* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download CAS LX 522 Syntax I
Lexical semantics wikipedia , lookup
Kannada grammar wikipedia , lookup
Japanese grammar wikipedia , lookup
Ukrainian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Zulu grammar wikipedia , lookup
Macedonian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Agglutination wikipedia , lookup
Compound (linguistics) wikipedia , lookup
Old Irish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Udmurt grammar wikipedia , lookup
Modern Hebrew grammar wikipedia , lookup
Latin syntax wikipedia , lookup
Morphology (linguistics) wikipedia , lookup
Arabic grammar wikipedia , lookup
Modern Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup
Esperanto grammar wikipedia , lookup
Portuguese grammar wikipedia , lookup
Lithuanian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Latvian declension wikipedia , lookup
Comparison (grammar) wikipedia , lookup
Ojibwe grammar wikipedia , lookup
Icelandic grammar wikipedia , lookup
Swedish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Ancient Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup
Old Norse morphology wikipedia , lookup
Literary Welsh morphology wikipedia , lookup
Romanian nouns wikipedia , lookup
Yiddish grammar wikipedia , lookup
English plurals wikipedia , lookup
Spanish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Old English grammar wikipedia , lookup
Scottish Gaelic grammar wikipedia , lookup
Turkish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Malay grammar wikipedia , lookup
Polish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Serbo-Croatian grammar wikipedia , lookup
CAS LX 522 Syntax I Week 1b. Morphosyntactic features ch. 2.1-2.4.1 The atoms of the system Syntax tells us which arrangements of words make good sentences. But yet the words themselves don’t seem to matter, they aren’t the basic elements of the system. Rather, it is the set of properties each word has that seem to be basic. Verb or not a verb, plural or not plural… Properties… features… Words have properties. There is an abstract concept of plural, that is morphologically realized in several different ways. A deer ate my bagel. Deer are funny. A dog ate my bagel. Dogs are funny. A goose ate my bagel. Geese are funny. The dog wants food. The dogs want food. The dog is hungry. The dogs are hungry. I am hungry. We are hungry. If the subject is plural (has a plural feature) then the verb must take on a “plural” form. Crosslinguistically common to have this kind of agreement relation between subject and verb. The plural feature is interpretable on the subject, contributes to the meaning. On the verb, the (agreeing) plural feature is uninterpretable—more on that later. What are the features? Il a dit qu’ elle était malade he[3.sg] have[3.sg] said that she was ill ‘He said that she was ill.’ Ils ont dit qu’ elle était malade they[3.pl] have[3.pl] said that she was ill ‘They said that she was ill.’ Standard 3-line format for examples from other languages (example, gloss, translation). Why does it matter what other languages do? Data from other languages In English, the subject and the verb of a sentence need to agree in number and (for be) person. Same “agreement” requirement, regardless of the actual morphological shape. The abstract property of “plural” (or “singular”) seems to be what the grammar is sensitive to. (Morphosyntactic) features. *enthusiastic are students the the students are enthusiastic *the student are enthusiastic the student is enthusiastic the students are enthusiastic *the students is enthusiastic this coffee is/*are hot. these muffins are/*is tasty. Agreement Some features matter for syntax, some don’t. No language says that subject and verb must agree in the feature [invented in early September], although there are things that have this property. For the purpose of describing the grammar and explaining syntactic principles, we don’t care about [invented in early September]. We have evidence, however, that [plural] matters to syntax. We’re looking for the minimal (least complicated) set of features that suffices to explain the grammar. 1 [plural] [plural] We know number matters. In English, things can be singular or plural. So, a first guess is that nouns have either a [singular] feature or a [plural] feature. Hypothesis: [sg] and [pl] are features a word can have. Prediction: Four classes of words: [sg], [pl], [sg.pl], []. But we really only have two classes in English. This hypothesis overgenerates—it predicts the existence of the actual distinctions, but it also predicts other distinctions that don’t exist. Already we have the basic structure of our theory and a means of analysis evaluation. Two independent features [pl] and [sg] predict four combinations, overgenerates. All attested combinations are predicted. Some predicted combinations are not attested. All predicted combinations are attested. Some attested combinations are not predicted. There is a simpler story we can tell, one that predicts exactly two classes. [plural] for plurals, [] for singulars. What kind of thing is a feature? An analysis that says “All words are singular” undergenerates. We observed the data (nouns can be singular or plural in English), we stated a hypothesis, which made predictions. We check the predictions… and it doesn’t seem right. The scientific method. Overgeneration / undergeneration Although features are “properties,” there are several views that have been taken on features. If we view a feature like [plural] as being either there or not, it is a privative feature. We might also view a feature like [plural] as having one of two values: [+plural] for plurals, [-plural] for singulars. This is a binary valued feature. We don’t know from the outset which view is the best for describing syntax, we want to choose the one that captures the generalizations we see. Duals For English, either a privative [plural] feature or a binary-valued [±plural] feature would work. In English there are two classes for number, singular and plural. Some languages also have a dual, a number reserved for pairs. Classical Arabic, for example, and Hopi. Hopi morphology Pam taaqa wari that man ran[sg] ‘That man ran.’ Puma ta?taq-t yu?ti those man[pl] ran[pl] ‘Those men ran.’ Puma ta?taq-t wari those man[pl] ran[sg] ‘Those two men ran.’ In Hopi, the dual is expressed by combining singular and plural. If we analyzed dual as [+pl, +sg] (or as [pl, sg]), we have a kind of explanation for that. 2 The fourth number? Three numbers are attested in the world’s languages: singular, plural, and dual. We can handle this by going back to the view that [sg] and [pl] are independent. The fourth possibility should be neither. But there doesn’t seem to be a fourth number. Hypothesis: General constraint on grammars: Nouns must have some number feature, [sg] is the default, added in if there is no number feature already. Singular: [sg] Plural: [pl] Dual: [sg, pl] Words and language Let’s take a moment to lay out the general structure of this theory. Knowing a language is knowing the “words” knowing how to put them together knowing how to pronounce them knowing what they mean in combination. (We’ll return to this) The lexicon To construct a sentence, we start with the “words” and put them together. We can describe the knowledge of the words of a language as being a list, a mental lexicon. Interfaces this is essentially the picture on p. 146 of the textbook We can view a “word” as a bundle of features, as defined by its properties. The grammar assembles words into sentences. The sentence is interpreted and pronounced. Lexicon Grammar A-P system Tension For English, it seems that independent [sg] and [pl] features is more complicated than we need, it seems to overgenerate. In the broader picture, Language needs to allow for independent [sg] and [pl] features in order to accommodate duals in, e.g., Hopi. Since we’re striving to explain the grammatical system underlying all languages, we need a hypothesis about what is different in languages with no dual (e.g., English). One possibility: The feature [sg] is not recorded in the English lexicon. Book [], books [pl]. All languages have singulars, but in languages without duals, singular is the default, the “number for nouns not specified for number.” So languages can differ in whether they record [sg] in the lexicon. C-I system The assembly process is the grammar proper. The system that interprets sentences is another cognitive module concerned with meaning, reasoning, etc. It interprets the constructed sentence at the interface. The system that determines the pronunciation of sentences is yet another cognitive module, interpreting the constructed sentence at its interface. What are the features? Hard to say. A universal set, some used in some languages, but not others? Learned? Some features seem not to exist, why? Ockham’s razor again—we want to define the simplest set of features we can to explain the data. 3 Category The study of syntax is concerned with distribution. Words seem to come in distributional classes. Distribution examples For example, one class of words can appear after the possessive pronoun my (my book, *my at, *my quickly, *my explode, *my purple). The nouns. One class of words is compatible with past tense. The verbs. One class of words is compatible with comparative (happier). The adjectives. Words can be separated into classes: noun, verb, adjective, preposition, etc. They have no noun. They can verb. They are adjective. Very adverb, very adjective. Classes also vary with respect to the kind of morphological endings they can have, and so forth. (Arrival, replacement, destruction; widen, computerize) so long as it makes sense (e.g., with gradable adjectives; #they are very absent) Right preposition right over the house Nouns and verbs Nouns have a category feature [N]. Books [N, pl]. Verbs have a category feature [V]. Binary vs. privative Complained [V] There’s something unsettling about saying the prepositions simply lack category features (neither nominal nor verbal). We can soothe ourselves somewhat by adopted binary category features instead of privative features: Two independent features, four predicted categories: [N, V] [] (adjectives) (prepositions) [+N, -V] [+N, +V] [-N, -V] [-N, +V] noun adjective preposition verb Same predictions, but more in line with our intuition about what “category” should be. [±N, ±V] The [±N, ±V] category system may seem a bit “out of the blue.” But it does yield some descriptive benefit. Consider what un- can attach to: untie, unfold, unwrap, unpack unhappy, unfriendly, undead *uncity, *uncola, *unconvention *unupon, *unalongside, *unat Basically, it applies to reversible verbs and adjectives, but not to nouns or prepositions. How can we state that in terms of our category features? 4