, Italy A B S T R A C T The memory capabilities of individuals of four species of crayfish (two invasive species and two native species) were tested in the laboratory. Individuals of the invasive Orconectes rusticus and the native O. virilis were tested in Michigan, and the invasive Procambarus clarkii and the native Austropatmobius pallipes were tested in Italy. Following pairing of conspecific alarm odour and a novel odour (goldfish odour), individuals were tested one day, one week, and either three weeks later (Italy) or two and four weeks later (Michigan) for inhibition of feeding responses by goldfish odour. In all four species, exposure of animals for just two hours was sufficient to establish an association between the novel odour and elevated predation risk. In both species pairs, individuals of the invasive species showed evidence of retention of the learned association longer than did individuals of the native species. The results are consistent with the general hypothesis that invasive species have a greater capacity for behavioural plasticity. Avoiding predation is of obvious importance for individuals of all species. While some animals respond to cues associated with elevated predation risk without past experience with those cues (conspecific alarm odour, Hazlett and Schoolmaster, 1998), most studies have indicated that learning is involved in the recognition of danger cues. Experience is necessary for recognition of predator odour in the minnow Pimephales promelas (see Mathias and Smith, 1993a) as well as for other species of fish (Magurran, 1989; Chivers et al., 1995). Chivers et al. (1996) demonstrated that a similar experiential requirement held for the damselfly, Enallagma species. Hazlett and Schoolmaster (1998) showed that at least some species of crayfish show predation avoidance responses to the introduction of the odour of predators only after such odours have been paired with alarm odours. In some cases, naïve individuals can learn to respond to predator cues from experienced individuals (Mathias et al., 1996). Aggressiveness may be associated with invasive species (Capelli and Munjal, 1982; Hill and Lodge, 1999); however, few studies have directly compared other aspects of the behaviour of invasive species and the native species they have displaced. In the case of species introduced to a new habitat, which is likely to contain predators not encountered in their former range, efficient learning about cues associated with elevated predation risk would seem to be especially critical. It is reasonable to suggest that successful invaders will exhibit a higher degree of plasticity in a number of behaviours (Hazlett, 2000a), including learning about predator cues and thus reducing predation risk (Mathias and Smith, 1993b). We tested these general predictions in two pairs of crayfish: the invasive Orconectes rusticus (Girard, 1852), an Appalachian drainage species, and the native species it has displaced in many parts of the midwest U.S.A. and Canada, Orconectes virilis (Hagen, 1870) (Garvey et al., 1994; Hill and Lodge, 1999) and the southern U.S.A. invasive Procambarus clarkii (Girard, 1852) and a native Italian species Austropotamobius pallipes (Lerebboullet, 1858) which is threatened in southern Europe by P. clarkii (Gherardi et al., 1999). In particular, we tested the predictions that species that are successful invaders should learn associations faster and remember those learned associations longer than native species that are not known to be successful invaders. MATERIALS AND METHODS The Orconectes species were studied in the laboratory in Ann Arbor, Michican, U.S.A., in September and 439 440 JOURNAL OF CRUSTACEAN BIOLOGY, VOL. 22, NO. 2, 2002 October, 2000. The Procambarus clarkii and Austropotamobius pallipes were studied in the laboratory in Florence, Italy, in June and July, 2000. The same methodology was used for both species pairs. Individuals of O. rusticus were collected from Burt Lake, Michigan, while the O. virilis were collected from the ponds at the DNR Fisheries Station in Saline, Michigan. The P. clarkii were collected from Padule di Fucecchio, a freshwater swamp 60 km from Florence, while the A. pallipes were collected from Fosso di Farfereta, a third-order stream 40 km from Florence. All individuals used were adults and approximately equal numbers of both sexes were used. All crayfish were fed pieces of freeze-thawed cod fish prior to the initiation of training and testing. Following the method of Chivers and Smith (1994), the crayfish were trained with a novel odour, the odour of the common goldfish, Carassius auratus. None of the crayfish would have encountered this species in the field, and because C. auratus is an herbivore it is not a potential predator. Control animals were tested with goldfish odour (see