Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
C 2004) Law and Human Behavior, Vol. 28, No. 4, August 2004 (! Firing Back at the Backfire Effect: The Influence of Mortality Salience and Nullification Beliefs on Reactions to Inadmissible Evidence Alison Cook,1,3 Jamie Arndt,1 and Joel D. Lieberman2 Two studies were conducted to explore psychological factors that contribute to the influence of inadmissible evidence (i.e., the backfire effect) on jurors’ verdicts. On the basis of hypotheses derived from terror management theory, we predicted that reminders of mortality, in contrast to an aversive control topic, should lead participants to be less punitive when confronted with inadmissible (as opposed to admissible) evidence, when participants were either situationally induced or dispositionally prone to follow their personal sense of justice. In Study 1, control participants who scored high on a measure of nullification beliefs, and thus were prone to relying on their own sense of justice rather than the law, exhibited the backfire effect. However, reminding participants of their mortality reduced the damaging influence of inadmissible evidence. Study 2 extended these findings by showing parallel effects with a manipulation of nullification proneness via judicial instructions. The implications of these factors on the judicial process are discussed. KEY WORDS: Terror management; backfire effect; juror motivation. INTRODUCTION “The jury is to disregard the witness’s last statement.” Such an admonition is likely familiar to most people whether they are legal professionals, academics acquainted with jury decision-making research, or lay people who only occasionally watch courtroom dramas on television. If evidence is obtained illegally, is misleading, or is prejudicial, it may be deemed inadmissible by a judge. The US legal system operates under the assumptions that jurors are both willing and able to ignore information that has been deemed inadmissible. Yet, research suggests this 1 Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri—Columbia, Columbia, Missouri. of Criminal Justice, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, Nevada. 3 To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri—Columbia, 210 McAlester Hall, Columbia, Missouri 65212; e-mail: [email protected]. 2 Department 389 C 2004 American Psychology-Law Society/Division 41 of the American Psychology Association 0147-7307/04/0800-0389/1 ! P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] 390 pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman may not be a reasonable assumption: admonitions to disregard inadmissible evidence are not always effective, especially when that evidence is incriminating (e.g., Fein, McCloskey, & Tomlinson, 1997; Wolf & Montgomery, 1977; for a review see Lieberman & Arndt, 2000). Indeed, limiting instructions to ignore information may even backfire, causing jurors to weigh inadmissible evidence more heavily than if the evidence was admissible (e.g., Broeder, 1959; Cox & Tanford, 1989; Edwards & Bryan, 1997). Given the important implications of the backfire effect for the legal system, it is critical to understand those psychological factors, both dispositional and situational, which facilitate and can potentially mitigate the emergence of this effect. One possibility investigated here, based on hypotheses derived from terror management theory (Solomon, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski, 1991), is that people’s awareness of their mortality might increase their desire to uphold beliefs about procedural fairness, thereby attenuating the backfire effect. This research also examines whether such a possibility would particularly be likely among individuals who are inclined to rely on their own sense of justice. Inadmissible Evidence and Backfire Effect Research Previous research has repeatedly shown that judicial admonitions to ignore evidence are ineffective (Lieberman & Arndt, 2000). In some cases, deeming information inadmissible may not only fail to prevent jurors from ignoring it, but also may promote a “backfire effect” in which the information actually becomes more influential on jurors’ verdicts (Cox & Tanford, 1989). In a classic demonstration of this effect, Broeder (1959) found that participants awarded higher damages to a plaintiff after being instructed by a judge to disregard a statement that a defendant was covered by insurance, than when no mention of insurance was made, or when jurors were told that the defendant had insurance but no admonitions to disregard that information were given. Since the original demonstration of the backfire effect, a number of studies have examined factors that moderate the effect and how the effect manifests itself in different types of cases (Pickel, 1995). For example, Wolf and Montgomery (1977) assessed whether the strength of admonition would influence mock jurors’ verdicts. Participants read a trial transcript that included critical evidence that supported either the prosecution or the defense. The evidence was ruled admissible, or inadmissible with either weak or strong judicial admonitions to disregard it. Mock jurors’ verdicts were influenced most in the direction of the presented testimony by the admissible and inadmissible plus strong admonishment conditions, but were less influenced when the information was deemed inadmissible using a weaker admonishment. Wolf and Montgomery (1977) interpreted these findings in light of reactance theory (Brehm, 1966). Briefly, reactance theory (Brehm, 1966) states that when someone feels her or his autonomy is threatened, that individual is motivated to regain freedom by doing whatever he or she feels prevented from doing. Therefore, the greater the threat to one’s freedom, the greater one desires to reassert her or his autonomy. In addition, the free behavior may become more attractive to an individual. Thus, Wolf and Montgomery (1977) suggested that when jurors are prevented by law from P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence 391 considering a piece of evidence, this admonition may increase reactance and thereby motivate people to ignore judicial instructions and use the forbidden information in order to reassert their freedom. These results suggest that jurors’ motivations may provide critical insight into when and why the backfire effect occurs. The present studies are also concerned with jurors’ motivation when confronted with inadmissible evidence; specifically, motivations provoked by peoples’ knowledge of death. According to recent research, an awareness of one’s own mortality may have wideranging influences on human social behavior broadly, and more specifically on jurors’ motivations in the context of rendering legal decisions (Arndt, Lieberman, Cook, & Solomon, 2003). Terror Management Theory and Research Terror management theory (Greenberg, Solomon, & Pyszczynski, 1997; Solomon et al., 1991) maintains that humans’ advanced cognitive abilities for thinking abstractly and symbolically lead to awareness of their own frailty and inevitable demise. This unique human awareness combined with a natural proclivity for selfpreservation causes the potential for anxiety and terror. This terror motivates individuals to uphold a set of culturally prescribed values and standards that constitute a cultural worldview, and imbues their lives with order, meaning, and permanence. Investing in a cultural worldview and, subsequently, maintaining one’s self-esteem by upholding these standards, buffers the individual from potential existential anxiety associated with his or her awareness of mortality. Because the worldview is an important buffer and also a fragile social construction, individuals defend their worldviews in order to quell death-related concerns. Over 150 studies to date, conducted in over nine different countries, indicate that reminding participants of their own mortality (mortality salience) leads to worldview defense: people are more likely to search for validation of