Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Sleep Medicine 7 (2006) 513–520 www.elsevier.com/locate/sleep Original article Chronic fatigue, unrefreshing sleep and nocturnal polysomnography Christian Guilleminault a,*, Dalva Poyares a,b, Agostinho da Rosa b, Ceyda Kirisoglu a, Tatiana Almeida b, Maria Cecilia Lopes a a Stanford University Sleep Disorders Clinic, 401 Quarry road, suite 3301, Stanford, CA 94305, USA Department of Psychobiology, Sleep Institute, Federal University of Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo, Brazil b Received 2 January 2006; received in revised form 28 February 2006; accepted 6 March 2006 Abstract Background and purpose: To investigate the complaint of unrefreshing sleep with study of sleep electroencephalogram (EEG) in patients with chronic fatigue. Patients and methods: Fourteen successively seen patients (mean age: 41.1 9.8) who complained of chronic fatigue but denied sleepiness and agreed to participate were compared to 14 controls (33.6G10.2 years) who were monitored during sleep recorded in parallel. After performing conventional sleep scoring we applied Fast Fourier Transformation (FFT) for the delta 1, delta 2, theta, alpha, sigma 1, sigma 2, beta EEG frequency bands. The presence of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep instability was studied with calculation of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) rate. Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to analyze FFT results and Mann–Whitney U-test to compare CAP rate in both groups of subjects. Results: Slow wave sleep (SWS) percentage and sleep efficiency were lower, but there was a significant increase in delta 1 (slow delta) relative power in the chronic fatigue group when compared to normals (P!0.01). All the other frequency bands were proportionally and significantly decreased compared to controls. CAP rate was also significantly greater in subjects with chronic fatigue than in normals (PZ 0.04). An increase in respiratory effort and nasal flow limitation were noted with chronic fatigue. Conclusions: The complaints of chronic fatigue and unrefreshing sleep were associated with an abnormal CAP rate, with increase in slow delta power spectrum, affirming the presence of an abnormal sleep progression and NREM sleep instability. These specific patterns were related to subtle, undiagnosed sleep-disordered breathing. q 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Chronic fatigue; Unrefreshing sleep; Slow wave sleep; Cyclic alternating pattern; Slow delta; Sleep-disordered breathing 1. Introduction Complaints of chronic fatigue are common in general medical clinics, and relating the complaint to a specific medical entity is often difficult. Effort has been made to define ‘Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS)’ to better investigate these patients. CFS is estimated to affect approximately 0.2–0.7% of the population in Western countries [1] and up to 800,000 Americans [2]. It is a medically unexplained illness and represents a socio-economical burden [2]. In association with the complaint of fatigue, sleep, musculoskeletal and somatic symptoms [3,4] are the most prevalent symptoms in CFS. * Corresponding author. Tel.: C1 650 723 6601; fax: C1 650 725 89 10. E-mail address: [email protected] (C. Guilleminault). 1389-9457/$ - see front matter q 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2006.03.016 Despite these clear symptoms, the pathophysiology underlying the complaint of chronic fatigue, or the so-called ‘chronic fatigue syndrome’ is still unclear. Immune system activation [5–8], neurocognitive and mental alterations [4,9,10], evidence of oxidative stress [8,11] and involvement of hypothalamic–pituitary axes (HPA) have also been described [1,5,6,12,13]. However, none of these findings are sufficient to fully explain the symptoms of chronic fatigue. Even the HPA alterations seem to be multifactorial and are not the primary cause of CFS. Rather they appear to be a consequence of the persistence of the symptomatology linked to CFS [12]. A recent study showed that the most prevalent complaint among subjects with idiopathic chronic fatigue is unrefreshing sleep [14]; however, using a Sleep Disorders Questionnaireq (SDQ), the study did not find any increased association between the complaint of chronic fatigue and excessive daytime somnolence or sleep apnea. The reported 514 C. Guilleminault et al. / Sleep Medicine 7 (2006) 513–520 prevalence of an association between an undiagnosed primary sleep disorder and the diagnosis of CFS varies between 0 and 50% [14,15]. Fisher [15] and Unger [14] emphasized the importance of excluding a treatable primary sleep disorder when evaluating patients with the complaint of chronic fatigue. Furthermore, clear controversies exist regarding the presence of specific abnormal sleep patterns in patients presenting with unexplained chronic fatigue. Several studies have described a sleep electroencephalogram (EEG) pattern known as ‘alpha–delta’ sleep (better called ‘delta–alpha’ based on successive appearance of the EEG frequencies) in patients suffering