below) without any prior exposure to that odour. Test animals were trained with either a short experience or a long experience with a combination of conspecific alarm odour and goldfish odour. The short experience consisted of placing two medium-size (30-mm standard length) goldfish in an opaque plastic container that had many holes drilled in the side into a communal aquarium containing the crayfish for two hours. At the beginning of the 2-h period, and after 1 h, conspecific alarm odour was slowly poured into the water. Conspecific alarm odour was prepared by crushing a medium-size (20–25-mm cephalothorax length) conspecific individual in 150 ml of water and filtering with coarse filter paper. In the case of the long-experience test individuals, the goldfish were left in the communal aquarium with the crayfish for 24 h, and an additional application of alarm odour was added 18 h after the first two applications. Immediately after the training period, crayfish were placed in individual containers. These containers were visually isolated from each other and contained 15 litres of continually aerated well water and half of a clay pot to serve as a shelter. Crayfish were tested after 24-h acclimation in the observation aquaria. Behaviours were observed and recorded on a personal computer with an event program. As in Hazlett (1999), the parameters recorded were: (a) time in shelter, (b) time spent in locomotion by movement of the ambulatory legs, (c) time spent executing cleaning movements, (d) time spent executing feeding movements of the chelipeds and chelate walking legs, and (e) time in one of three postures: raised, intermediate, or lowered. These latter postures were characterised in the following ways: in the raised posture the body was elevated off the substrate, the chelipeds were held off the substrate parallel to the substrate or higher, and the abdomen and telson were extended; in the intermediate posture, the body was held just off the substrate, the tips of the chelipeds were lightly touching the substrate and the telson was perpendicular to the substrate; in the lowered posture, the body was in contact with the substrate, the chelipeds were drawn in towards the body, and the telson curled under the abdomen. Individuals could move both while in and out of the shelter; thus, those categories of behaviour are independent measures. Because an animal had to be in one of the three postures, the times in those postures are not independent, and only times spent in the raised and lowered postures were analysed. Each crayfish was observed during a two-minute control period following the introduction of 5 ml of well water, then a 2-min period following the introduction of 5 ml of food odour, and immediately following that, a 2-min period following the introduction of 5 ml of goldfish odour. All solutions were introduced via a syringe. Earlier studies (Hazlett, 1999; Bouwma and Hazlett, 2001) suggest that the use of two, potentially conflicting stimuli yields more sensitive measures of changes in behaviour and reception of stimuli than presentation of a single stimulus. Food odour was generated by macerating 200 g of freeze-thawed cod in 150 ml of well water. Goldfish odour was generated by placing two medium-size goldfish in two litres of well water for 24 h prior to use in testing. After the initial testing, crayfish were returned to communal tanks and the individual observation tanks were cleaned and refilled with well water. The Orconectes species were tested again 1, 2, and 4 weeks after the initial tests, while P. clarkii and A. pallipes were tested again 1 and 3 weeks after initial testing. The scheduling of other observations dictated the different patterns in Michigan and Italy. Ten individuals of each species and experience treatment (control, short experience, long experience) were tested. Mortality resulted in a sample size of 8 or 9 in some of the later tests for several species × treatment categories. Obviously the repeated testing of the same individuals with just goldfish odour will affect the rate of memory decay (Dukas, 1998). However, all individuals of all species were treated in the same way, and the comparisons of interest were between the species of crayfish. For each crayfish, the number of seconds spent in the various behaviours during the three test conditions (control, food odour, goldfish odour) on a given test day were extracted from the event recorder files. An ANOVA was used to test for a treatment effect on each behaviour, and only for those behaviours with a significant treatment effect (P < 0.05) the Tukey test was used for pairwise comparisons of the three treatments. These analyses were conducted for each species, for both experience durations, for each date. Where the number of seconds spent in behaviours was different