their worldview and to defend their worldview against people or ideas that threaten it (for a review see Greenberg et al., 1997). This corpus of work has also demonstrated that these effects occur relative to a variety of other aversive topics (e.g., failure, pain, paralysis, and social exclusion). One direction of terror management research has demonstrated that mortality salience engenders more punitive reactions toward violators of cultural standards in legal settings. For example, Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, and Lyon (1989; Study 1) showed that after municipal court judges were reminded of their own mortality, they recommended higher bonds for an alleged prostitute compared to judges in the control condition, indicating mortality salience increased the need to uphold cultural standards by punishing the legal transgressor. Florian and Mikulincer (1997) replicated these findings and discovered that mortality salient individuals also showed increased negative reactions toward other legal and moral violations. In addition, a recent study revealed that mortality salience led to greater agreement with hate-crime legislation and statements that promoted harsher sentences for hate-crime perpetrators when there was no mention of a specific victim (Lieberman, Arndt, Personious, & Cook, 2001; Study 1). These findings converge with previous terror management research to show that notions of justice often appear to be extensions of one’s worldview and that the punishment of legal and moral transgressors can be seen as a reaction in defense of one’s worldview. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 392 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman Terror Management and the Backfire Effect Mortality Salience and Evidence Admissibility On the basis of this previous research, on the surface, we would expect people reminded of their death to be more punitive toward lawbreakers. As a result, mortality salience could potentially lead jurors to pay undue attention to incriminating inadmissible evidence, exaggerating the backfire effect, because the additional damaging information provides justification for punitive treatment of the defendant. However, a closer terror management analysis suggests that mortality salience might be expected to produce conflicting motivations that would instead result in the mitigation of the backfire effect. Specifically, although previous research has shown that participants reminded of their mortality tend to be more punitive toward legal and moral transgressors, part of this punitive reaction may stem from supporting a worldview that prescribes what are believed to be fair and just laws. In addition, part of the perceived fairness of laws may stem from the procedural proscriptions that protect the rights of the accused. Consistent with this idea, in a series of studies, van den Bos and Miedema (2000; see also van den Bos, 2001) found that participants reminded of their mortality had a greater need for procedural fairness. Participants reminded of their mortality in these studies showed increased positive reactions (e.g., positive affect; support) when they were confronted with fair rather than unfair procedures compared to control participants, and showed increased negative reactions when confronted with unfair relative to fair procedures. Presumably, these reactions occurred because fairness is an important component of most individuals’ worldview that imbues reality with order and meaning. Therefore, when a judge rules incriminating evidence inadmissible, the conflict between the procedural instructions designed to protect the defendant and the substantive instructions that outline the charge may lead to competing motivations for jurors to both protect and convict the defendant. If procedural fairness is part of an individual’s worldview and mortality has been made salient, jurors may be more motivated to adhere to the judge’s admonition (that protects a defendant and provides for a fair trial). This mortality salience provoked desire for fairness should in turn result in people being less punitive toward a defendant when incriminating evidence is ruled inadmissible than when it is ruled admissible. The Role of Justice Orientation Although there are reasons to expect that mortality salience will mitigate the backfire effect, there are also reasons to suspect that the effects of mortality salience and evidence admissibility will be moderated by dispositional or situational factors that evoke different orientations to justice. A number of prior terror management studies point to the moderating roles of various personality and situational factors in response to mortality salience (e.g., Arndt, Greenberg, & Cook, 2002; Greenberg et al., 1990; Harmon-Jones et al., 1997; McGregor et al., 1998). In the present context, we wanted to evaluate participants’ orientation toward following the spirit versus the letter of the law, a distinction otherwise known as “nullification” (Horowitz, 1985, 1988; Niedermeier, Horowitz, & Kerr, 1999; P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence Style file version June 4th, 2002 393 Lieberman & Sales, 1997; Wiener, Habert, Shkodriani, & Staebler, 1991). We reasoned that individuals prone to nullification would be more likely to ignore both specific procedural and substantive law instructions, and instead use any available relevant information so that a judgment can be made that upholds the spirit of the substantive law. When incriminating evidence that would lead toward a conviction of someone who has broken the spirit of the law exists, nullification prone jurors should be inclined to reject procedural law governing the admissibility of the evidence. Thus, nullification prone jurors should be more susceptible to the backfire effect. However, as previous research has indicated (van den Bos, 2001; van den Bos & Miedema, 2000), when mortality is made salient, notions of procedural fairness may become particularly compelling. As procedural law governs the application of substantive law (e.g., jurors may be instructed to ignore inadmissible evidence when deciding guilt or innocence), enhanced concerns for procedural fairness may outweigh substantive law motivations within nullification prone jurors, and the backfire effect therefore may be eliminated. Finally, when procedural fairness concerns are heightened by mortality salience, but there is not a conflict between procedural and substantive law, mortality salient individuals should primarily focus on the application of substantive law, and exhibit punitive reactions toward defendants. Thus, for participants oriented toward nullification, we expect to see significant mortality salience x evidence admissibility interactions on verdicts. In contrast, we do not necessarily expect to see such interactive effects among those who are oriented toward strictly following the law. These participants should be less prone to the backfire effect and thus also less prone to its reversal after mortality salience. These findings should occur in individuals who are either dispositionally or situationally prone to nullify the law. Overview These hypotheses were tested through two experiments using a jury decisionmaking paradigm. In Study 1, participants completed a packet of questionnaires that included a measure assessing nullification proneness and then were led to think of their own mortality or another aversive control topic (dental pain). Participants then read a trial transcript of a robbery in which critical incriminating evidence was deemed admissible, inadmissible, or was omitted. The primary dependant variable was the verdict participants rendered after reading the transcript and jury instructions. Study 2 was designed to replicate and extend the findings of the first experiment by manipulating justice orientation through judicial instructions. STUDY 1 Method Participants and Design Four to six participants per session were randomly assigned to conditions in a 2 (mortality