from CFS [16,17]. However, many believe that the ‘delta–alpha’ sleep pattern is not a specific marker of CFS [18] but instead is a non-specific pattern [19], and its association with CFS has been questioned [20,21]. Patients seen in a subdivision of rheumatology dealing with the complaint of unexplained chronic fatigue were regularly referred for objective investigation of their sleep once all other possible etiologies for their complaint of chronic fatigue had been ruled out. Clinical sleep evaluation obtained for these patients did not reveal any abnormalities that could explain their subjective complaints. The only consistent finding in the polysomnograph recordings have been a decrease in slow wave sleep (SWS), despite the fact that an increase in stage 4 NREM sleep has been mentioned in at least one report [22], but this finding is non-specific. We have performed several investigations using the cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) scoring system in other patients with abnormal SWS findings. Based on these preliminary clinical findings of the other patients with abnormal slow wave sleep, this study was designed as a prospective investigation of the potential relationship between the complaint of chronic fatigue, unrefreshing sleep and abnormal slow wave sleep (SWS). The study was based on one night of polysomnography and specifically evaluated SWS. Prior to this study we performed several investigations using the CAP scoring system in patients with abnormal SWS findings. The goal of this study was to compare the CAP rate seen in patients with fatigue to matched controls and to explore the relationship between CAP pattern and quantitative EEG measured during the same night as one of the central EEG derivations (C4/A1). This report presents the results of this investigation on subjects referred to the sleep clinic with an unexplained complaint of chronic fatigue and unrefreshing sleep, who agreed to undergo all steps of the investigation. All who enrolled signed informed consent forms for anonymous usage of their data for research. 2. Methods 2.1. Subjects Fourteen patients (3 men and 11 women), with a mean age of 41.1G9.8, who had been evaluated by the specialized rheumatologic clinic and found to have unexplained chronic fatigue, were referred for sleep investigation. They were compared to 14 controls (3 men and 11 women) aged 33.6G 10.2-year-old (PZ0.06) recruited from the community; the controls were professionals and had a socio-economic background that was similar to those presented by the patients. Controls were matched for gender and age as closely as possible. The need to monitor patients and controls nearly simultaneously to avoid effect of seasonal change with variable light intensity, time change, respiratory allergy, school schedule for children, etc. was in part responsible for a small discrepancy in age between groups. Controls were excluded if they had a complaint of chronic fatigue or suffered from a sleep disorder. Additionally, controls had to report that they were in good health and took no medications, and sign the informed consent form. 2.2. Evaluation Patients and controls underwent the same evaluation. They all filled out standard questionnaires, including the Epworth sleepiness scale (ESS) [23] and the fatigue severity scale (FFS) [23]. The former is a scale with eight questions and four possible responses evaluating degree of sleepiness in specific situations. The latter is a validated fatigue scale designed to assess functional outcomes related to fatigue. It is a 9-item scale scored from 1 to 7, where 1 indicates no impairment and 7 indicates severe impairment. Its clinical utility, reliability and validation criteria have been reported. Higher scores suggest greater fatigue [23]. Fatigue in CFS patients and in disorders of sleep continuity have been associated with increased scores on the FFS [24,25]. Sleep logs indicating bed time, wake time, and sleep interruption were obtained for 15 days prior to the recording and included 2 weekend or days off each week. Patients were given pre-stamped envelopes and were asked to mail their sleep logs every 3 days. Medical data obtained from evaluation at the chronic fatigue/fibromyalgia clinic in the rheumatology division was reviewed, and blood tests were repeated if they had been obtained more than 2 months prior to the sleep clinic investigation. All patients and controls had normal blood tests, and no evidence of infections or immunologic disorders. 