Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
BWlOgicaljoumal ofthe LinncM Socieb (1997),61: 485-499.With 6 figures Cryptic coloration and choice of escape microhabitats by grasshoppers (Orthoptera: Acrididae) PAULA CABRAL ETEROVICK AND JOSE EUGfiNIO CORTES FIGUEIRA Departamto de Biologia Geral, Univerdade Federal de Minas Gerais, 3 0 1 61-970 Minas Gerais, Brasil JOAO VASCONCELLOS-NET0 I3IE Departamento de <oologia, Universidade Esladual de Campinas, CP 6109 13083-970 Campinas, ScZo Paulo, B r a d Received 2 September 1996; acceptedfor publicahin 6 December 1996 Cryptic coloration is found in many Orthoptera, especially in Acrididae, showing a great variety of forms. In a grasshopper assemblage in southeastern Brazil, preferences for escape places were detected; grasshoppers tended to escape to backgrounds in which they seem to be more cryptic. Coloration was measured using the Simpson diversity index, to quantifL ‘aspect diversity’ (diversity of colours and shapes of patches along the insect’s body). A weak positive correlation was found between grasshoppers’ aspect diversity and diversification in use of escape places (use of many backgrounds to escape). Grasshoppers with similar colour patterns tended to use the same structures (leaves, sandy soil, stones) to escape. 0 1997 The Linnean Society of London ADDITIONAL KEY WORDS:--colour pattern - habitat selection - escape behaviour. - morphological diversity - polymorphism CONTENTS Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Methods. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Study site . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Testing grasshoppers’ preference for escape structures and backgrounds . Influence of aspect diversity on escape structure and background choice . Convergence of colour pattern and use of structures . . . . . . . Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 486 487 487 487 489 492 493 496 498 499 Correspondence to P. Cabral Eterovick. Email: [email protected] + 0024-4066/ 1997/080485 15 $25.00/0/bj960134 485 0 1997 The Linnean Society of London 486 P. CABRAL ETEROVICK E T A INTRODUCTION Cryptic coloration, common in many taxonomic groups of animals (Cott, 1940), has an important evolutionary meaning for prey species, making their detection by visually-oriented predators difficult (Tinbergen, 1957; Papageorgis, 1975; Schultz, 1986). To be considered cryptic, an animal must match some part of its microhabitat (Endler, 1978). Microhabitat is defined here as background, comprising a portion of the environment with specific colours and composition, e.g. types of soil and plant cover. Coincidence of an animal colour pattern with the background pattern can reduce predation, and natural selection by predators can lead to similarity among forms that use the same background (Vasconcellos-Neto, 1987). On the other hand, in apostatic selection, diversity in prey is maintained because predators learn to recognize common morphs (or the most common species) and prey upon them more intensely (Rand, 1967; Vasconcellos-Neto, 1987). Cryptic beetles can be expected to show their real cryptic potential after detecting a predator’s presence, when they then choose the most suitable place to avoid detection (Schultz, 1986). Grasshoppers and other insects which match the same background can still be very Werent from one another (Rand, 1967). This is because insects which use the same background can match different portions of it, which we call here ‘structures’, that is, surfaces with particular colour and texture, e.g. sandy soil, grass, a leaf or a stone (see Endler, 1978; Sandoval, 1994). Some insects considered to be specialists can match a given structure in colour as much as in body shape and position while resting (Endler, 1978). In these cases, an insect’s colour pattern has become so similar to that of its preferred structure that it is conspicuous only when on other structures or backgrounds. On the other hand, it should be expected that cryptic insects using many different