salience vs. dental pain) × 3 (admissible vs. inadmissible vs. omitted P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 394 Style file version June 4th, 2002 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman evidence) between-subjects design. The primary dependent measure was the verdict rendered by each participant. One-hundred and thirty one introductory psychology students (36 males and 91 females) from a large university participated in the study in return for partial course credit. Procedure Upon participants’ arrival, an experimenter, who was blind to experimental conditions, described the study as looking at the relationship between personality characteristics and jury decision-making. Participants were informed that the study would consist of a series of personality questionnaires followed by a trial transcript of a robbery in which they would be asked to imagine themselves as actual jurors and to render a verdict based on the information presented in the transcript. The participants were assured that their responses were anonymous and they completed all materials in separate cubicles to further ensure a sense of anonymity. After the participants read and signed a consent form, the experimenter brought them a personality packet, followed by a transcript, and finally a verdict form. Once all participants had finished, they were probed for suspicion, thoroughly debriefed, thanked for their time, and dismissed. Personality Packet The mortality salience manipulation was embedded among a series of filler personality questionnaires to lend credence to the cover story. The initial questionnaires included a measure of nullification proneness based on previous research by Edwards and Bryan (1997) and Lieberman and Silver (2000). This measure contained eight statements used to assess nullification proneness. That is, how strongly an individual feels about upholding the letter versus the spirit of the law. Participants were asked to rate their agreement with each statement on a 9-point Likert scale with endpoints 1 (strongly disagree) to 9 (strongly agree). Example items are “A person who has broken the law should always be punished”; “A victimless crime is not a real crime”; “When legal guidelines conflict with what may be fair, a juror must uphold the law at all costs”; and “Laws are made to be broken.” Higher scores indicate a greater likelihood of relying on one’s own beliefs in deciding what is just, whereas lower scores indicate a greater tendency to follow the written law. After completing questions about nullification proneness and other filler questionnaires, as a purported measure of personality, participants completed two openended questions regarding either death or dental pain. This is the most common mortality salience treatment in terror management research (see Greenberg et al., 1997), and asks participants to respond to the following two questions: “Please briefly describe the emotions that the thought of your own death arouses in you” and “Jot down, as specifically as you can, what you think will happen to you as you physically die and once you are physically dead.” A dental pain questionnaire was used as an aversive control topic and the questions were parallel to those of the mortality salience manipulation. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence Style file version June 4th, 2002 395 Transcript Participants were presented with a simulated transcript of a case. The evidence was designed to be ambiguous regarding the defendant’s guilt to allow for variation in verdicts. The transcript presented a robbery trial in which a young man was accused of taking money and jewelry at gunpoint from two teenagers parked in a car at a local park. In addition to the opening and closing statements by the attorneys, the case included testimony from five witnesses who presented information about the events of the robbery, the description of the perpetrator, and/or the alibi of the defendant. Critical Evidence Following a procedure used by Thompson, Fong, and Rosenhan (1981), we manipulated the evidence admissibility through a judge’s ruling on a critical aspect of a detective’s testimony. Specifically, in the two experimental conditions (admissible and inadmissible), “Detective (Russell),” a witness for the prosecution, testified that he had been conducting an investigation on an illegal bookmaking operation by another individual, and as part of the investigation, had wiretapped calls made by the subject of this investigation. From these tapes, the detective learned that the robber phoned the bookmaker. Critical evidence obtained from the wiretap was presented in the transcript as follows: The caller identified himself as [Ian or Rashaun]4 and said, “I finally got the money to pay you. Can you meet by the basketball courts in Grant Park at 3:00?” Prosecution: What was his response to this? Russell: The bookmaker asked him “Where the hell did you get the money, you don’t even have a job” and the caller replied “I jacked a couple of kids in a car there yesterday.” Russell: This testimony was rendered admissible or inadmissible by the judge after the defense attorney objected to the legality of the wiretap used to obtain the information. In the inadmissible condition, the transcript read: Judge: I am not going to allow this. The wiretapping applied only to the operator and not to his callers. Neither the tape made from the wiretap, nor the detective’s statements regarding it are admissible. Strike from the record all reference to the alleged telephone conversation between the defendant and the bookmaker. Members of the jury, you are hereby ordered to disregard all mention of that conversation. It is your legal duty to ignore testimony ruled inadmissible when considering your verdict. 4 The study also included an additional race manipulation that failed to produce consistent effects on any measure and is thus not discussed more fully. We manipulated the race of defendant by referring to the defendant as Ian Jones (White) or Rashaun Jones (Black) and having the witnesses remark on the race of the defendant as they offered their description. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 396 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman The admissible version stated: Judge: I’m going to allow this. The legality of the wiretapping applied to the bookmaker as well as to his callers. In a control condition, this critical information and the wiretap were excluded altogether. Jury Instructions and Verdict The participants were then presented with the substantive and procedural instructions that explained the relevant laws and jurors’ duties in the case. Participants were asked to fulfill their duty as jurors by considering all of the testimony and exhibits admitted into evidence. Furthermore, the weight of the evidence and the judgment of credibility of the witnesses rested solely on the juror. The instructions emphasized that in order to convict the defendant, the jurors must be convinced beyond a reasonable doubt. The definition for robbery was included to clarify the crime of which the defendant was accused and the “presumption of innocence” was also defined. Lastly, participants were informed that “[i]f the commonwealth has failed to prove the elements of the charged offenses beyond a reasonable doubt, it is your duty to find the defendant not guilty.” The primary elements of our dependent variable, drawn from Pyszczynski, Greenberg, Mack, and Wrightsman (1981) and Edwards and Bryan (1997), asked participants to respond to the instruction, “The defendant has been charged with Robbery. Imagine that you are a real juror in this case. Please consider the evidence, and render a verdict.” Participants circled one of two phrases: “Guilty of Robbery” or “Not Guilty.” Participants were then asked to rate “How confident of your verdict are you?” They responded on a 9-point scale with endpoints of 1 (not at all confident) and 9 (totally confident). Impressions of the Case and Defendant