2.3. PSG recording Every subject went to bed at the usual bedtime and had a minimum of 7 1/2 h of PSG recordings. The following sleep variables were collected and stored using a dedicated computerized 32-channel sleep system with a sampling rate of 256 Hz per channel: a total of 20 EEG leads, two electrooculogram (EOG) channels, two electromyogram (EMG) channels (chin and both legs), and an electrocardiogram (ECG) channel. Respiration was monitored with a nasal cannula-pressure transducer system, mouth thermocouple, C. Guilleminault et al. / Sleep Medicine 7 (2006) 513–520 two channels for chest and abdominal efforts with calibrated inductive respiratory plethysmography, esophageal pressure (Pes), neck microphone, and pulse oximetry with a Nellcore (Alameda, CA, USA) oximeter. Standard evening and morning questionnaires were filled out before lights-out time and after lights-on. 2.4. EEG fast Fourier transformation (FFT) analysis Once visual scoring had been performed, EEG spectral analysis was applied on the C4-A1 channel, with a sampling rate of 256 Hz per channel. Visual inspection eliminated movement artifacts and awakening segments longer than 10 s. Absolute and relative power was assessed for each classical EEG frequency bands, defined by delta 1 (0.25– 2 Hz), delta 2 (2.25–4 Hz), theta (4.25–8) Hz, alpha (8.25– 12) Hz, sigma 1 (12.25–14), sigma 2 (14.25–16), and beta (16–20). Results were given in relative power. The FFTs were performed on averaged, non-overlapping, four-second windows, with a Hanning window, free of artifacts and arousals O10 s of EEG segments during the total PSG recording. The window results were averaged to obtain a 60s value, and analysis covered the total sleep. 2.5. Data analysis 2.5.1. Scoring criteria All records were to be scored following the international sleep scoring criteria of Rechtshaffen and Kales [26]. Short EEG arousals and respiratory events were also defined using the American Sleep Disorders Association (ASDA) and American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) recommendations [27,28]. Monitoring of respiratory variables allowed recognition and typification of sleep apneas and hypopneas. A hypopnea is defined as a decrease of nasal cannula pressure transducer signal by at least 30% associated with an oxygen drop of 3% or an EEG arousal of 3 s or longer. Flow limitation is defined as a drop of nasal flow compared to prior recording between 3 and 30% with a ‘flattening’ of the curve as described, for at least four breaths, associated with changes in Pes. An abnormal respiratory effort was identified with the presence of Pes crescendos or continuous sustained effort, terminated by an abrupt return to normal effort which is referred to as a Pes reversal [29]. Visual scoring of sleep stages and recognition of respiratory events were performed blindly on both patients and controls. This scoring process also included identification of all sleep phenomena such as body or leg movements. The different respiratory criteria allowed scoring of an apnea–hypopnea index (AHI) based on the calculation of apneas and hypopneas per hour of sleep, and a respiratory disturbance index (RDI) with integration of flow limitation events, respiratory event-related arousal, and Pes events terminated by a Pes reversal [29]. 515 2.5.2. Cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) Automatic detection of CAP [30,31] was applied to C4A1 and visually corrected, based on the international atlas rules [32]. A CAP [31,33] is formed by electrocortical events indicated by periodic EEG activity occurring during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, and characterized by transitory events that recur at regular intervals in the range of seconds during NREM sleep. These events are clearly distinct from the background EEG rhythm as abrupt frequency shifts or amplitude changes. Two phases (A and B) are present that are part of a CAP cycle and recur within 2–60 s. When none of the phases (A and B) are identifiable, sleep has reached a new stable state [32,33]. Phase A is identified by transient events typically observed in NREM sleep. It includes EEG patterns of higher voltage, slower frequency and faster-lower voltage than the background EEG (with an increase in amplitude by at least 1/3 compared to the background EEG). Phase A has been subdivided into three subtypes based on the presence in association with high amplitude slow waves (delta waves) and the presence of faster and lower amplitude activity that can mimic for phase A3 what has been described as delta–alpha EEG pattern. Phase B, which follows, is defined by an EEG pattern of less EEG amplitude with EEG figures of stages 1, 2 NREM sleep. If there is difficulty reaching a new stable state during NREM sleep passing from wake to REM sleep, the CAP rate will increase with the greater the number of cycles (i.e. the greater the number of Phases A and B, or CAP rate) signifying a greater instability of NREM sleep. Phase A was analyzed as a whole; i.e. subtypes of phase A were not considered in this study. Main sleep EEG patterns recognized in this detection were delta bursts, also called delta-cluster, sequences of K-complexes, vertex sharp wave, and polyphasic bursts of slow and fast frequencies. The parameters analyzed were CAP rate (time occupied by CAP sequences over total NREM sleep time expressed in percent) and CAP time. 2.6. Statistical analysis One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to compare sleep stages and demographic parameters between fatigue and controls. Comparison of relative and absolute power between patients and controls was done with t-test, and comparison of CAP rate and CAP time with Mann–Whitney U-test (after Kolmogorov–Smirnov test). 