structures and backgrounds to escape from predators will have many Werent colours along the body, thus ensuring that by disrupting the body shape, at least some part of the body matches each potential escape place. Orthopterans are well known for their crypsis patterns (e.g. Gill, 1979; Dearn, 1981;Joern & Lawlor, 1981; Schultz, 1981; Sandoval, 1994). Disruptive patterns, that is, alternating light and dark patches or stripes, probably improve the escape effect of cryptic coloration, causing the body outline to be less evident (Cott, 1940). Morphological radiations are also known in the Acrididae (Cott, 1940) and may be due to apostatic selection. Grasshoppers are preyed upon by lizards, birds and mammals (Schultz, 1981; Rocha & Bergallo, 1994; Sandoval, 1994). However, the effect of selection on the existing morphological variability in Acrididae still needs rigorous testing (Rand, 1967). In this study, we investigated three assumptions related to the aspect and spatial organization of a diversified grasshopper assemblage that occurs in natural montane fields (‘campos rupestres’) of the Serra do Cipb (Minas Gerais state, Brazil). First, we tested the existence of preferences for structures or backgrounds as escape space by Merent forms of grasshoppers. Second, we checked if grasshoppers’ ‘aspect diversity’ is related to the diversity in structure and/or background use. ‘Aspect diversity’ was considered here as the diversity of colours and mottling along an insect’s body. Third, we investigated if grasshoppers using the same structures to escape were the most similar among the total assemblage. CHOICE OF ESCAPE MICROHABITATS BY C R m C GRASSHOPPERS 487 METHODS The study was conducted in a montane field (43”35’32’’W, 19”17’23”S, 1200 m alt.) in the Serra do Cip6, located in the Serra do EspinhaCo range. The region has a rich flora, with the following families especially well represented Veloziaceae, Eriocaulaceae, Cyperaceae, Melastomataceae and various shrubby Asteraceae (Giulietti et al., 1987). The terrain is hilly, with quartzite elevations, sandy or stony soil, and small streams and marshes in seepage areas (Sazima & Bokermann, 1977). The assemblage of acridids studied was located in a field of about 28000m2, enclosed by quartzite elevations. This field presented a mosaic of sandy soils in varying shades of grey, and patches of stones and quartz crystals. A herbaceous stratum consisting primarily of bush grasses and sedges varied throughout the area. These elements resulted in a mosaic of many backgrounds. Potential visually-oriented predators of grasshoppers occur at the study site: the lizards Tmpidum nanuzae and Tmpidulur montunus (T torquatus group), and the birds Molothm b0nahnsi-s (Icteridae),Cyanocorax cnStutellus (Corvidae), Mimus saturninus (Mimidae), Pobstictus supercilianS (Tyrannidae), Jvystulus chacuru (Bucconidae), Anthus hlLmyri (Motacilidae), Rhynchotus r u j s c m and Jvothura maculosa (Tinamidae) (M.F. Vasconcelos, pers. comm.). Tmpidum montanus is known to prey on grasshoppers in the study site (M.C. Kiefer, pers. comm.). Tyrannids and icterids have been already cited as grasshopper predators in other sites (Cott, 1940; Schultz, 1981). 5stin.ggrasshoppers’ prejimcefor escape structures and backg/ounds The acridids sampled in the study site were grouped into morphospecies by external morphology. Since our predictions are based on camouflage properties, and colour pattern may vary intraspecifically (Gill, 1979; Dearn, 1981; Sandoval, 1994))each grasshopper with a Merent morphological aspect was considered as a distinct morphospecies, not considering, for the preference tests, their being polymorphic forms of the same biological species or from different species. Among the ten morphospecies studied, those numbered 1, 2, 3 and 4 belong to the genus Rhammatocerus (Gomphocerinae),four others are also Gomphocerinae (Nos. 5, 8, 9 and 10) and two are Leptysminae (Nos. 7 and 8) (Fig. 1). Observations were made in the dry season, from August to November of 1995, before the