for food-odour periods and goldfish-odour periods, this was taken as evidence of an effect of the goldfish odour on food-related responses. That is, when the addition of goldfish odour significantly depressed the levels of food-related responses, we judged that the crayfish had treated goldfish odour as a cue related to potential predation risk. Similar patterns of the depression of feeding responses by detection of the odour of real predators have been reported for other species of crayfish (Blake and Hart, 1993; Hazlett and Schoolmaster, 1998; Hazlett, 1999; Bouwma and Hazlett, 2001). RESULTS In all species tested, the behaviours that differed most consistently among control and the two test periods (food alone, goldfish + food odour) were feeding, locomotion, and time in the raised posture. The other behaviours or categories (cleaning, time in shelter, and time in the lowered posture) did not differ among odour treatments for the vast majority of cases and were judged not to be good measures of the response to test stimuli. Thus, HAZLETT ET AL.: MEMORY CAPABILITIES IN CRAYFISH 441 these behavioural measures were not used in looking for an effect of goldfish odour. Orconectes rusticus and Orconectes virilis When food odour was introduced, the control animals in both species showed a significant increase in at least some of the behaviours related to feeding (feeding, locomotion, and raised posture) compared to control periods. When goldfish odour was introduced there was no change in behaviour and the animals continued to show feeding behaviours at the same level as when food odour alone was introduced (Fig. 1). Thus, animals that had had no experience with goldfish odour did not change their behaviour when that odour was introduced. Tests One Day After Training.—For both species and both experience durations, when tested one day after training, crayfish responded to the introduction of goldfish odour by a significant reduction in the number of seconds spent in at least some food-related activities (Fig. 2; Tables 1, 2). In both species and for both experience treatments, feeding was reduced to control levels (Fig. 2). In the case of O. rusticus with short experience, this was true also for raised posture. For longexperience O. rusticus, three behaviours (feeding, locomotion, and raised posture) were significantly affected and the levels reduced to control-period durations (Table 1). Short-experience O. virilis showed significant reduction in feeding, locomotion, and raised posture compared to food-alone treatments, and there was no difference between goldfish odour added and control periods. Longexperience O. virilis showed a significant reduction only in feeding (Table 2). Tests One Week After Training.—When tested one week after training, both the shortexperience and long-experience individuals of the invasive species, O. rusticus, showed significant reductions in the time spent feeding (Fig. 2), locomoting, and in the raised posture when goldfish odour was introduced (Table 1). The short-experience individuals of the native species, O. virilis, appeared to have forgotten after one week as there were no significant reductions in any behaviours following introduction of goldfish odour (Table 2), and these animals were not tested further. For the long-experience O. virilis both feeding Fig. 1. Number of seconds (mean + SE) spent by control animals of (A) Orconectes rusticus and (B) Orconectes virilis in three activities under different odour treatment conditions. For each behaviour, different letters indicate significant differences between odour treatments by Tukey tests (P < 0.05). (Fig. 2) and locomotion showed marginal differences between food odour alone and goldfish odour conditions, but there was clearly no difference between goldfish odour and control conditions. Tests Two Weeks After Training.—For the short-experience O. rusticus, there were significant reductions for feeding (Fig. 2) and locomotion (Table 1), whereas for the longexperience O. rusticus the reductions following introduction of goldfish odour were significant for feeding (Fig. 2), locomotion, and raised posture (Table 1). 442 JOURNAL OF CRUSTACEAN BIOLOGY, VOL. 22, NO. 2, 2002 Fig. 2. Number of seconds (mean + SE) spent in feeding behaviours for (A) Orconectes rusticus, short experience, (B) O. rusticus, long experience, (C) Orconectes virilis, short experience, and (D) O. virilis, long experience. Data are presented for the three odour conditions at different times after experiencing the pairing of goldfish odour and conspecific alarm odour for either two hours (short-experience animals) or 24 hours (long-experience animals). Different letters indicate significant differences between odour treatments during a given time since training (Tukey tests, P < 0.05). In the case of the long-experience O. virilis there were no changes in feeding-related behaviours when goldfish odour was introduced (Table 2), and thus the association between alarm odour and goldfish odour was no longer detectable. These crayfish were not tested further. in feeding activities (Fig. 2) or any other behaviours following the introduction of goldfish odour (Table 1). This pattern was observed also for the long-experience individuals of O. rusticus (Table 1). Thus, all of the crayfish had apparently forgotten the learned association by four weeks after training. Tests Four Weeks After Training.