3. Results 3.1. Patient history, subjective complaints, sleep logs, and scales Social variables: all patients and controls worked outside of the home. Three women in the patient group and four in the controls worked part-time only. Three chronic fatigue 516 C. Guilleminault et al. / Sleep Medicine 7 (2006) 513–520 and three control subjects had no children. There was a difference between the two groups in the age of the patients’ children, with the youngest child in the chronic fatigue group being 9-year-old compared to the youngest child being 2-year-old in the control group. Overall the youngest child in the family tended to be younger in controls than in chronic fatigue patients, despite the fact that if all ages of children were tabulated no significant difference between groups could be shown, as large SD existed. Chronic fatigue patients had complaints of continuous fatigue during the day, which impaired their social and professional life and had been present for a mean of 6G4.3 years (range 2–12 years). This fatigue was present upon awakening and was associated with a feeling of unrefreshing sleep. Fatigue was present on weekdays as well as weekends and did not diminish after a longer time was spent in bed, which usually occurred on weekends. On their 15-day sleep logs, chronic fatigue patients reported 7.38 hG24 min of time in bed during weekdays but spent as much as 12 h in bed on weekends. The reported time in bed during weekdays was not significantly different in chronic fatigue patients than controls (7.14 hG35 min). However, on weekends, time spent in bed was much greater in patients with fatigue than in controls (mean difference on weekend: 190G29 min). Eleven chronic fatigue subjects had a complaint of nocturnal disrupted sleep, and sleep logs indicated a mean of 1.8G.9 episodes of awakening during sleep. The mean duration of these reported awakenings was 14G9 min. None of the chronic fatigue patients considered themselves an ‘insomniac’, but six of them mentioned muscle pain at awakening. Four had nocturia, but related it to a prior awakening. With regard to their complaint of fatigue, 12 chronic fatigue patients reported difficulty concentrating during the daytime and at work. Seven of them believed that their fatigue was responsible for limitation in job performance, and all reported great curtailment in social activities due to their symptoms. None of the patients smoked and 8/14 reported they did not consume alcohol. The remaining six patients stated they consumed alcohol only on social occasions, with one alcoholic beverage every 4–6 weeks. Caffeine intake was variable but always limited to morning hours and was usually limited to 1–2 cups of coffee or tea. Chronic fatigue patients had all previously consulted internists in search of a solution for their problems, and had tried to live as ‘healthy’ as possible with regular schedules to improve their condition. None of the patients took any medications for the prior 10 months. Chronic fatigue patients who had tried pharmacotherapy in the past (10/14) had used polyvitamins, or specific vitamin supplements, preparation such as ATP, 5-HTP, calcium or magnesium supplements. No benefit was derived from any of these therapies and all had been discontinued. In addition, 6/14 had previously been placed on a low dosage of a Tricyclics or selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) from their general practitioners for symptomatic treatment. Two men had undergone treatment with a low dosage of testosterone for unclear reasons, as their endocrinologic evaluation was normal. Despite the variety of pharmaceutical agents, none of these medications had any positive effect on symptoms. As mentioned, all medication intakes had been stopped many months before the referral. Krupp et al.’s FSS scores [24] were 4.9G1.2 and 2.1G 0.8, P!0.01 for fatigue patients versus controls, indicating the presence of abnormal fatigue in chronic fatigue patients. Though two women with chronic fatigue had signs and symptoms for fibromyalgia, they did not fulfill the examination criteria for this condition. At clinical evaluation, body mass index (BMI) was similar for both groups (24.6G2.4; 22.1G4.5 kg/m2, fatigue and controls, respectively; PZ0.14). No explanation for chronic fatigue had been found after in-depth evaluations at the rheumatologic clinic, which included normal endocrine and immunological tests. 