spring rains began. We recorded the structure and background where adult grasshoppers positioned themselves after jumping when disturbed by our walking. We used the term ‘escape place’ to refer to both background and structure. To make the use of statistical tests possible, we included in this study only the 10 morphospecies having at least 10 observations each. Four of these morphospecies were relatively rare and we observed individuals more than once after successive jumps. It is important to stress that they always had the possibility of changing the structure and background used in each observation due to the proximity of all structures and backgrounds in the habitat mosaic. The six habitat structures we considered to possess distinct colour and texture in the field habitat were green leaves, dead (brownish) leaves, light sand, dark grey sand, brownish stones and quartz crystals. Backgrounds, considered as larger patches CHOICE OF ESCAPE MICROHABITATS BY CRYF'TIC GRASSHOPPERS 489 of the field habitat, were grouped in nine categories, according to the availability of structures within them (Fig. 2). 1. Dark sandy soil with an abundant herbaceous cover. 2. Dark sandy soil with sparse grass bushes. 3. Light sandy soil with or without brownish stones and an abundant herbaceous cover. 4. As 3 but with sparse grass bushes. 5. Mosaic soil of dark and light sand and an abundant herbaceous cover. 6. As 5 but with sparse grass bushes. 7. Soil covered by small quartz crystals (up to 2cm in their longer axis) and abundant herbaceous cover. 8. As 7 but with sparse grass bushes. 9. Completely covered by a dense herbaceous vegetation. Structures and backgrounds were quantified in the environment by measuring, in mm, their extensions in contact with folding rules extended in 30 line transects 1 m long, randomly positioned in the study site. Two measures of structures were made: one just above the ground and another 10 cm up, to q u a n q their whole spatial availability, as grasses are used as escape places by grasshoppers, in general, to nearly this height. Proportion out of whole environment was calculated for each structure and background type. Results for use and occurrence of escape places were compared by Chi-square tests (Fowler & Cohen, 1990). The less common five morphospecies provided insufficient data to conduct this test; instead, their observed and expected values of use were compared. Data from morphs of the same grasshopper species (those numbered 2, 3 and 4) were compared by Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests, which were adequate to the small sample size (Zar, 1984). I@uace ofaspect diversity on escape stmctun and background choice We postulated that grasshoppers with a greater aspect diversity could remain cryptic in more escape places, using structures and backgrounds with greater variability. To test this hypothesis, we projected slides showing the morphospecies, and measured aspect diversity using two perpendicular transects along the grasshoppers' bodies, all in the same scale (Fig. 3). We measured the length of each body patch of different colour in the transects, including patches of the same colour when apart from each other. Endler (1984) measured crypsis of colour pattern of moths against many backgrounds in a similar way. We calculated two diversity indices, based on Simpson's Diversity Index, which was chosen because it emphasized larger patches and more abundant colours. Large patches contribute more to the definition of the prevalent colour in the insect's body, and the more abundant and prevalent colour was expected to have an important influence on the cryptic properties of the insect. An index for patch shape diversity (PJ was given by the formula: CHOICE OF ESCAPE MICROHABITATS BY CRYF’TIC GRASSHOPPERS 49 1 Figure 3. Transects made through grasshoppers’ bodies to quantify ‘aspect diversity’ Where Pp=number of mm corresponding to each patch length @) divided by the transects’ total length in mm. This index gives a measure of mottling (small