Participants were then asked to rate elements of the case. The questions included: “How strong was the case presented by the prosecution?” and “How strong was the case presented by the defendant?” (each rated on a 9-point scale with endpoints 1 (very weak) and 9 (very strong)). The next series of questions asked participants to rate the defendant on the traits of honesty, likeability, remorse, intelligence, similarity to the participant, success, sincerity, wealth, and dangerousness (each on 9-point scales). Results Initial analyses of our primary dependent measure included sex of participant as a factor but because no significant main or interaction effects were found with this variable (here or in Study 2), it is not discussed further. Nullification Before assessing our main hypotheses, we first examined the reliability of the eight questions designed to index participants’ personal beliefs regarding reliance on P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 397 individual frames of reference when dealing with issues of justice. Cronbach’s alpha was low (.41).5 After performing a principal axis factor analysis, we did not find a clear solution, although five of the eight items loaded more highly on the one factor with an eigenvalue > 1 (loadings > .35), whereas the other three items did not. We therefore created a composite that averaged only these five items. However, because there was not a substantial increase in reliability as a result of dropping the three items (α = .54), and the results of all analyses were similar to those including all eight items, the analyses reported below included all eight items of the nullification measure. Verdicts The primary dependent measure was the verdict rendered by each participant. Following Pyszczynski et al. (1981) and Edwards and Bryan (1997), we computed an index of participants’ verdicts by multiplying their indications of guilty (coded as +1) or not guilty (coded as −1) by their level of confidence (scored on 9-point Likert scale) in their verdict. Thus, scores could range from −9 (very confident defendant is not guilty) to +9 (very confident defendant is guilty). We performed a median split on responses to the nullification proneness measure and conducted a 2 (high vs. low nullification scores) × 2 (mortality salience vs. dental pain) × 3 (admissible vs. inadmissible vs. omitted evidence) analysis of variance (ANOVA). This analysis found a two-way interaction between mortality salience and evidence, F(2, 126) = 4.41, p < .02, η2 = .07, which was qualified by a significant three-way interaction between mortality salience, evidence, and nullification, F(2, 126) = 3.15, p < .05, η2 > .05.6 To clarify this interaction effect, we tested for the simple mortality salience × evidence interactions within the high and low nullification groups, as this was the one nonmanipulated variable. This revealed that the mortality salience × evidence interaction was not evident for low nullification score participants, F < 1 (see Fig. 1), but was quite reliable among the high nullification score participants, F(2, 63) = 8.10, p < .01, η2 = .05 (see Fig. 2). Within this interaction, high nullification participants in the dental pain conditions who were told the incriminating evidence was inadmissible were more likely to confidently convict than were participants in either the admissible evidence condition or the no-evidence control condition, both ts > 2.14, ps < .04. Thus, in the absence of mortality salience, the typical backfire effect was pronounced among those individuals whose intuitive beliefs of justice guide their legal decisions. In contrast, among mortality salient high nullification score participants, inadmissible instructions lead to reduced guilty verdicts relative to their admissible and no-evidence counterparts, both ts > 2.00, ps ≤ .05. Looked at differently, mortality salient high nullification 5 We address the low reliability of these questions both in Study 2 and in the General Discussion section. 6 A multiple regression was performed between the verdicts as the dependent variable and salience, nulli- fication, and evidence as the independent variables. Evidence was dummy-coded since there were three levels of this categorical variable. The main effects were entered, then the two-way interactions, and then the three-way interactions. The full model was significant, F(11, 115) = 1.97, p < .05, and accounted for 15.9% of the variance in the verdict. Further, the regression coefficients for the three-way interaction between salience, nullification, and inadmissible evidence (dummy-coded) replicated the ANOVA findings, β = −2.19, F(1, 125) = 7.26, p < .01. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] 398 pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman Fig. 1. Evidence × Salience interaction among low nullification participants’ verdicts in Study 1. Note: Higher positive numbers reflect more confident guilty verdicts whereas higher negative numbers reflect more confident not guilty verdicts. participants were slightly more likely to vote guilty than their dental pain counterparts in the no-evidence control condition, t(63) = 1.54, p < .13, and were significantly more likely to vote guilty in the admissible evidence conditions than their dental pain counterparts, t(63) = 2.00, p < .05. However, mortality salient high nullification participants were significantly less likely to vote guilty than their dental pain counterparts when the incriminating evidence was ruled inadmissible, t(63) = 3.15, p < .01. Supplemental Questions on Impressions of the Case We then turned to examine whether participants’ perceptions of the case, as assessed with the questions that followed their verdicts, were similarly affected via 2 (justice nullification group) × 2 (mortality salience) × 3 (evidence) ANOVAs. Fig. 2. Evidence × Salience interaction among high nullification participants’ verdicts in Study 1. Note: Higher positive numbers reflect more confident guilty verdicts whereas higher negative numbers reflect more confident not guilty verdicts. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence Fig. 3. Style file version June 4th, 2002 399 Mortality salience × Evidence interaction on the impressions of the prosecutions’s case in Study 1. Note: Higher numbers reflect a stronger impression of the prosecution’s case. With respect to participants’ perceived strength of the prosecution’s case, only a mortality salience by evidence interaction was obtained, F(2, 130) = 5.05, p < .01, η2 = .08, all other Fs < 1.70, ps > .19. The cell means are displayed in Fig. 3 and reflect a pattern that mirrors the previous patterns of high nullification participants on verdicts. Dental pain participants thought the prosecution’s case was stronger when the incriminating evidence was ruled inadmissible compared to the no evidence condition, t(130) = 2.29, p < .03, but the inadmissible condition was not significantly stronger than the admissible condition, t < 1. Mortality salient participants in contrast, tended to rate the prosecution’s case as weaker in the inadmissible versus no evidence condition, t(130) = 1.73, p < .09, and admissible condition, t(130) = 2.65, p < .01. A 2 (justice nullification group) × 2 (salience) × 3 (evidence) ANOVA on perceived strength of the defense’s case revealed no main effects or interactions, all Fs < 1.87, ps > .23. Defendant Trait Ratings To analyze the impact of these treatments on participants’ trait ratings of the defendant, we created a composite that averaged participants’ ratings on the eight traits after reverse scoring the negative trait items (e.g., dishonest), Cronbach’s α = .67. Thus, higher scores indicate more positive ratings. There was a significant mortality salience by evidence interaction, F(2, 130) = 4.63, p < .02, η2 = .07, all other Fs < 1.85, ps > .16. Figure 4 presents these means. The nature of this interaction was also consistent with the mortality salience × evidence interaction previously described, although many of these comparisons did not reach significance. Whereas dental pain participants tended to rate the defendant less positively in the inadmissible evidence condition than in the admissible condition, t(130) = 2.86, p < .01, mortality salient participants rated the defendant more positively in the inadmissible condition relative to the others, both ts > 1.50, ps < .14. In the inadmissible evidence condition, mortality salient participants evaluated the defendant more positively than did dental pain participants, t(130) = 2.40, p < .02. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 400 Fig. 4. Style file version June 4th, 2002 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman Mortality salience × Evidence interaction on defendant trait ratings in Study 1. Note: Higher numbers reflect more positive evaluations of the defendant. Discussion This study hypothesized that the backfire effect would primarily emerge among participants who rely on their own intuitions of justice to guide legal decisions and that this effect would be mitigated by reminding participants of their death. The results offer support for these ideas. The present study successfully replicated the backfire effect, but only in the absence of mortality salience and among participants who scored high on the nullification proneness measure. This suggests that in situations not involving heightened concern with mortality, inadmissible evidence may only be more influential than admissible evidence when people follow their own intuitions of justice. However, when mortality is made salient, a very different pattern emerges. Indeed, among high nullification participants, mortality salience actually reversed the backfire effect: mortality salient participants were less punitive in the inadmissible condition compared to the admissible and control conditions. According to the present analysis, this pattern occurs because mortality salience leads people to defend a worldview that often includes upholding the law and an increased desire for fairness. The supplemental questions, however, were not fully consistent with the results found with participants’ verdicts. With regard to perceived strength of the prosecution’s case and the defendant trait ratings, only mortality salience by evidence interactions were found and not the three-way interactions involving nullification. However, both of these patterns were consistent with a mortality salience induced reversal of the backfire effect: dental pain participants rated the prosecution’s case as stronger and the defendant more negatively in the inadmissible compared to the admissible conditions whereas mortality salience participants showed a reversed pattern. We return to the issue of how perceptions of the case may relate to verdicts in the General Discussion section. Overall, the present findings are provocative because they suggest that motivations in legal context may shift under mortality salience in a way that is further moderated by nullification proneness. However, confidence in the role of nullification in these effects must be cautioned. The measure we designed to index nullification P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence Style file version June 4th, 2002 401 beliefs, though based on measures used successfully in prior research (Lieberman & Silver, 2000) and conforming to the predicted pattern, did not show conventionally acceptable reliability in this study. Presumably, the effects would only be strengthened by the use of a more reliable measure of nullification proneness. However, to increase confidence in our findings, we conducted a second study to address this limitation. STUDY 2 This experiment was designed to replicate and extend the results of Study 1. Whereas the previous study met with only marginal success in measuring nullification beliefs, in this study we opted to manipulate whether participants relied on their personal feelings or strictly followed the law in rendering their decision. Thus, when participants were given their jury instructions, these instructions included text that informed them as to whether they should follow their own consciences or strictly adhere to the law. It should be noted that several states such as Maryland provide jury instructions that specifically inform jurors as to their nullification powers. We again predicted that the backfire effect should emerge primarily in the absence of mortality salience among participants instructed to base their decision upon personal feelings. However, after mortality salience, participants who receive the justice nullification instructions should be more punitive in the admissible than inadmissible conditions. Such convergent results using this quite different operationalization of justice nullification would offer strong support for the present analysis. Method Design and Procedure Participants were randomly assigned to conditions in a 2 (mortality salience vs. dental pain) × 2 (admissible or inadmissible evidence) × 2 (justice orientation: nullification or law) between-subjects design. The primary dependent measure was again the verdict rendered by each participant. One-hundred and twenty one introductory psychology students (56 males and 63 females) from a large university participated in the study in return for partial course credit. The materials and procedure were the same as in Study 1 with the exceptions noted below. Personality Packet The only change to the initial personality packet was the omission of measuring nullification proneness. The packet included the same mortality salience and dental pain manipulations as in Study 1. Transcript The transcripts were identical to those used for Study 1. However, we did not include a “no-evidence” condition because the results of Study 1 revealed that the no-evidence conditions were generally similar to the admissible conditions. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 402 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman Jury Instructions The jury instructions were simplified to emphasize the nullification versus legal components of the instructions. These key components were presented in bold-face type to draw participants’ attention to them. The nullification instructions, based on Maryland nullification instructions and materials used in previous research by Horowitz (1985, 1988), stated: Although you, as a jury, are a public body bound to give respectful attention to the laws, you have the final authority to decide whether or not to apply a given law, or the court’s interpretation of it, to the acts of the defendant. You represent your community, and it is appropriate to consider the feelings of the community, and your own feelings based on conscience, in reaching your decision. The law orientation instructions stated: The cornerstone of every nation is its justice system. Without a justice system, the important cultural values and moral standards of a community can not be upheld. Justice means protecting citizens and the public interest through the enforcement of laws. In order for justice to be carried out, it is important that all defendants are treated in a fair and equitable manner under the law. If jurors do not uphold the laws they are presented with, then it is impossible for the legal system to operate in a fair manner. You are not permitted to change the law, nor apply your own conception of what you think the law should be. Additionally, definitions for presumption of innocence and robbery were provided in the jury instructions. Verdict Form The primary dependant variable, participants’ verdicts, was measured as in Study 1, by combining the dichotomous verdict (guilty or not guilty) with the participant’s confidence rating. Impressions of the Case and Defendant The questions assessing the impressions of the case and the defendant were identical to Study 1. However, two questions were added to assess the effectiveness of the instruction manipulation. The questions asked, “Did the judge inform you that your personal feelings might