3.2. Sleep, sleep stages and, respiratory parameters Pre- and post-sleep questionnaires obtained in the laboratory indicated that all chronic fatigue patients felt that their day had been of poor quality, with constant fatigue, which impaired their professional activities. In the morning, these patients reported the presence of awakenings which matched polysomnography findings. In addition, they reported very unrefreshing sleep with significant tiredness which was equal or greater to what they felt at the previous bedtime. Despite the impression of their sleep being of poor quality, none complained of ‘insomnia’. Sleep stage analyses: as shown in Table 1, patients with complaint of chronic fatigue had a significantly lower percentage of Stages 3, 4 NREM sleep, lower sleep efficiency, and a similar AHI (0.9G0.6 versus 0.7G0.9, pZns) but higher RDI compared to normal subjects (Table 1). Table 1 Main PSG data and Epworth sleepiness score for fatigue and control subjects PSG parameters RDI Lowest O2 Saturation TST (min) Sleep efficiency REM sleep (%) Stages 3, 4 NREM sleep (%) Arousal index/ hour of sleep Epworth sleepiness scale Fatigue (nZ14) Controls (nZ14) F(1,27) P 4.7G3.3a 92.6G2.8 1.0G1.4 92.0G2.1 11.8 0.34 0.01 n.s. 398.8G72 84.1G9.4a 17.2G6.1 13.9G7.2a 377.2G75.5 90.6G4.6 19.4G4.7 25.4G6.7 2.8 4.2 0.88 15.1 n.s. 0.06 n.s. 0.01 9.5G3.8 9.3G5.2 0.006 n.s. 10.3G4.7 8.9G2.4 0.37 n.s. RDI, respiratory disturbance index; TST, total sleep time; Fatigue: chronic fatigue patients; Number of subject involved: 28. a One-way ANOVA. C. Guilleminault et al. / Sleep Medicine 7 (2006) 513–520 Table 2 CAP parameters and NREM sleep duration for chronic fatigue and control subjects CAP parameters Fatigue (nZ14) CAP time (min) CAP rate 171.1G89.3a Slow wave sleep (min) NREM sleep (min) Controls (nZ14) U P 91.3G97.6 39 0.04 50.9G32.0a 27.0G26.2 55.43G28.7b 95.8G25.3 37.5 F(1,26) 15.1 0.04 P 0.01 331.5G68.3 304.0G46.3 1.4 n.s. CAP, cyclic alternating pattern; fatigue: chronic fatigue patients; total number of subjects, 28. a Mann–Whitney U-test. b One-way ANOVA. 3.3. CAP parameters CAP time and CAP rate were significantly higher in chronic fatigue patients despite similar NREM sleep time (as measured in minutes) in both subject groups. (Table 2) Further phase A analyses were not performed. 3.4. Sleep EEG spectral power frequencies There was a significant increase in slow delta (delta 1) power in chronic fatigue patients compared to normal controls as shown in Tables 3A and 3B. This was found when considering relative and absolute power (Tables 3A and 3B and Fig. 1). 4. Discussion We studied subjects complaining of unexplained fatigue and unrefreshing sleep lasting for several years. In many reports, patients with ‘chronic fatigue syndrome’ have normal sleep including sleep stage distribution by polysomnography despite their sleep complaints [34]. Others have reported sleep stage changes, with increase in stage 2 NREM sleep and decreases or increases in slow wave sleep [22,35,36]. A sleep pattern called ‘delta–alpha’ has also been Table 3A Sleep EEG frequency bands relative power EEG frequencies Fatigue (nZ13) Controls (nZ14) P Delta 1 (%) Delta 2 (%) Theta (%) Alpha (%) Sigma 1 (%) Sigma 2 (%) Beta (%) 0.74G0.14a 0.15G0.07a 0.07G0.04a 0.02G0.03a 0.01G0.02a 0.01G0.01a 0.003G0.009a 0.47G0.18 0.26G0.09 0.12G0.04 0.06G0.03 0.04G0.03 0.03G0.09 0.01G0.01 !0.01 !0.01 !0.01 !0.01 !0.01 !0.01 !0.01 t-Test; Total number of subjects, nZ27 (one CD-ROM with recording data unreadable). a Significantly different with t-Test analysis. 517 Table 3B Sleep EEG frequency bands absolute power (mV2) EEG frequencies Delta 1 Delta 2 Theta Alpha Sigma 1 Sigma 2 Beta Fatigue (nZ13) a 1459.3G1113.2 294.0G154.1 89.3G41.8 41.7G18.0 35.0G24.8 22.3G17.6 6.7G3.3 Controls (nZ14) P 516.7G394.2 247.5G129.7 127.57G59 57.33G24.75 37.0G20 16.7G13.2 14.36G7.05a !0.01 n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. !0.01 t-Test (one CD with recording data unreadable, nZ27). Fatigue, chronic fatigue patients. a Significantly different with t-Test analysis. associated with fibromyalgia and fatigue, but publications reporting it have been criticized due to lack of specificity of this pattern [19]. The real issue, we believe, is not the specificity of the polysomnographic pattern: It is the fact that patients with chronic fatigue syndrome have an abnormal sleep EEG pattern of shorter duration than the one used for sleep staging. This abnormal sleep EEG pattern is not recognized by the conventional scoring methods of polysomnography, and critics of the finding should have raised the question of the underlying problems behind the presence of delta–alpha sleep disturbances [14]. Our study addressed the question of unrefreshing sleep using new tools. None of our patients had secondary gain to their claim of fatigue, and all were greatly handicapped by their syndrome. These patients tried to treat their fatigue by staying in bed on weekends and days off, despite the negative impact it had on their social and family lives. They denied ‘sleepiness’, and their ESS was borderline normal. Several of the sleep logs showed some nocturnal sleep disruption, though all patients denied having ‘insomnia’, defined as difficulty falling asleep at sleep onset or difficulty returning to sleep if awakened during the night. The major complaint of the patients was of ‘unrefreshing sleep’ and a continuous feeling of fatigue that was described as ‘exhaustion’ by a few, which was already present at the time of morning awakening. In our study the subjective investigations did not yield important findings. Sleep staging analyses did not account for the reported fatigue. Though there were subtle abnormalities, a decrease in sleep efficiency and a decrease in stages 3, 4 NREM sleep (SWS) compared to normal controls as already mentioned by others [36], these