patches) vs. blotchiness (large patches or spots). Blotchiness is related to more uniform and less disrupted coloration patterns, which were expected to keep grasshoppers cryptic in a relatively small number of structures, which is opposite to mottling. According to Endler (1984) the shape of blotches on the insect’s body can influence its cryptic potential. Another index was calculated to measure colour diversity (PJ. For this purpose, all the patches of each distinct colour had their lengths summed to give a measure of the abundance of this colour: Where P,, = sum of lengths of all the patches (mm)of the same colour (co) divided by the transects’ total length in mm. The measures of ‘aspect diversity’ (&) was given by the product of ‘Ps’ and ‘Pc’. &=P, x P, Two indices were also used to quantify the diversity of escape places preferred, one to structures (S,) and another to backgrounds (&): Where P,, =number of observations in which a given morphospecies used structure ‘st’ divided by the total number of observations for this morphospecies. Where Pb=number of observations in which a given morphospecies used the background ‘b’ divided by the total number of observations for this morphospecies. P.CABRAL ETEROVICK ETAL 492 Zona lateralis Linea media carina Barred femur / Striped wing Figure 4. Features and measurements related to crypsis. An overall index for diversity in use of escape places D (), product of both indices: was given by the D,, = s,X S b The relation between aspect diversity (&) and diversity in structures and backwas evaluated using Spearman’s correlation (Wilkinson, 1990). grounds use Pep) Correlations among patch shape diversity (P,), colour diversity (PJ, structures use diversity (S,) and backgrounds use diversity ( s b ) were also examined. Convergence of colour pattern and we of structures We postulated that grasshoppers with similar colour patterns could use structures in a similar way. To compare all morphospecies pairs, we considered quantitative and qualitative features related to colour pattern and cryptic behaviour. They included body length, the body lengthldepth ratio, presence or absence of the colours present in the grasshoppers studied (white, light grey, dark grey, black, beige, brown and green) and presence or absence of stripes and patches commonly found in grasshoppers (Linea media, Carina media, Lnea scapuhris, striped wing, lateral stripe, Xona laterah, barred femur and mottled wing; Fig. 4). Similarities between each CHOICE OF ESCAPE MICROHABITATS BY CRypTlC GRASSHOPPERS 493 TABLE 1. Proportion of structures and backgrounds in the study site (measured in centimeters, with folding rules on line transects). Substrates are numbered as in Figure 2. In Chi-square tests, degrees of freedom are 5 for substrates and 8 for backgrounds Background types Total Availability in the study site (Yo) GL* DL Ls DS BS QC 16.3 20.0 6.9 5.2 10.1 25.9 2.3 6.6 6.7 53.3 37.8 62.5 32.6 52.2 27.8 70.3 27.3 89.0 17.9 11.5 7.8 8.7 17.0 12.8 1 .o 22.7 11.0 0.7 1 .8 26.1 54.8 13.4 23.0 27.9 48.6 17.4 27.8 20.3 4.9 3.6 3.8 6.6 0.2 0.3 - - 100.0 46.4 13.5 11.6 23.3 Availability of each stNcture (“10) ~ - ~ - 1.8 - 2.0 8.4 43.3 - 2.0 3.2 - ~~ * GL =green leaves, DL= dead leaves, LS =light sand, DS =dark grey sand, BS =brown stones, QC =quartz crystals. morphospecies pair (S,) were obtained by Colles’ (1967) method, which makes possible the simultaneous use of quantitative and qualitative variables: 1 s, =-c.c,, n Where n =number of variables considered; Ci, = similarity between morphospecies ‘p’ and ‘q’ considering feature ‘i’ To qualitative variables: Ciw = 1 if ‘p’ and ‘q’ have the same feature ‘i’ Ci, = 0 if ‘p’ and ‘q’ don’t have the same feature ‘i’ To quantitative variables: Where pi and qi are measures of variable ‘i’ in ‘p’ and ‘q’ and P, and Pmh measure the total variation of this variable in the grasshopper assemblage. Similarities in structures use were calculated for each morphospecies pair using Symmetric Overlap Index ( o j k ) , proposed by Pianka (1973): Where