be considered when rendering a verdict?” and “Did the judge inform you about the importance of following the law as it is presented to you?” Each answer was indicated on a 9-point Likert-type scale with endpoints of 1 (he definitely did not) and 9 (he definitely did). Results Manipulation Checks In order to check that participants were aware of the judge’s justice orientation instructions, we conducted a one-way ANOVA for justice orientation on each of the two manipulation check variables (judge’s emphasis of feelings and the judge’s emphasis of the law). The first analysis revealed a main effect of jury instructions on the item “Did the judge inform you that your personal feelings might be considered when P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 403 rendering a verdict,” F(1, 120) = 5.92, p < .02, η2 = .05 Nullification-oriented participants were more likely to indicate that they were told to consider their personal feelings compared to the law-oriented participants (Ms = 5.29 vs. 4.05, respectively). A second ANOVA conducted on responses to the question “Did the judge inform you about the importance of the following the law as it is presented to you?” also revealed a main effect for justice orientation, F(1, 120) = 6.08, p < .02, η2 = .05. Here law-oriented participants were significantly more certain that they had been told the importance of following the law, M = 7.69, compared to the responses of the nullification-oriented group, M = 6.77. These results suggest that the justice orientation manipulation was effective. Verdict Verdict scores were computed as in Study 1. A 2 (mortality salience vs. dental pain) × 2 (admissible vs. inadmissible evidence) × 2 (law vs. nullification orientation) ANOVA revealed a main effect for evidence, F(1, 117) = 8.13, p < .01, η2 = .07, which was qualified by the predicted three-way interaction between mortality salience, evidence, and justice orientation, F(1, 117) = 3.95, p < .05, η2 = .03. Following the strategy of Study 1, this interaction was further explored through separate ANOVAs within the justice orientation groups (see Figs. 5 and 6). For law-oriented participants, there was a main effect for evidence, F(1, 57) = 10.52, p < .01, η2 = .17. The means suggested that overall, law-oriented participants followed the evidence instructions, showing greater punitiveness in the admissible, M = 3.24, compared to inadmissible evidence conditions, M = −2.58. For nullification-oriented participants, however, the ANOVA revealed the predicted mortality salience by evidence interaction, F(1, 59) = 4.38, p < .05, η2 = .09. As in Study 1, mortality salient participants were more punitive in the admissible compared to the inadmissible condition, t(59) = 2.03, p < .05. However among dental pain participants, the means Fig. 5. Evidence × Salience interaction among law-oriented participants’ verdicts in Study 2. Note: Higher positive numbers reflect more confident guilty verdicts whereas higher negative numbers reflect more confident not guilty verdicts. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 404 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman Fig. 6. Evidence × Salience interaction among nullification-oriented participants’ verdicts in Study 2. Note: Higher positive numbers reflect more confident guilty verdicts whereas higher negative numbers reflect more confident not guilty verdicts. were in the direction of a backfire effect, although the difference was not significant, t(59) < 1. Looked at differently, whereas the nullification orientated mortality salient condition showed a nonsignificant trend to be less punitive than their dental pain counterparts when the evidence was inadmissible, t < 1, they were significantly more likely than their dental pain counterparts to vote guilty when the evidence was admissible, t(59) = 2.04, p < .05. Although the difference between the evidence conditions was not significant within the nullification-oriented dental pain conditions, it may be informative to note that participants in this condition showed the predicted effects relative to laworiented dental pain participants. That is, in the dental pain conditions, nullificationoriented participants tended to be less punitive than law-oriented participants in the admissible conditions, t = 1.65, p < .11, but significantly more punitive in the inadmissible conditions, t(59) = 2.02, p = .05. Supplemental Questions on Impressions of the Case We then assessed whether the interactive effects of the manipulations extended to participants’ perceptions of the case, as assessed with the questions after they rendered their verdicts. Two 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVAs on participants’ ratings of both the prosecution’s and defense’s case revealed main effects for evidence, both Fs > 6.89, ps ≤ .01, η2 s = .06, all other Fs < 2.22, ps > .13. The means indicate that overall, participants felt the prosecution’s case was stronger when the wiretapped evidence was admissible (M = 6.68) than when it was inadmissible (M = 5.84). Likewise, participants rated the defense’s case as stronger when the wiretap was inadmissible (M = 5.31) than when it was admissible (M = 4.37). Defendant Trait Ratings The trait ratings were averaged after reverse scoring the appropriate items so that higher scores reflected more positive evaluations of the defendant (α = .79). P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence 405 A 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA revealed only a marginal main effect for justice orientation, F(1, 120) = 3.22, p < .08, η2 = .03, suggesting a trend in which law-oriented participants rated the defendant more positively (M = 4.17) compared to nullificationoriented participants (M = 3.81), all other Fs < 1.48, ps > .22. Discussion The results of Study 2 generally replicate and extend the findings from Study 1. This study again uncovered a three-way interaction between mortality salience, evidence admissibility, and justice orientation on mock juror verdicts even though a markedly different operationalization of justice orientation was employed. More specifically, nullification-oriented participants tended to be more prone to a backfire effect in the dental pain conditions, a trend which mortality salience again significantly reversed. Mortality salient nullification-oriented participants were more punitive when the incriminating evidence was admissible than when it was inadmissible. Unlike in Study 1, however, there was only a main effect of admissibility on the supplemental questions about case perceptions. We return to this and other issues in the General Discussion section. GENERAL DISCUSSION The backfire effect, whereby people are more influenced by inadmissible than admissible evidence, is a phenomenon that can obviously have important implications for the integrity of the trial process. Thus, the present studies sought to explore how certain psychological motivations can operate in legal contexts to influence backfire effect manifestation. Drawing from previous terror management research showing that reminding participants of their mortality engenders motivations to uphold cultural worldviews, we reasoned that because most worldviews stipulate beliefs about fairness, part of the motivational profile evoked by mortality salience should lead participants to follow judicial instructions to ignore incriminating evidence. Moreover, we reasoned that both the backfire effect, and thus the mortality salience induced reversal, would be more prominent when participants approached the legal judgment using their personal intuitions about justice. The results of two studies offer support for this analysis. In Study 1, the backfire effect emerged among participants who were dispositionally prone to nullify the law and Study 2 generally showed the same pattern when participants were given judicial instructions to rely on their personal feelings. However, under these same conditions, reminding participants of their mortality led them to