Download Beyond Genetics Dr Craig Albertson

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Pathogenomics wikipedia , lookup

Epigenetics of neurodegenerative diseases wikipedia , lookup

Dual inheritance theory wikipedia , lookup

Site-specific recombinase technology wikipedia , lookup

Polymorphism (biology) wikipedia , lookup

Gene expression programming wikipedia , lookup

Epigenetics of human development wikipedia , lookup

Minimal genome wikipedia , lookup

Adaptive evolution in the human genome wikipedia , lookup

Gene expression profiling wikipedia , lookup

Genetic testing wikipedia , lookup

Twin study wikipedia , lookup

Gene wikipedia , lookup

Artificial gene synthesis wikipedia , lookup

Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance wikipedia , lookup

Genome evolution wikipedia , lookup

Nutriepigenomics wikipedia , lookup

Behavioural genetics wikipedia , lookup

Public health genomics wikipedia , lookup

Genetic engineering wikipedia , lookup

Designer baby wikipedia , lookup

Population genetics wikipedia , lookup

History of genetic engineering wikipedia , lookup

Quantitative trait locus wikipedia , lookup

Genome (book) wikipedia , lookup

Human genetic variation wikipedia , lookup

Koinophilia wikipedia , lookup

Biology and consumer behaviour wikipedia , lookup

Heritability of IQ wikipedia , lookup

Microevolution wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Beyond Genetics
Dr Craig Albertson
BEYOND GENETICS
First, a large number of genes have been
identified for their involvement in the
development of the head, but there is no
direct evidence that these genes play a
role in the normal variation of the face. As
Dr Albertson explains: ‘For instance, if you
mutate a gene in a mouse, and you end up
with a mouse with no head, you may deduce
that that gene is necessary for building a
head. But it tells you nothing about how
the head is shaped over development. Why
do some people have longer mandibles
while others have shorter mandibles, for
example? This represents a major gap in our
knowledge, and likely requires the combined
action of both genes that the environment.’
Dr Craig Albertson and his colleagues at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst conduct research in evolutionary
developmental (evo-devo) biology. The team’s research focus is at the intersection of genes, development and
evolution, using the craniofacial skeleton in bony fishes as their primary model. Dr Albertson’s goal is to decode
the black box of development and to challenge the prevailing view that development within an organism and
adaptation among organisms is being driven, largely, by genetic variation.
It’s likely that many genes that are not
known for their role in early craniofacial
development contribute to normal variation
in the face. This is because the genes that
contribute to development are most active
in the first days or weeks of embryological
development (depending on whether you are
a fish or a human), whereas variations can
arise during both early (e.g., patterning) and
later (e.g., growth) processes. Thus, variations
in facial shape may come from unanticipated
genetic sources.
Shifting the Central Dogma
The classic Central Dogma of biology
provides a pathway for how genetic
information flows from DNA to yield
a phenotype. This flow follows the
transcription of DNA into RNA, which is
subsequently translated into protein.
Thus, as plainly stated, the Central Dogma
recognises a gene-centric view of a
biological system. This means that, based
on the Central Dogma, all of the information
necessary to define the state of an organism
is contained in the sequence of its genome.
This is actually a pretty fair assessment of
molecular biology when one is describing
simple single-cellular organisms, such as
bacteria. However, this gene-centric view
does not hold up when studying complex
multicellular organisms. It is well known
for example that monozygotic twins, with
identical genomes, can act, behave and even
look quite different. Clonal lines of plants
grown under drought or wet conditions
will display very different and stable
phenotypes. Butterflies that metamorphose
in spring will have more dark pigment on
their wings compared to the same species
that metamorphose in the summer. These
observations suggest that there is further
information that is responsible for the
production and maintenance of phenotypes.
‘As it turns out, genes cannot tell
the entire story. Rather, we want to
know how genetic and epigenetic
processes combine to tell the story
of life, of development
and evolution.’
Another thought provoking notion is
that genes play a very minor role in the
development of complex morphologies.
For example, while it is clear from decades
of research that genetic variation causes
dissimilarities at the phenotypic level, for
complex morphologies such as the facial
skeleton genetic variation can only explain
a small percentage of the total phenotypic
variation, typically much less than 50%. This
means that the majority of variation in such
phenotypes arises from different sources.
Given that the Central Dogma does not
provide a complete explanation for biological
processes in various fields of biology, many
disciplines are shifting this paradigm to
better describe biology. One such discipline
is evo-devo – a field of biology that combines
evolutionary biology and developmental
biology. Many researchers in this field
believe that the basic Central Dogma cannot
define complex traits; for example, if gene
expression was simply occurring as the
Central Dogma describes, then scientists
W W W.SCIENTI APUBLIC ATI ONS .CO M
could reconstruct a human face from a DNA
sample. This would mean that DNA from