P, =relative use of structure ‘i’ by morphospecies 2’; Pik=relative use of structure ‘i’ by morphospecies ‘k’. We compared Colles’ indices to symmetric overlap indices for all morphospecies pairs, using Spearman’s rank correlation. RESULTS The proportions of structures and backgrounds in the study site are given in Table 1. Among the 10 morphospecies of Acrididae studied, five showed statistically P. CABRAL ETEROVICK EIAL. 494 TABLE2. Data for use of structures and backgrounds by the five most common grasshopper morphospecies. Preferred escape places are printed in boldface. Morphospecies are numbered as in Figure 1 Morpho species 2 Morpho species 4 Morpho species 5 Morpho species 7 Morpho species 8 Structures obs exp obs exp obs exp obs exp obs exp Green leaves Dead leaves Light sand Dark sand Brown stones Quartz crystals No. of observations 20 5 8 24 1 4 28.8 8.4 7.2 14.4 1.2 2.0 43 16 32 91 1 8 88.6 25.8 22.2 44.5 3.8 6.1 63 33 5 23 0 3 59.0 17.1 14.7 29.6 2.5 4.1 95 21 0 0 0 0 53.8 15.7 13.5 27.0 2.3 3.7 42 15 30 42 9 7 67.3 19.6 16.8 33.8 2.9 4.6 P Background No. of observations xz P 191 82.83 <0.01 62 12.53 <0.05 xz 145 36.93 10.0 1 116 127 25.66 <0.01 obs exp obs exp obs exp obs exp obs exp 14 16 1 2 4 10 3 9 5 10.4 12.8 4.4 3.3 6.5 16.6 1.5 4.2 4.3 41 58 1 2 7 55 3 16 9 31.3 38.4 13.3 10.0 19.3 49.7 4.4 12.7 12.9 23 36 2 3 4 23 9 9 18 20.7 25.4 8.8 6.6 12.8 32.9 2.9 8.4 8.5 29 19 0 0 6 26 6 4 26 18.9 23.2 8.0 6.0 11.7 30.0 2.7 7.7 7.8 14 14 4 9 14 92 1 12 0 26.1 32.0 11.0 8.3 16.2 41.4 3.7 10.6 10.7 64 15.85 <0.05 192 41.69 <0.01 127 44.20 <0.01 116 72.17 <0.01 160 92.17 <0.01 obs=tknes each morphospeciesused a structure or background to escape. exp=tknes each morphospccies would be expected to use a given structure or background to escape due only to this structure or background proportion in the environment. sigdicant preferences for certain structures and backgrounds (Table 2). Fewer data were obtained for the other five, but they also seemed to have preferences (Table 3). Although some grasshopper forms (Nos. 2, 3 and 4)belong to the same species, 21.26, they differed in their use of escape space appropriate to the structure (Dmax= RO.001, df=6, 162 for Nos. 2 and 3; D,=18.61, RO.001, df=6, 191 for Nos. 2 and 4 and D,=76.13, RO.001, df=6, 191 for Nos. 3 and 4).Nos. 2 and 3 also differed in their use of backgrounds (D-= 12.18, RO.01, df=9, 64) as did Nos. 3 and 4 (D,=42.53, RO.001, df=9, 192). However, Nos. 2 and 4 did not differ in their use of backgrounds (D-= 7.06, B0.20, df= 9, 64); they differed in their use of escape space by using distinct structures in the same background. On the other hand, similarities in escape space use occurred between morphospecies belonging to different subfamilies (as in morphospecies 1 and 9, see Table 3). The grasshoppers studied can be grouped by their escape behaviour in those using the ground (Nos. 1, 2, 3, 4,8 and 9) and those using the grassy vegetation to escape (Nos. 5, 6 and 7). Grasshoppers which are grey and have mottled wings tended to use sandy grey soils. Some of the grasshoppers matched the soil not due to their mottled wings, but because they were the same size as patches formed by dark and light sand in CHOICE OF ESCAPE MICROHABITATS BY CRYPTIC GRASSHOPPERS 495 TABLE3. Data for use of structures and backgrounds by the five least common grasshopper morphospecies. Preferred escape places are printed in boldface. Morphospecies are numbered as in Figure 1 Morpho species I Structures Green leaves Dead leaves Light sand Dark sand Brown stones Quartz crystals No. of observations Background 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 No. of observations obs 7 1 3 7 0 . exp obs 8.3 7 1 6 2 2.4 2.1 4.2 0.4 0.6 0 Morpho species 3 Morpho species 10 obs exp obs exp ohs exp 7.4 16 12 0 0 0 0 13 3.8 3.2 6.5 0.6 0.9 3 1 5 7 0 0 7.4 2.2 1.9 3.7 1 5 6 0.3 1 0 7.0 2.0 1.7 3.5 0.3 0.5 0.5 16 18 Morpho species 9 exp 2.2 1.9 3.7 0.3 0 0 Morpho species 6 0.5 2 15 16 28 obs exP obs exp obs exp obs exp obs 1 5 0 0 1 2.9 2.8 3.4 5 4 4.6 2.0 