Download Shining Light on Depression

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts
no text concepts found
Transcript
PERSPECTIVES
NEUROSCIENCE
Real-time, high-speed optical imaging is a
promising approach for elucidating networks of
brain activity associated with depression.
Shining Light on Depression
Thomas R. Insel
CREDIT: P. HUEY/SCIENCE
J
The author is at the National Institute for Mental Health,
Room 8235, 6001 Executive Boulevard, Bethesda, MD
20892, USA. E-mail: [email protected]
HUMAN BRAIN
Subgenual prefrontal cortex
Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on August 10, 2007
ust as research during the Decade of the
Brain (1990–2000) forged the bridge
between the mind and the brain,
research in the current decade is helping us
to understand mental illnesses as brain disorders. As a result, the distinction between
disorders of neurology (e.g., Parkinson’s and
Alzheimer’s diseases) and disorders of psychiatry (e.g., schizophrenia and depression)
may turn out to be increasingly subtle. That
is, the former may result from focal lesions
in the brain, whereas the latter arise from
abnormal activity in specific brain circuits
in the absence of a detectable lesion. As we
become more adept at detecting lesions that
lead to abnormal function, it is even possible
that the distinction between neurological
and psychiatric disorders will vanish, leading to a combined discipline of clinical
neuroscience (1).
But before we can understand depression
as a brain disorder, we need information on
the specific neuronal circuits that contribute
to the hopeless despair that forms the core of
this illness. Neuroimaging studies of people
with depression might be helpful for identifying brain regions of interest, but the temporal
and spatial resolution of current functional
magnetic resonance imaging and positron
emission tomography may not capture the
real-time dynamics of brain function that are
most relevant to mood and cognition. In a new
approach, Airan et al. report on page 819 of
this issue the use of optical imaging to capture
cellular activity at millisecond resolution in
brain slices (2). Their study, which uses
rodents with some of the behavioral features
of depression, does not define the neurobiology of depression in humans, but it demonstrates how optical imaging—in this case,
using voltage-sensitive dyes—can identify
changes in brain activity, enabling correlations between real-time cellular activity and
changing affective state.
The findings of Airan et al. are consistent
with other results that implicate the hippocampus in rodent studies of depression.
Chronic or intense stressors, such as social
defeat, result in behaviors that resemble
human depression, and these stressors have
In search of the core of depression. Imaging brain activity in
human patients suffering from depression or in animal models of
the disease will help to link brain activity in regions such as the
hippocampus and prefrontal cortex to behavioral disorders (10, 11).
been reported to reduce hippocampal neurogenesis (3). They also down-regulate the hippocampal expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (4), a molecule that promotes
neuron survival, proliferation, and differentiation. Clinically effective antidepressants increase hippocampal neurogenesis (5), and
blocking neurogenesis during treatment prevents the antidepressant effect in rodents (6).
What about the hippocampus and human
depression? Major depressive disorder is
associated with cognitive deficits and dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
(HPA) axis, part of the neuroendocrine system that controls the stress response. Because
the hippocampus is involved in both forming
new memories and regulating the HPA axis,
one might expect a link between depression
and the hippocampus. Indeed, some human
neuroimaging studies have reported a subtle
reduction in the size of the hippocampus in
patients with depression (7), and postmortem
studies have reported alterations in hippocampal gene expression (8). But the evidence
thus far is unconvincing. Humans with hippocampal lesions have memory deficits but not
mood disorders (9). And none of the imaging
or postmortem findings have been shown
to be specific to the hippocampus or to
major depressive disorder. Although the
absence of evidence is hardly evidence of
absence, most recent clinical studies of the
neurobiology of depression have been following a different lead.
Neuroimaging studies of humans with
major depressive disorder have largely
pointed to prefrontal sites, especially implicating an area in the midline subgenual ante-
www.sciencemag.org
SCIENCE
VOL 317
Published by AAAS
rior cingulate cortex, often denoted as area 25
(see the figure) (10, 11). Not only does this
region appear abnormal on structural and
functional scans (10, 11), but also it is enriched
with the serotonin transporter, a target for
many antidepressant drugs. Individuals inheriting a risk allele within the promoter of the
serotonin transporter gene have reduced volume of area 25 and reduced functional coupling of this region to the amygdala, a subcortical region implicated in the regulation of emotion (12). An initial study of treatment-resistant
depressed patients reports that deep brain stimulation adjacent to area 25 relieves the symptoms of major depressive disorder (13).
How do we resolve the differences between rodent studies that implicate the hippocampus and human studies that implicate
the midline prefrontal cortex? Of course, the
discrepancies might be attributed to neuroanatomical differences between rodent and
human brains. Rodents have at most a primitive subgenual anterior cingulate cortex,
whereas this region in the primate brain shows
extensive connections with subcortical and
cortical targets (14). But other fundamental
issues should be kept in mind when jumping
from studies of rodent behavior to human psychopathology. Human psychiatric disorders
are complicated amalgams of affective, cognitive, and behavioral abnormalities. We might
model aspects of one of these dimensions,
such as helplessness or memory loss, in rodents;
but we are then studying an aspect of the disorder, not the disorder itself.