be more punitive when the incriminating evidence was admissible than when it was inadmissible. LIMITATIONS OF THE PRESENT STUDIES Although the present findings offer encouraging support for the relevance of psychological motivations in jury decision-making, there are ambiguities with the data that should be noted. Whereas the admissible conditions are similar in both P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] 406 pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman studies, within the inadmissible conditions the patterns are less clear. For example, in Study 1 it was only high nullification participants who were significantly less punitive in the inadmissible mortality salient condition. But in Study 2, mortality salient participants in the inadmissible conditions generally found the defendant less culpable regardless of the justice orientation. These differential findings may stem from different components of people’s worldview being activated by the judicial instructions, from participants’ difficulty in fully following the judicial instructions (see Lieberman & Sales, 1997), or some combination of both. In Study 1, the instructions were written in typical pattern instruction style, which often is dry and difficult to follow (Lieberman & Sales, 1997). Such language may have caused jurors to become bored and instead rely on their commonsense beliefs about justice (Finkel, 2000), that were then enhanced by mortality salience effects. However, for the purpose of Study 2, it was important that participants focused on the relevant instruction manipulation. As a result, dry and confusing language was removed, as were extraneous phrases. Thus, it may have been easier for jurors to understand and adhere to the instructions. If this was indeed the case, then it is not surprising that in Study 2 mortality salient inadmissible participants tended to have less guilty verdicts with both the nullification and the law-oriented instructions as the latter may have inadvertently emphasized procedural fairness. Clearly, further research is needed to clarify this issue, and thus, conclusions must be tempered with caution. However, given that the two-way interactions between evidence admissibility and judicial instruction were not significant and the patterns are generally consistent across studies, the present findings can be viewed as an informative beginning to understanding how psychological motivations can play a role in the backfire effect. It is also worth noting that whereas in Study 1 responses to the supplemental questions generally mirrored the verdict responses, in Study 2 there was no evidence that the supplemental questions tapped judgment domains that informed participants’ verdicts. Interestingly, in Study 2 the ratings of the prosecution’s and defense’s case were consistent with what “should” be the case if participants were following judicial instructions (i.e., perceiving the prosecution’s case as stronger and the defense’s case as weaker in the admissible versus inadmissible conditions). As the classic work of Nisbett and Wilson (1977) would suggest, it may be that participants lacked access to the cognitions that lead to their verdicts. In addition, this effect may also have emerged as a result of the manipulation check questions that directly preceded these judgments. That is, reminding participants of the judicial instructions may have primed them to think about evidence admissibility when rating their perceptions of the case. THE CONTRIBUTION OF TERROR MANAGEMENT TO UNDERSTANDING JURY DECISION-MAKING Terror management theory offers a broad account of human motivation and provides one avenue for understanding how often peripheral social psychological motivations may influence jury decision-making. Moreover, as shown by the present P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence 407 work, the theory informs how potentially discrepant motivations provoked by reminders of death can be reconciled in a legal context. On the one hand, mortality salience evokes the motivation to be more punitive toward someone who violates the worldview by breaking the law. On the other hand, there is also the motivation to uphold the law by following a judicial ruling. At least with the paradigm used in this research, the motivation to follow legal guidelines appeared to be the stronger of the two. As suggested by Lieberman et al. (2001), however, it is unlikely that people will always try to be oriented toward fairness after being reminded of death (cf. van den Bos & Miedema, 2000), and that a number of additional factors play a role in determining which defensive response will predominate. Previous terror management research suggests that mortality salience effects are often swayed by salient components of an individual’s worldview. Thus, in the present studies, the mortality salience induced desire for procedural fairness may have emerged because the mock-trial scenario participants were presented with created situations where fair treatment of the defendant became an important issue and there were minimal competing demands. It may be that in other situations, mortality salience may lead participants to respond in an especially punitive manner, even if they lack the legal justification to do so. For example, if the defendant was charged with a crime that produced great harm (e.g., aggravated assault, sexual assault, murder) individuals’ desire to punish the law breaker might override their procedural fairness concerns. Also, increased punitive reactions might be particularly likely in situations where the defendant is a threat to a critically important domain of an individual’s worldview, for example, if a Muslim was charged with a crime in the United States following the September 11th attacks. Legal scenarios may thus provide a way to tease apart important issues pertaining to different routes of psychological defense engaged by reminders of death. This may be especially relevant when one considers the potential for legal scenarios to activate concerns related to mortality. Although death-related cognitions are not systematically manipulated in trials, mortality salience may be inadvertently induced in a variety of ways in the course of legal proceedings (Arndt et al., 2003). For example, Judges (1999) suggests that the consideration of capital punishment is a mortality salience induction itself, leading to even more punitive reactions toward moral and legal transgressors. In addition, research (Cook, Arndt, & Lieberman, 2003; Pickel & Brown, 2002) has recently found that closing arguments in a simulated trial that remind participants of their mortality can increase punitive judgments in robbery, assault, and murder cases. One important direction for future research may be to further explore how existential motivations for psychological defense affect legal outcomes. Further, the application of terror management theory to legal settings could be a fruitful means to understand the consequences of death-related thought as such settings lend themselves to the juxtaposition of multiple motivational pressures. IMPLICATIONS FOR UNDERSTANDING THE BACKFIRE EFFECT In addition to exploring the motivations provoked by the awareness of death, the findings of Study 2 indicate that jury instructions emphasizing legal responsibil- P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 408 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman ity to uphold the law reduced the backfire effect whereas instructions emphasizing nullification powers did not. Much previous nullification research has focused on the effects of informing jurors of their nullification power on the application of substantive instructions (i.e., specific charges against the defendant). Extant studies have generally shown that jurors prudently use their nullification powers to acquit defendants whom they believe have violated the letter, but not the spirit, of the law (e.g., Horowitz, 1985, 1988; Niedermeier et al., 1999). The results of the