2.4 0 0 1.2 1 1 2 0.8 0.6 I .8 2 4.7 0.4 1.2 1.2 6 0 0 3 0.9 1.7 4.4 0.4 1.1 1.9 1.5 2.8 7.3 0.6 I .8 1.9 0 1 0 0.9 1 4 0 1 0 8 1.2 3.1 0 1 0 0.3 0.8 0 0 2 13 0 0 0.8 0 - 3.6 1.3 8 1 I 1 18 1.1 17 5.6 0 2 0 2 13 28 12 exp 2.4 3.0 I .o 0.8 1.5 3.9 0.3 1.0 1.0 15 obs = times each rnorphospecies used a structure or background to escape. exp = times each rnorphospecies would be expected to use a given structure or background to escape due only to this structure or background proportion in the environment. the mosaic soil of background 6 (see also Schultz, 1986). Ground-users also have barred femora and/or dorsal stripes (Linea media, Carina media, striped wings), which seem to function as disruptive coloration (Cott, 1940) and so allow grasshoppers to use dark as well as white sand or quartz crystals to avoid detection. It is also interesting to notice that grasshoppers with small green patches or stripes (Nos. 1 and 3) used relatively more grass leaves than those with a similar pattern of patches but no shades of green (Nos. 2 and 4).This reinforces the importance of coloration in their choice of escape places. Grasshoppers with a predominance of green or brown and beige colours (Nos. 5 , 6 and 7) often have lateral stripes and used only green or dead grass blades to escape. The two species of Leptysminae matched grass blades not only in their colours, but also in the elongated body shape and the positioning of their bodies and antennae parallel to leaves. The grasshoppers of morphospecies No. 10 were the only ones that did not follow any of the tendencies described above. They seem to have another escape strategy: when they jump to escape, they show a bright red spot on the second pair of wings and hide it just before settling down. It is important to stress that during escape many individuals from all morphospecies when landing on a structure which did not match their colour pattern, walked immediately to another, better matching substrate. This suggests an active structure choice. There was an upper limit to the number of different morphospecies using the 496 P. CABRAL ETEROVICK E T A . 20 40 60 80 100 Aspect diversity (Ad) Figure 5. Relation between diversity in use of structures and backgrounds and aspect diversity in acridid morphospecies. same escape place. Morphospecies using the same structure did not exceed 5 (mean=3.33, s = 1.63). The largest number of users of a background was also 5 (mean = 3.33, s = 1.37). More ‘aspect diverse’ morphospecies tended to use more structure and background types, though the correlation between ‘aspect diversity’ (&) and diversity in use of was not significant at critical value of 0.05 (r,=0.60, -0.10, escape places Pep) n = 10) (Fig. 5). No correlation was found among patch shape diversity (P,), colour diversity (PJ, diversity in use of structures (S,) and diversity in use of backgrounds (sb)* Similaritiesamong grasshoppers’ colour pattern (Table 4) were positively correlated to their convergence in use of structures (Y= 26.4 1 O.42Xy&0.005, n = 45) (Fig. 6). The maximum colour pattern similarity between morphospecies pairs (mean= 0.85, s =0.085) was smaller than the maximum similarity in the use of structures (mean=0.92, s =0.087). + DISCUSSION The correspondence of an insect’s colour pattern with its background helps it to avoid detection by visually-orientedpredators (Dearn, 1981;Schultz, 1981;Feltmate, Williams 8z Montgomerie, 1992). Avoidance of visually-oriented predators can be the selective factor responsible for the existence and maintenance of polymorphism in a species, when each form uses a Werent microhabitat (Sandoval, 1994). This kind of divergence occurred in the polymorphic species of Rhammatocm studied here. We found that cryptic grasshoppers using the same structure still differ among themselves, in spite of being more distinct from morphospecies using other structures. It suggests that apostatic selection has been maintaining morphological diversity in this grasshopper assemblage, as maximum similarity of appearance among