findings are interesting but are not specific. Additionally, experiments with short-term sleep deprivation and recovery have shown that sleep duration appears to be more important for daytime alertness than SWS content [37] and total sleep time, which were similar in our fatigue patients and controls. We also failed to demonstrate alterations in sleep consolidation and fragmentation parameters. Our fatigue patients had similar arousal indices compared to normals. Notably, this study did not find an increase in alpha EEG frequencies (fast frequencies), primary components of delta–alpha pattern and of arousals from sleep. The lack of sleep fragmentation may explain 518 C. Guilleminault et al. / Sleep Medicine 7 (2006) 513–520 Fig. 1. EEG relative power in fatigue (nZ13) and controls subjects (nZ14). Significant differences are indicated in Table 3A: all bands were significantly different between controls and patients (P!0.01). why these patients were not sleepy during the daytime but complained only of chronic fatigue. However, the CAP analyses and quantitative EEG analyses changes were significant compared to controls. These changes suggest one should analyze the polysomnograms of patients complaining of fatigue differently. In our chronic fatigue patients, there was a significant increase in slow delta power despite an overall decrease in SWS compared to normals. Some of the findings are close to those observed with parasomnias [38–42], including an increase in CAP rate and CAP time as observed here. Such increases are associated with complaint of unrefreshing sleep and sleep disorders [31,33,43]. In our analysis we did not dissociate the different phases A of CAP because we believe that the important question here is not related to types of phase A but instead to the following: why are the phases A interrupted and why is there a return to phase B instead of a normal progression toward consolidated SWS? The presence of an increase in slow delta indicates presence of ‘delta burst’ or ‘delta-cluster waves’ [44]; however, the decrease in overall delta power and slow wave sleep indicates that the ‘delta stage’ cannot be maintained during NREM sleep, leading to ‘instability of NREM sleep’ as emphasized by Terzano et al. [32,33]. Instability of NREM sleep has been seen with a variety of different syndromes and is a non-specific finding but raises the question of why such instability is present. We believe NREM sleep instability suggests an impairment of the normal homeostatic process. Increase in CAP rate is associated with the presence of increased respiratory effort and of flow limitation events as shown with nasal cannula and esophageal pressure measurements. With these combined recording techniques, which are not routinely used in most sleep laboratories, we found a significant increase in the RDI (an index very different from the well-known AHI calculated in obstructive sleep apnea) in chronic fatigue subjects compared to controls. The main complaint of patients, particularly premenopausal women, with upper airway resistance syndrome (UARS [45]) is ‘chronic fatigue’. The difference between patients labeled ‘chronic fatigue’ or UARS appears mainly dependant on the referral route of patients rather than their clinical complaint. Initially, UARS patients were sent to the sleep clinic because of snoring, but it is well-known that UARS may be present without snoring [45]. The main complaint of patients in this study was ‘chronic fatigue’, which led them to be referred and evaluated by rheumatologists. Despite the diagnosis ‘chronic fatigue’ or ‘UARS’ both groups of patients share similar sleep disturbances that result in similar consequences. There are now a series of reports that show abnormal breathing without obstructive apneas, and hypopneas may be associated with changes in sleep EEG. Chervin et al. showed that in both children and adults [46,47] EEG changes may be seen with each abnormal breath and each increase in respiratory effort by utilizing a much more sophisticated technique than visual sleep stage scoring. There is now an array of reports that demonstrate the importance of intact sensory input from the oro-pharynx and upper larynx to avoid the development of the obstructive sleep apnea syndrome [48]. The presence of a local neuropathy appears to be associated with OSAS, but persistence of normal local sensory input seems to be inducing UARS, with its cortege of daytime fatigue [48], abnormal CAP rate and changes of sleep EEG at quantitative EEG analyses. In 1999, Lentz et al. showed that SWS sleep disruption resulting in an increase in fatigue and a decrease in pain threshold [49]. Vergara et al. [50] reported that an important predictor of subjective quality of sleep and sleep depth is related to delta maximum power measured with quantitative EEG analysis. Instability of NREM sleep is associated with changes in delta power from the first to the last NREM– REM cycle as demonstrated in investigation of chronic sleepwalkers [42]. Subtle sleep-disordered breathing (UARS type) leads to these changes in delta power. The challenges now are to understand