—When tested four weeks after the initial training, the individuals of O. rusticus that had had the short experience did not show any change Procambarus clarkii and Austropotamobius pallipes For the control animals that had not had prior exposure to goldfish odour, individuals HAZLETT ET AL.: MEMORY CAPABILITIES IN CRAYFISH 443 Table 1. Results from Tukey tests (probability values) comparing the time spent in 3 behaviours (feeding, locomotion, raised posture) in response to pairs of odour treatments (control vs. food odour, food odour vs. goldfish odour, and control vs. goldfish odour) by individuals of Orconectes rusticus. Results are presented separately for animals with short experience (2 h of pairing of goldfish odour and alarm odour) and long experience (24 h of pairing of goldfish odour and alarm odour). Results from Tukey tests are reported only for behaviours in which the overall ANOVA showed a significant difference (P < 0.05). Short-experience O. rusticus 1 day after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture 1 week after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture 2 weeks after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture 4 weeks after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture Long-experience O. rusticus Control-Food Food-Goldfish Control-Goldfish Control-Food 0.001 – 0.014 0.001 – 0.022 0.896 – 0.979 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.003 0.292 0.650 0.827 0.001 0.010 0.012 0.002 0.047 0.024 0.825 0.773 0.945 0.001 0.005 0.009 0.001 0.056 0.074 0.530 0.578 0.619 0.001 0.007 – 0.001 0.014 – 0.975 0.953 – 0.001 0.021 0.013 0.001 0.058 0.044 0.847 0.892 0.847 0.008 – – 0.861 – – 0.027 – – 0.015 0.025 0.08 0.984 0.997 0.893 0.010 0.025 0.031 of both species showed strong increases in feeding, locomotion, and time in the raised posture when food odours were introduced, and there were no changes in those behaviours when goldfish odour was introduced (Fig. 3). The crayfish continued to show high levels of feeding-related activities during the period following goldfish-odour introduction, indicating that naïve animals did not treat goldfish odour as a cue related to possible predation risk. Food-Goldfish Control-Goldfish Tests One Day After Training.—Individuals of Procambarus clarkii showed significant reductions of all three feeding-related behaviours when goldfish odour was introduced for the long-experience animals (Fig. 4; Table 3). For locomotion and raised posture the time spent when goldfish odour was introduced was significantly lower than when food odours alone were present but also significantly higher than during control periods. For the short-experience P. clarkii, the reductions Table 2. Results from Tukey tests (probability values) comparing the time spent in 3 behaviours (feeding, locomotion, raised posture) in response to pairs of odour treatments (control vs. food odour, food odour vs. goldfish odour, and control vs. goldfish odour) by individuals of Orconectes virilis. Results are presented separately for animals with short experience (2 h of pairing of goldfish odour and alarm odour) and long experience (24 h of pairing of goldfish odour and alarm odour). Results from Tukey tests are reported only for behaviours in which the overall ANOVA showed a significant difference (P < 0.05). Short-experience O. virilis 1 day after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture 1 week after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture 2 weeks after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture a experiment not conducted Long-experience O. virilis Control-Food Food-Goldfish Control-Goldfish Control-Food Food-Goldfish Control-Goldfish 0.001 0.001 0.007 0.001 0.002 0.012 0.934 0.916 0.966 0.002 – – 0.003 – – 0.975 – – 0.003 0.023 0.032 0.295 0.575 0.674 0.075 0.174 0.169 0.005 0.009 – 0.077 0.079 – 0.477 0.600 – a a a a a a a a a 0.028 0.023 – 0.964 0.629 – 0.048 0.150 – 444 JOURNAL OF CRUSTACEAN BIOLOGY, VOL. 22, NO. 2, 2002 after the initial training, showed significant reductions in feeding (Fig. 4) and time in raised posture when goldfish odour was introduced (Table 3). For the long-experience P. clarkii, only feeding behaviours (Fig. 4) were significantly