morphospecies using the same structure does not quite reach the level of similarity CHOICE OF ESCAPE MICROHABITATS BY CRYPTIC GRASSHOPPERS 497 TABLE 4. Morphospecies characterization according to quantitative and qualitative features and preferred structures and backgrounds Morphospecies Qualitative features Colours White Light grey Dark grey Black Beige Brown Green Othen Lima mulia Catina mdia Linca stapulnris Striped wing Lateral stripe <ow lntcralis Barred femur Mottled wing Quantitative features Length (mm) Width (mm) 1 2 3 +* + + + + + + + 4 5 + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + 34 8 36 7 32 7 33 6 6 + + + + + + 7 8 + + + + + + + 9 + + + + + + + + + 23 4 + + + + + + 0 + + + + + + + + + + + 21.5 5 24 5 47 11 + 25 34 2 . 5 3 1 * + =presence of the considered feature. 100 '8P p 40 30 0 20 0 I 20 I I I 40 60 80 Morphological similarity (Spq) 100 Figure 6. Relation between similarity in use of structures and colour pattern similarity in acridid morphospecies. in their use of structures. A higher similarity among the grasshoppers could increase predation levels upon them (see Rand, 1967). Sandoval (1994) found that grey forms of Z m a cristim have a preference for grey and brown resting places, and Gill (1979) studied a form of Chorthippus brunneus 498 P. CABRAL ETEROVICK ETAL. with mottled wings which spent most time on the ground, just like the grey mottled grasshoppers studied here. It seems that mottled wings perfectly match the sand texture, causing cryptic coloration to be very efficient. Gill (1979) also described a morph of Chorthippus brunneus with striped wings that frequently rested on leaves, and Samdoval (1994) found a preference for green leaves in a green morph of limema cridnae. In the present study, longitudinal stripes seemed to be associated with the use of leaf blades, in which the insects rest parallel to the blade so their stripes align with the margins of the leaves. Cott (1 940) also described this behaviour for A c d u turrita. The species of Leptysminae studied can be considered specialists in the use of grass blades, due to their uniform colour, lateral stripes, and resting behaviour, features that make them remarkably cryptic on grass blades. Morphospecies No. 10, in spite of choosing escape places in which it was not so cryptic, had another kind of escape behaviour, showing a bright red spot while flying. When the grasshoppers landed, they hid the spot and became difficultto see. This kind of display, named flash-colour, is common among grasshoppers (Vasconcellos-Neto, pers. observ.). It attracts a predator’s attention and then suddenly disappears, confusing the predator and making prey location difficult (Cott, 1940). Joern & Lawlor (1981) also defined two guilds of grasshoppers in the southern United States, based on microhabitat use: those using the ground and those using the vegetation to escape. They found a maximum of five species in each microhabitat in Marathon and three in the Sul Ross Mountains, which they related to the grasshopper guild structure. Organization of an assemblage in escape strategy guilds can be due to predation pressures in evolutionary time (Ricklefs & O’Rourke, 1975). We also found a maximum of five grasshoppers using the same escape places. It may exist an upper limit to the number of grasshopper morphological units which can use the same escape place in assemblages structured based on cryptic properties. It would be interesting to look for this limit in grasshopper assemblages from other different habitats. Orthoptera species that use the ground as escape space tend to be more widely distributed in a habitat than those using plant species (Sandoval, 1994). In the present study, however, the distribution of grasshoppers among backgrounds seemed to be more related to body coloration than to the structure used. Some grasshoppers which preferred to use grass blades and have a more variegated colour pattern (morphospecies No. 5 ) occurred in a greater variety of backgrounds than other less variegated grasshoppers