why similar breathing disorders during sleep lead to different clinical presentations, why subjects with similar sleep disturbances have C. Guilleminault et al. / Sleep Medicine 7 (2006) 513–520 different types of parasomnia, or just a complaint of chronic fatigue. Could it be that the type of anatomical problem presented by these patients (nasal impairment versus base of tongue impairment, versus pharyngeal impairment) plays a role on the severity of the sleep EEG disturbance? Is an increase in nasal resistance more detrimental in creating an anxiety response than a base of tongue narrowing? These are unanswered questions but are important to consider as the types and number of sensory receptors are very different depending on the region of the upper airway that is the primary site of narrowing. The projections of these sensors and their association with certain regions of the brain may also be important: again an abnormality in the nose passage may not have the same impact as one behind the base of tongue. There will also be the need to explore whether subtle disruption of other biological functions leads to some subtle disturbances of sleep and complaints of chronic fatigue due to the impact on SWS and appearance of NREM sleep instability. In the recent past, the use of investigative approaches such as quantitative EEG, CAP scoring, or complex algorithms such as the one used by Chervin et al. [46,47], have demonstrated that much more information could be extracted from the sleep EEG of patients with complaint of ‘fatigue’, and these approaches may reveal much more than simple sleep staging. References [1] Evengard B, Klimas N. Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: probable pathogenesis and possible treatments. Drugs 2002;62:2433–46. [2] Reynolds KJ, Vernon SD, Bouchery E, Reeves WC. The economic impact of chronic fatigue syndrome. Cost Eff Resour Alloc 2004;2:4. [3] Nisenbaum R, Jones JF, Unger ER, et al. A population-based study of the clinical course of chronic fatigue syndrome. Health Qual Life Outcomes 2003;03:1–49. [4] Nisenbaum R, Reyes M, Unger ER, Reeves WC. Factor analysis of symptoms among subjects with unexplained chronic fatigue: what can we learn about chronic fatigue syndrome? J Psychosom Res 2004;56: 171–8. [5] Jason LA, Holbert C, Torres-Harding S, et al. Chronic fatigue syndrome versus neuroendocrineimmune dysfunction syndrome: differential attributions. J Health Soc Policy 2003;18:43–55. [6] Papanicolaou DA, Amsterdam JD, Levine S, et al. Neuroendocrine aspects of chronic fatigue syndrome. Neuroimmunomodulation 2004; 11:65–74. [7] Skowera A, Cleare A, Blair D, et al. High levels of type 2 cytokineproducing cells in chronic fatigue syndrome. Clin Exp Immunol 2004; 135:294–302. [8] Staines DR. Is chronic fatigue syndrome an autoimmune disorder of endogenous neuropeptides, exogenous infection and molecular mimicry? Med Hypotheses 2004;2:646–52. [9] Busichio K, Tiersky LA, Deluca J, Natelson BH. Neuropsychological deficits in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10:278–85. [10] De Lange FP, Kalkman JS, Bleijenberg G, et al. Neural correlates of the chronic fatigue syndrome–an fMRI study. Brain 2004;127:1948–1957. [11] Richards RS, Roberts TK, McGregor NR, et al. Blood parameters indicative of oxidative stress are associated with symptom expression in chronic fatigue syndrome. Redox Rep 2000;5:35–41. 519 [12] Cleare AJ. The HPA axis and the genesis of chronic fatigue syndrome. Trends Endocrinol Metab 2004;15:55–9. [13] Crofford LJ, Young EA, Engleberg NC, et al. Basal circadian and pulsatile ACTH and cortisol secretion in patients with fibromyalgia and/or chronic fatigue syndrome. Brain Behav Immun 2004;18: 314–25. [14] Unger ER, Nisenbaum R, Moldofsky H, et al. Sleep assessment in a population-based study of chronic fatigue syndrome. BMC Neurol 2004;4:6. [15] Fischler B. Review of clinical and psycholobiological dimensions of the chronic fatigue syndrome: differentiatation from depression and contribution to sleep dysfunctions. Sleep Med Rev 1999;3:133–46. [16] Moldofsky H. Non-restorative sleep and symptoms after a febrile illness in patients with fibrositis and chronic fatigue syndrome. J Rheumatol 1989;16:150–3. [17] Whelton CL, Salit I, Moldofsky H. Sleep, Epstein-Barr virus infection, musculoskeletal pain, and depressive symptoms in chronic fatigue syndrome. J Rheumatol 1992;19:939–43. [18] Manu P, Lane TJ, Matthews DA, et al. Alpha–delta sleep in patients with a chief complaint of chronic fatigue. South Med J 1994;87: 465–70. [19] Mahowald ML, Mahowald MW. Nighttime and daytime functioning (sleepiness and fatigue) in less well-defined chronic rheumatic diseases with particular reference to the ‘alpha–delta NREM sleep anomaly’. Sleep Med 2000;1:195–207. [20] Mahowald MW, Mahowald ML, Bundlie SR, Ytterberg SR. Sleep fragmentation in rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 1989;32: 974–83. [21] Flanigan MJ, Morehouse RL, Shapiro CM. Determination of observer-rated alpha activity during sleep. Sleep 1995;18:702–6. [22] Zubieta JK, Demitrack MA, Shipley JE, et al. in chronic fatigue syndrome: comparison with major depression. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 33:73A–74. [23] Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 1991;14:540–5. [24] Krupp LB, LaRocca NG, Muir-Nash J, Steinberg AD. The fatigue severity scale. Application to patients with multiple sclerosis and systemic lupus erythematosus. Arch Neurol 1989;46:1121–3. [25] Morriss RK, Wearden AJ, Battersby L. The relationship of sleep difficulties to fatigue, mood and disability in chronic fatigue syndrome. J Psychosom Res 1997;42:597–605. [26] Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A Manual of standardized terminology, technique and scoring system for sleep stages of human sleep. Los Angeles, CA: Los Angeles Brain Information Service, Brain Information Institute; 1968. [27] American Sleep Disorders Association. EEG arousals: scoring rules and examples. A preliminary report from Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association. Sleep 1992;15: 173–84. [28] American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Sleep-Related Breathing Disorders in Adults: Recommendations for syndrome definition and measurement techniques in clinical research. The report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Task Force. Sleep 1999;22: 667–89. [29] Guilleminault C, Poyares D, Palombini L, et al. Variability of respiratory effort in relationship with sleep stages in normal controls and Upper Airway Resistance Syndrome patients. Sleep Med 2001;2: 397–406. [30] Rosa AC, Parrino L, Terzano MG. Automatic detection of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) sequences in sleep: preliminary results. Clin Neurophysiol 1999;110(4):585–92. [31] Terzano MG, Parrino L, Boselli M, et al. Polysomnographic analysis of arousal responses in obstrutive sleep apnea syndrome by means of the cyclic alternating pattern. Clin Neurophysiol 1996;13:145–55. [32] Terzano MG, Parrino L, Sherieri A, et al. Atlas, rules, and recording techniques for the scoring of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep. Sleep Med 2002;3:187–99. 520 C. Guilleminault et al. / Sleep Medicine 7 (2006) 513–520 [33] Terzano MG, Parrino L, Spaggiari MC. CAP variables and arousals as sleep electroencephalogram markers for primary insomnia. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:1715–23. [34] Sharpley A, Clements A, Hawton K, Sharpe M. Do patients with ‘pure’ chronic fatigue syndrome (neurasthenia) have abnormal sleep? Psychosom Med 1997;59:592–6. [35] Fischler B, Le Bon O, Hoffmann G, et al. Sleep anomalies in the chronic fatigue syndrome. Neuropsychobiology 1997;35:115–22. [36] Ball N, Buchwald D, Schmidt D, et al. Monozygotic twins discordant for chronic fatigue syndrome. Objective measures of sleep. J Psychosom Res 2004;56:207–12. [37] Zucconi M, Oldani A, Ferrini-Strambi L, Smirne S. Arousal fluctuations in non-rapid eye movement parassomnias: the role of cyclic alternating pattern as a measure of arousal instability. J Clin Neurophysiol 1995;12:147–54. [38] Gillberg M, Akerstedt T. Sleep restriction and SWS-suppression: effects on daytime alertness and night-time recovery. J Sleep Res 1994;3:144–51. [39] Guilleminault Lee JH, Chan A, Lopes MC, et al. Non-REM-sleep instability in recurrent sleepwalking in pre-pubertal children. Sleep Med 2005. [40] Guilleminault C, Kirisoglu C, Bao G, et al. Adult chronic sleepwalking and its treatment based on polysomnography. Brain 2005;128:1062–9. [41] Lopes MC, Rosa A, Roizenblatt S, et al. Cyclic alternating pattern in peripubertal Children. Sleep 2005;28:215–9. [42] Guilleminault C, Kirisoglu C, Rosa A da, et al. Sleepwalking, a disorder of NREM sleep instability. Sleep Med 2006;7:163–170. [43] Schenck CH, Pareja JA, Patterson AL, Mahowald MW. Analysis of polysomnographic events suroundind 252 slow wave sleep arousals in 38 adults with injurious sleepwalking and sleep terrors. J Clin Neurophysiol 1998;15:159–66. [44] Pressman MR. Hypersynchronous delta sleep EEG activity and sudden arousal from slow-wave sleep in adults without a history of parasomnia: clinical and forensic implication. Sleep 2004;27:706–10. [45] Bao G, Guilleminault C. Upper airway resistance syndrome 1 decade later. Curr Opin Pulm Med 2004;10:461–7. [46] Chervin RD, Burns JW, Subotic NS, et al. Correlates of respiratory cycle-related EEG changes in children with sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep 2004;27:116–22. [47] Chervin RD, Burns JW, Ruzicka DL. Electroencephalographic changes during respiratory cycles predict sleepiness in sleep apnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2005;171:652–8. [48] Guilleminault C, Huang YS, Kirisoglu C, Chan A. Is OSAS a neurologic disorder? A CPAP follow-up study. Ann Neurol 2005;58: 880–7. [49] Lentz MJ, Landis CA, Rothermel J, Shaver JL. Effects of selective slow wave sleep disruption on musculoskeletal pain and fatigue in middle aged women. J Rheumatol 1999;26:1586–92. [50] Vergara JM, Brualla J, Serrano M, et al. Subjective sleep quality and spectral analysis of electroencephalography during nocturnal sleep. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1999;152:765–7.