reduced and similar to control period levels when goldfish odours were introduced (Table 3). The short-experience and long-experience A. pallipes, when tested one week after initial training, showed significant reduction of feeding behaviours (Fig. 4), and time in the raised posture when goldfish odours were introduced (Table 4). For short-experience A. pallipes, the time spent in locomotion was also significantly reduced (Table 4). Fig. 3. Number of seconds (mean + SE) spent by control animals of (A) Procambarus clarkii and (B) Austropotamobius pallipes in three activities under different odour treatment conditions. For each behaviour, different letters indicate significant differences between odour treatments by Tukey tests (P < 0.05). in feeding movements (Fig. 4) and time in raised posture were significantly lower than during food-only periods, but locomotion time was not altered (Table 3). Individuals of Austropotamobius pallipes showed significant reductions in feeding (Fig. 4), locomotion, and raised posture for both the short-experience and long-experience animals (Table 4). Individuals of both species and both experience treatments responded to the goldfish odour as if it represented an elevated predation risk. Tests One Week After Training.—The shortexperience P. clarkii, when tested one week Tests Three Weeks After Training.—When tested three weeks after training, the shortexperience P. clarkii did not show significant treatment effects for any of the three behaviours based on the Tukey test comparisons (Table 3). In the case of the long-experience P. clarkii, individuals showed a very significant reduction in feeding activity (Fig. 4) upon goldfish introduction, and the decrease in time in raised posture was almost significant (P = 0.075) (Table 3). For the short-experience A. pallipes, only feeding activities (Fig. 4) showed an almost significant overall treatment effect (ANOVA, P = 0.070), but there was clearly no difference between food alone and goldfish odour periods (Fig. 4C). The long-experience A. pallipes tested three weeks after training showed no reduction in activities upon goldfish odour introduction (Table 4). Individuals of A. pallipes of both experience treatments had apparently not retained memory of the learned association by three weeks after training. DISCUSSION The lack of change in the behaviour of control animals when goldfish odour was introduced indicates that individuals of none of these crayfish species treat goldfish odour as a stimulus related to predation risk (or anything else). The absence of a response is reasonable given the lack of overlap in distribution, currently and historically, and the herbivorous nature of goldfish. Following the experience of goldfish odour and conspecific alarm odour at the same time, individuals of all four species showed changes in behaviour fol- HAZLETT ET AL.: MEMORY CAPABILITIES IN CRAYFISH 445 Fig. 4. Number of seconds (mean + SE) spent in feeding behaviours by individuals of (A) Procambarus clarkii, short experience, (B) P. clarkii, long experience, (C) Austropotamobius pallipes, short experience and (D) A. pallipes, long experience. Data are presented for the three odour conditions at different times after experiencing the pairing of goldfish odour and conspecific alarm odour for either two hours (short-experience animals) or 24 hours (long-experience animals). Different letters indicate significant differences between odour treatments during a given time since training (Tukey tests, P < 0.05). lowing the introduction of goldfish odour alone. The direction of the changes (reduced levels of feeding, locomotion, and time in the raised posture) is consistent with the crayfish treating goldfish odour as a cue associated with increased predation risk. This pattern of association is very similar to the results reported for several species of fish (Magurran, 1989; Mathias and Smith, 1993a; Chivers et al., 1995) and for damselflies (Chivers et al., 1996). A brief exposure to the two odour sources together (two hours) was sufficient to form an association. The learned association shown by these crayfish should afford some protection from predation in the field, based upon the demonstration of a reduction in predation risk by changes in behaviour following the detection of cues associated with elevated predation risk in fish (Mathias and Smith, 1993b). In both pairs of species, the invasive species showed evidence of memory of the 446 JOURNAL OF CRUSTACEAN BIOLOGY, VOL. 22, NO. 2, 2002 Table 3. Results from Tukey tests (probability values) comparing the time spent in 3 behaviours (feeding, locomotion, raised posture) in response to pairs of odour treatments (control vs. food odour, food odour vs. goldfish odour, and control vs. goldfish odour) by individuals of Procambarus