found on the ground (Nos. 8 and 9) (Fig. 5). The advantage of aspect diversity may be that the variety of colours and shapes of patches on the insect’s body makes crypsis possible on a greater variety of backgrounds and structures. The colour on the insect that matches a part of its background may disrupt the outline of the body so that predators cannot detect it. Specialists, on the other hand, must remain associated with the structure to which they are adapted in order to maintain their cryptic potential. However, specialists, which always rest on a specific structure, are less likely to be found by generalist predators. Thus, a trade-off between predation avoidance efficiency and behavioral plasticity may exist in cryptic grasshoppers. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We are grateful to W.W. Benson, G.W. Fernandes, M.A. Marine, R.P. Martins and D. Yanega who kindly read the manuscript and gave helpful suggestions. We also thank D. Yanega for revision of the English. CHOICE OF ESCAPE MICROHABITATS BY CRYPTIC GRASSHOPPERS 499 REFERENCES Colles DH. 1967. An examination of certain concepts in phenetic taxonomy. systematic <oology 16: 6-2 7. Cott HB. 1940. Adaptive coloration in animals. London: Methuen. DearnJM. 1981. Latitudinal cline in a color pattern polymorphism in the Australian grasshopper Phaularridium uittutum. Hmdi& 47: 1 1 1-1 19. Endler JA. 1978. A predator view of animal color patterns. Euohtionaly Biology 11: 319-364. Endler JA. 1984. Progressive background in moths, and a quantitative measure of crypsis. Biological Journal ofthe Lintuan So&& 22: 187-231. Feltmate BW, Williams DD, Montgomerie A. 1992. Relationship between diurnal activity patterns, cryptic coloration, and subsequent avoidance of predaceous fish by perlid stoneflies. Canadian Journal of F k h s and Aquatic Sciences 49: 263Ck2634. Fowler J, Cohen L. 1990. Practical stutktisforjeld biology. Philadelphia: Open University Press. Gill PD. 1979. Color-pattern variation in relation to habitat in the grasshopper Chorthippus brunneur (Thurnberg). Ecological Entomology 4: 249-257. Giulietti AM, Menezes NL, PiraniJR, Meguro MyWanderley MGL. 1987. Flora da Serra do Cip6, Minas Gerais: caracterizaq2o e lista de espkcies. Bobtim Botcinico, Uniuersidade de Sao Paulo 9: 1-151. Joern A, Lawlor LR. 1981. Guild structure in grasshopper assemblages based on food and microhabitat resources. Oikos 37: 93-104. Papageorgis C. 1975. Mimicry in Neotropical butterflies. American Scientist 63: 522-532. Pianka ER. 1973. The structure of lizard communities. Annual Review ofEcology and Syslematics 4: 53-74. Rand AS. 1967. Predator-prey interactions and the evolution of aspect diversity. A& do Simpdsio sobre a Biob Ama&ica 5: 73-83. Ricklefs RE, O'Rourke K. 1975. Aspect diversity in moths: a temperate-tropical comparison. Evolution 29: 3 13-324. Rocha CF, Bergallo HG. 1994. I: torqualm (Collared Lizard) diet. Herpetological Revieu, 25: 69. Sandoval C. 1994. Differential visual predation on morphs of limema &tinae (Phasmatodeae: Timemidae) and its consequences for host range. BiologicalJournal ofthe Linnean So&& 52: 341-356. Sazima I, Bokermann WCA. 1977. Anfibios da Serra do Cip6, Minas Gerais, Brasil. 3: ObservaqBes sobre a biologia de Hyla aluamgai Bok. (Anura, Hylidae). Re&tu Brm'kira de Biologia 37: 41 3 4 1 7. Schultz JC. 1981. Adaptive changes in antipredator behavior of a grasshopper during development. Euolution 35: 175-1 79. Schultz JC. 1986. Role of structural colors in predator avoidance by tiger beetles of the genus Cicindela (Coleoptera: Cicindelidae). Bulbtin ofthe ESA (Fall):142-146. Tinbergen N. 1957. Defense by color. Scimtjfic American 197: 49-55. Vasconcellos-Net0 J. 1987. GenCtica ecobgica de C h b o r p h a cribaria F. 1775 Cassidinae, Chrysomelidae. PhD. Thesis, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Silo Pado, Brazil. Willtinson L. 1990. SYSTAE 2% g s h f o r stutistics. Evanston, I L Systat Inc. Zar JH. 1984. Biostutistical anahh. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.