present studies extend this line of research, and suggest that nullification instructions may also encourage jurors to rely on their personal feelings when considering procedural instructions. That is, jurors may be less likely to adhere to judicial admonitions after being given nullification instructions. Taken with the dispositional findings from Study 1, this suggests that future research could benefit from exploring such factors in reactions to different types of inadmissible evidence cases (Pickel, 1995). CONCLUSION The present research provides insight into two factors that may influence the backfire-effect–nullification-beliefs and mortality salience. We are certainly not suggesting that mortality salience be used in trials as a tool to encourage procedural compliance. However, on the basis of current findings as well as other research, there is reason to believe that reminders of death can be a useful empirical tool for exploring how psychological factors can impact legal judgments. Hopefully with this and further studies, continued progress can be made toward facilitating the most equitable administration of the law. ACKNOWLEDGMENT We thank Paula Emke, Marcus Johnson, Michelle Nard, and Alison Wooddell for their help with collecting the data. REFERENCES Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., & Cook, A. (2002). Mortality salience and the spreading activation of worldviewrelevant constructs: Exploring the cognitive architecture of terror management. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 307–324. Arndt, J., Lieberman, J. D., Cook, A., & Solomon, S. (2003). Death in the courtroom: Exploring the effects of mortality salience on legal decision-making. Manuscript under review, Columbia, MO. Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic Press. Broeder, D. W. (1959). The University of Chicago jury project. Nebraska Law Review, 88, 744–760. Cook, A., Arndt, J., & Lieberman, J. D. (2003). The influence of mortality salience induced by prosecutorial statements on juror verdicts. Manuscript in preparation. University of Missouri, Columbia. Cox, M., & Tanford, S. (1989). Effects of evidence and instructions in civil trials: An experimental investigation of rules of admissibility. Social Behaviour, 4, 31–55. Edwards, K., & Bryan, T. S. (1997). Judgmental biases produced by instructions to disregard: The (paradoxical) case of emotional information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 849– 864. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 Mortality Salience and Inadmissible Evidence 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 409 Fein, S., McCloskey, A., & Tomlinson, T. (1997). Can the jury disregard that information? The use of suspicion to reduce the prejudicial effects of pretrial publicity and inadmissible testimony. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1215–1226. Finkel, N. J. (2000). Commonsense justice and jury instructions: instructive and reciprocating connections Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 6, 591–628. Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1997). Fear of death and the judgement of social transgressions: A multidimensional of terror management theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 369– 380. Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S., et al. (1990). Evidence for terror management II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 308–318. Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror management theory of self-esteem and social behavior: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 61–139). New York: Academic Press. Harmon-Jones, E., Simon, L., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & McGregor, H. (1997). Terror management theory and self-esteem: Evidence that increased self-esteem reduced mortality salience effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 24–36. Horowitz, I. A. (1985). The effect of jury nullification instruction on verdicts and jury functioning in criminal trials. Law and Human Behavior, 9, 25–36. Horowitz, I. A. (1988). Jury nullification: The impact of judicial instructions, arguments, and challenges on jury decision making. Law and Human Behavior, 12, 439–453. Judges, D. P. (1999). Scared to death: Capital punishment as authoritarian terror management. University of California, Davis Law Review, 1–138. Lieberman, J. D., & Arndt, J. (2000). Understanding the limits of limiting instruction: Social psychological explanations for the failure of instructions to disregard pretrial publicity and other inadmissible evidence. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 6, 677–711. Lieberman, J. D., Arndt, J., Personious, J., & Cook, A. (2001). Vicarious annihilation: The effect of mortality salience on perceptions of hate crimes. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 547–566. Lieberman, J. D., & Sales, B. D. (1997). What social science teaches us about the jury instruction process. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 3, 1–56. Lieberman, J. D., & Silver, N. C. (2000). The effects of rational and experiential processing, judicial instructions, and personal beliefs on juror nullification. Poster session presented at the American Psychological-Law Society Biennial Conference, Las Vegas, NV, March, 2002. McGregor, H., Lieberman, J. D., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Simon, L., et al. (1998). Terror management and aggression: Evidence that mortality salience motivates aggression against worldview threatening others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 590–605. Niedermeier, K. E., Horowitz, I. A., & Kerr, N. L. (1999). Informing jurors of their nullification power: A route to a just verdict or judicial chaos? Law and Human Behavior, 23, 331–351. Nisbitt, R., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. Pickel, K. L. (1995). Inducing jurors to disregard inadmissible evidence: A legal explanation does not help. Law & Human Behavior, 19, 407–424. Pickel, K. L., & Brown, J. R. (2002). Mortality salience and jurors’ evaluations of criminal defendants: The effects of thinking about one’s own or another person’s death. Manuscript submitted for publication. Pyszczynski, T. A., Greenberg, J., Mack, D., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1981). Opening statements in a jury trial: The effect of promising more than the evidence can show. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 11, 434–444. Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory I: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681–690. Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1991). A terror management theory of social behavior: The psychological functions of self-esteem and cultural worldviews. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 93–159) New York: Academic Press. Thompson, W. C., Fong, G. T., & Rosenhan, D. C. (1981). Inadmissible evidence and juror verdicts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 453–463. van den Bos, K. (2001). Reactions to perceived fairness: The impact of mortality salience and self-esteem on ratings of negative affect. Social Justice Research, 14, 1–23. van den Bos, K., & Miedema, J. (2000). Toward understanding why fairness matters: The influence of mortality salience on reactions to procedural fairness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 355–366. P1: KVK Law and Human Behavior [lahu] 410 pp1287-lahu-491191 July 19, 2004 19:9 Style file version June 4th, 2002 Cook, Arndt, and Lieberman Wiener, R. L., Habert, K., Shkodriani, G., & Staebler, C. (1991). The social psychology of jury nullification: Predicting when jurors disobey the law. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 1379– 1401. Wolf, S., & Montgomery, D. A. (1977). Effects of inadmissible evidence and level of judicial admonishment to disregard on the judgments of mock jurors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 7, 205– 219. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.