clarkii. Results are presented separately for individuals with short experience (2 h of pairing of goldfish odour and alarm odour) and long experience (24 h of pairing of goldfish odour and alarm odour). Results from Tukey tests are reported only for behaviours in which the overall ANOVA showed a significant difference (P < 0.05). Short-experience P. clarkii 1 day after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture 1 week after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture 3 weeks after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture Long-experience P. clarkii Control-Food Food-Goldfish Control-Goldfish Control-Food 0.001 0.005 0.001 0.001 0.571 0.028 0.437 0.056 0.155 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.042 0.008 0.398 0.001 0.004 0.001 0.008 0.001 0.001 0.411 0.001 0.876 0.128 0.936 0.001 0.002 0.016 0.015 0.994 0.998 0.245 0.003 0.014 0.142 – – 0.517 – – 0.671 – – 0.001 0.001 0.020 0.001 0.400 0.075 0.002 0.001 0.821 learned association longer than the native species. Orconectes rusticus showed changes in food-related behaviour patterns for at least two weeks after training for both the shortexperience and long-experience animals. In contrast, the short-experience individuals of O. virilis showed no indication of a learned association even one week after training, and the long-experience O. virilis apparently had forgotten by two weeks after training. Individuals of Procambarus clarkii had forgotten the learned association between one and three weeks after training in the case of the shortexperience animals, but the long-experience animals still treated goldfish odour as a cue associated with elevated predation risk three weeks after training. Individuals of the native Austropotamobius pallipes, whether trained for the short or longer period of training, apparently had forgotten by three weeks. There was an indication in some of the species that the long-experience animals retained the learned association longer than the short-experience animals. Short-experience O. virilis forgot by one week after training, whereas the long-experience animals of that species did not appear to forget until two weeks. The short-experience P. clarkii forgot between one and three weeks post-training, whereas the long-experience animals still remembered after three weeks. The other two species did not show a difference between length of experience treatments. Longer memory with more training is of course the general rule in animals (Dukas 1998). Food-Goldfish Control-Goldfish In all four species of crayfish, individuals exposed to the training situation for just two hours showed evidence of memory of the learned association when tested one day later. The lack of differences among species means that the hypothesis of invasive species learning faster was not supported by these data. In many cases, crayfish almost completely shut down food-related responses upon detection of the learned predation-related cue. This is in contrast to the results reported earlier (Hazlett, 1999, 2000b) that when presented with conflicting food and predationrisk cues, crayfish showed intermediate levels of food-related activities but did not cease feeding completely. That is, while a number of other crustaceans show a hierarchical organisation in which one cue almost completely dominated the other (Hazlett, 1999, 2000b), crayfish showed a level of activity intermediate to that shown to just one cue or the other. In the case of all four species used in this study, very recent exposure to a cue (one day after training) apparently was strong enough to result in a hierarchical type of integration of inputs, at least temporarily. Orconectes virilis, one week after training, showed more of an intermediate pattern of response as was the case for O. virilis tested with the odour of a natural predator (Hazlett, 1999). Long-experience P. clarkii also showed an intermediate pattern of activity by three weeks after training. These results indicating longer memory of learned association in invasive species of HAZLETT ET AL.: MEMORY CAPABILITIES IN CRAYFISH 447 Table 4. Results from Tukey tests (probability values) comparing the time spent in 3 behaviours (feeding, locomotion, raised posture) in response to pairs of odour treatments (control vs. food odour, food odour vs. goldfish odour, and control vs. goldfish odour) by individuals of Austropotamobius pallipes. Results are presented separately for individuals with short experience (2 h of pairing of goldfish odour and alarm odour) and long experience (24 h of pairing of goldfish odour and alarm odour). Results from Tukey tests are reported only for behaviours in which the overall ANOVA showed a significant difference (P < 0.05). Short-experience A. pallipes 1 day after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture 1 week after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture 3 weeks after training Feeding Locomotion Raised posture Long-experience A. pallipes Control-Food Food-Goldfish Control-Goldfish Control-Food 0.001 0.001 0.002 0.001 0.001 0.041 0.872 0.216 0.423 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.190 0.011 0.008 0.001 0.001 0.003 0.001 0.002 0.002 0.500 0.175 0.968 0.001 0.001 0.003 0.001 0.163 0.027 0.211 0.035 0.600 – – – – – – – – – 0.010 – – 1.000 – – 0.010 – – crayfish as compared to native species are consistent with other studies of behavioural plasticity in these categories of animals. Hazlett (2000a) reported that individuals of the invasive Orconectes rusticus responded more strongly to heterospecific alarm odours than did native species of Orconectes which are being displaced by O. rusticus. Similar results have been obtained for Procambarus clarkii and Austropotamobius pallipes (Hazlett et al., unpublished). Future tests of the level of behavioural plasticity of species that do well in new habitats compared to displaced native species are needed to examine the different ways animals use information about their environments. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We acknowledge support from a NATO collaborative grant (97-6229). We thank Professor Guido Chelazzi for his hospitality, Aloyzas Burba for his assistance, and Catherine Bach for comments on the manuscript. LITERATURE CITED Blake, M. A., and P. J. B. Hart. 1993. The behavioural responses of juvenile signal crayfish Pacifastacus leniusculus to stimuli from perch and eels.—Freshwater Biology 19: 89–97. Bouwma, P., and B. A. Hazlett. 2001. Integration of multiple predator cues by the crayfish Orconectes propinquus.—Animal Behaviour 61: 771–776. Capelli, G. M., and B. L. Munjal. 1982. Aggressive interactions and resource competition in relation to species displacement among crayfish of the genus Orconectes.—Journal of Crustacean Biology 2: 486–492. Chivers, D. P., G. E. Brown, and R. J. F. Smith. 1995. Acquired recognition of chemical stimuli from pike, Esox lucius, by brook stickleback, Culaea inconstans (Osteichthyes, Gasterosteidae).—Ethology 99: 234–242. Food-Goldfish Control-Goldfish ———, and R. J. F. Smith. 1994. Fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas, acquire predator recognition when alarm substance is associated with the sight of unfamiliar fish.—Animal Behaviour 48: 597–605. ———, B. D. Wisenden, and R. J. F. Smith. 1996. Damselfly larvae learn to recognize predators from chemical cues in the predator’s diet.—Animal Behaviour 52: 315–320. Dukas, R. 1998. Constraints on information processing and their effects on behavior. Pp. 89–127 in R. Dukas, ed. Cognitive Ecology. Chicago Univ. Press, Chicago. Garvey, J. E., R. A. Stein, and H. M. Thomas. 1994. Assessing how fish predation and interspecific prey competition influence a crayfish assemblage.—Ecology 75: 532–547. Gherardi, F., G. N. Baldaccini, P. Ercolini, S. Barbaresi, G. DeLuise, D. Mazzoni, and M. Mori. 1999. The situation in Italy. Pp. 107–28 in F. Gherardi and D. M. Holdich, eds. Crayfish in Europe as Alien Species. How to Make the Best of a Bad Situation? A. A. Balkema, Rotterdam. Hazlett, B. A. 1999. Responses to multiple chemical cues by the crayfish Orconectes virilis.—Behaviour 136: 161–177. ———. 2000a. Information use by an invading species: do invaders respond more to alarm odors than native species?—Biological Invasions 2: 289–294. ———. 2000b. Responses to single and multiple sources of chemical cues by New Zealand crustaceans.—Journal of Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 34: 1–20. ———, and D. R. Schoolmaster. 1998. Responses of cambarid crayfish to predator odor.—Journal of Chemical Ecology 24: 1757–1770. Hill, A. M., and D. M. Lodge. 1999. Replacement of resident crayfish by an exotic crayfish: the roles of competition and predation.—Ecological Applications 9: 678–690. Magurran, A. E. 1989. Acquired recognition of predator odour in the European minnow (Phoxinus phoxinus).—Ethology 82: 216–223. Mathias, A., D. P. Chivers, and R. J. F. Smith. 1996. Cultural transmission of predator recognition in fishes: in- 448 JOURNAL OF CRUSTACEAN BIOLOGY, VOL. 22, NO. 2, 2002 traspecific and interspecific learning.—Animal Behaviour 51: 18–201. ———, and R. J. F. Smith. 1993a. Fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas, learn to recognize northern pike, Esox lucius, as predators on the basis of chemical stimuli from minnows in the pike’s diet.—Animal Behaviour 46: 645–656. ———, and ———. 1993b. Chemical alarm signals increase the survival time of fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) during encounters with northern pike (Esox lucius).—Behavioral Ecology 4: 260–265. RECEIVED: 15 March 2001. ACCEPTED: 4 September 2001.