Major depressive disorder, the result of an
unfortunate convergence of genetic and environmental factors, is certainly more than the
10 AUGUST 2007
757
PERSPECTIVES
dynamics in a defined circuit through the use
of a technique with combined spatial and temporal resolution that we have not even begun
to consider for human studies. We are not able
to apply voltage-sensitive dye imaging to people with major depressive disorder, but studies
in model animals that help us to link behavior
to real-time circuit information will be the
foundation for understanding depression as a
brain disorder.
References
1. T. R. Insel, R. Quirion, J. Am. Med. Assoc. 294, 2221 (2005).
2. R. D. Airan et al., Science 317, 819 (2007); published
online 5 July 2007 (10.11126/science/1144400).
EVOLUTION
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
R. M. Sapolsky, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 925 (2000).
N. Tsankova et al., Nat. Neurosci. 9, 519 (2006).
R. S. Duman, Biol. Psychiatry 56, 140 (2004).
L. Santarelli et al., Science 301, 805 (2003).
A. Neumeister et al., Biol. Psychiatry 57, 935 (2005).
A. L. Lopez-Figueroa et al., Biol. Psychiatry 55, 225
(2004).
L. R. Squire, C. E. L. Stark, R. E. Clark, Annu. Rev.
Neurosci. 27, 279 (2004).
W. C. Drevets et al., Nature 386, 824 (1997).
H. S. Mayberg et al., Neuroreport 8, 1057 (1997).
L. Pezawas et al., Nat. Neurosci. 8, 828 (2005).
H. S. Mayberg et al., Neuron 45, 651 (2005).
D. Ongur, J. L. Price, Cereb. Cortex 10, 206 (2000).
J. Houenou et al., Mol. Psychiatry 10.1038/sj.mp.
4002010 (2007).
10.1126/science.1147565
Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on August 10, 2007
sum of its observable parts. Identifying brain
regions correlated with “the parts” will be an
important next step for human imaging studies, but the field will need to avoid high-tech
phrenology. Understanding the neurobiology
of abnormal mood regulation will not be
accomplished through the identification of a
focal lesion or a single explanatory hot spot.
The task will be to define altered activity
within a functional neuronal network that
might well include both the ventral hippocampus and midline prefrontal cortex (15). The
importance of the new report by Airan et al.
is the demonstration of abnormal network
A surprising abundance of evolutionary
changes in transcription factor binding
sites may obscure the causes of phenotypic
divergence.
An Embarrassment of Switches
Leonid Kruglyak and David L. Stern
W
The authors are in the Department of Ecology and
Evolutionary Biology, and L. Kruglyak is also at the LewisSigler Institute for Integrative Genomics, Princeton
University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA. E-mail: leonid@
genomics.princeton.edu, [email protected]
758
Transcription factors
Target gene X in human
Protein-coding region
Binding sites
Species difference in regulatory region of gene X
Target gene X in mouse
Man or mouse? Both the presence and absence of transcription factor binding sites in a genome, as well as
the binding of transcription factors to sites that are present, can differ between species and may account for
differences in gene expression and phenotype.
always specifies a particular amino acid.
Thus, mutations that alter a protein can be
immediately read off from the DNA sequence.
By contrast, we are only beginning to understand how the cis-regulatory code works (3).
Cis-regulatory regions contain short strings
of nucleotides, from 6 to 20 nucleotides in
length, scattered irregularly in the vicinity of
the protein-coding DNA. Proteins called transcription factors bind to these short DNA
strings—transcription factor binding sites—
to regulate the production of messenger RNA
and thus the synthesis of proteins. In 1975,
King and Wilson found that only about 1% of
10 AUGUST 2007
VOL 317
SCIENCE
Published by AAAS
amino acids differed between a set of human
and chimpanzee proteins (4). They thus
proposed that changes in cis-regulatory
regions—evolutionary switching of transcription factor binding sites—might cause the
majority of phenotypic differences between
species. This hypothesis has gained support
from studies over the past decade (5).
Recent computational studies across
species illustrate that many transcription factor binding sites have evolved quickly. That is,
binding sites present in one species are often
absent in another (6–8). New findings provide
experimental evidence for this conclusion.
www.sciencemag.org
CREDIT: P. HUEY/SCIENCE
hat makes a human different from
a chimpanzee or a mouse? Of
course, we know the answer in
broad outline. Mutations in the genome, sifted
by natural selection, cause changes in appearance, physiology, and behavior—what geneticists call the phenotype. But we have only a
vague picture of a more detailed answer.
Precisely which mutations generate phenotypic evolution? It’s not that we can’t find the
mutations. Today’s DNA sequencing technology readily identifies all differences between
two genomes. There are simply too many differences—tens of millions between human
and chimp, for example (1). An unknown fraction of these mutations alter the phenotype.
Nonetheless, the molecular effects of mutations provide a rough guide to their phenotypic effects. Some mutations change the
amino acid sequence of proteins, thereby
altering their functions, and some change socalled cis-regulatory regions, altering when
and where proteins are produced. We know a
lot about the first class, but much less about
the second. Several recent papers, including
one by Borneman et al. on page 815 of this
issue (2), demonstrate a surprising abundance
of cis-regulatory changes between closely
related species.
It is easy to identify mutations that alter
proteins, because of the simplicity of the
genetic code. Linear strings of DNA nucleotide triplets encode proteins, and each triplet