a crime scene could be used to generate
a facial image of a suspect, the remains
of prehistoric people could be used to
reconstruct their faces or the face of a baby
could be predicted before birth using DNA
from the amniocentesis. This is an exciting
idea, but in reality it’s actually quite farfetched. There are several reasons that DNA
alone cannot predict facial shape.
It is becoming increasingly evident that
explanations in biology, and specifically
evo-devo, cannot purely rely on the Central
Dogma to explain the complex biological
processes that permit the development of
complex traits like the skeletal system. These
are the types of questions that Dr Albertson
has been interested in from a young age,
when he worked as an assistant to an oral
and maxillofacial surgeon. It was during this
time that he first gained an appreciation for
the skull as an organ of unique complexity:
‘I asked myself, how did that happen? How
does the skull arise over development?
How did the vertebrate skull evolve from an
invertebrate “head”? How does variation
in skull shape arise within and between
populations?’
In driving toward answers to these questions,
Dr Albertson and his team focus their studies
on the genetic and epigenetic (i.e., other
than genes) processes that contribute to
craniofacial development and evolution.
Some current research directions are
highlighted below.
Variation in a single locus can alter
multiple aspects of a dynamic system
Traditionally, the focus of evolutionary
developmental biology was to link genes
with straightforward shifts in morphology,
but in order for a species to adapt to new
environments, a divergence in resource
use must also occur. This divergence often
involves variation in multiple components
of complex functional systems. Based on
this idea, Dr Albertson and his colleagues
predicted that, for evo-devo research, the
elucidation of broad evolutionary processes
should investigate more dynamic traits
within this framework.
W W W . SC I EN TI A P UBLI C A TI ON S. COM
Dr Albertson’s work focuses on the
diversification of craniofacial morphology
and during vertebrate evolution. The
adaptive variation of craniofacial structure
occurs in response to different food sources
and habitats, which in turn plays a role in
niche partitioning and speciation. Actually, a
majority of the morphological and functional
divergence between vertebrates occurs in
the craniofacial region. Thus, it’s no surprise
that numerous studies have examined the
patterns of craniofacial divergence in a wide
variety of animals. Of the animals studied,
East African cichlids are considered an
excellent system to study the genetic and
developmental mechanisms that promote
craniofacial divergence, because they exhibit
a large degree of variation in craniofacial
morphology.
To study variations in the craniofacial
skeleton, Dr Albertson and his team
combined traditional quantitative trait
locus mapping, population genetics and
experimental embryology to understand how
differences in gene expression during larval
development in African cichlids contributes to adaptive morphological
variation in adults. In one study, Dr Albertson’s team showed that a
gene in the Hedgehog (Hh) signalling pathway mediates widespread
variation in bones that affects the kinematics of lower jaw depression
in cichlids. This seemingly simple action is controlled by a complex
arrangement of bones and tendons, and the efficiency of this system
has major implications for how well an animal can feed. This finding
contributes to the one of the key questions in evolutionary studies,
which asks how genetic variation translates into ecomorphological
adaptation and ultimately, fitness. Dr Albertson’s study presents
experimental data that variation in a single gene affects various aspects
of a dynamic mechanical system. Ultimately, this work links adaptive
variations at genetic, developmental and functional levels.
Meet the researcher
Epigenetics – a divergence from the Central Dogma
Epigenetics is the study of cellular and physiological phenotypic trait
variations that are caused by external or environmental factors that
turn genes on and off. While the study above highlights the genetic
roles for adaptive variation in the jaw, these genetic effects only
contribute to a relatively small percentage of the phenotypic variation
that is observed. Cichlids rapidly evolve and display a large spectrum of
variation in their craniofacial skeletons, and Dr Albertson is interested
in how these changes occur at the genetic level. However, his team also
found that there is an epigenetic origin for the adaptive variation that
occurs in the cichlid jaw.
The team revealed that cichlid larvae begin gaping their mouths
right after the cartilaginous lower jaw forms, which is just prior to the
commencement of bone development. They showed further that the
frequency of gaping varies between species in a manner that predicts
variations in bone deposition. Moreover, when the gaping is disrupted
in the fast gaping species, the jaw forms in a manner that is similar to
the slow gaping species. In contrast, when the gaping is forced to occur
at a higher frequency, the jaw of the slow gaping species develops to
resemble that of the fast gaping species. Finally, Dr Albertson and his
colleagues showed that these epigenetic changes also occur through
the Hh signalling pathway. Thus, both the genetic and epigenetic path
to jaw shape variation appears to leverage the same pathway. Taken
together, these results emphasise the complexity of how craniofacial
shape takes place and propose a novel experimental framework for
investigating sources of phenotypic variation beyond those determined
by changes in gene sequences.
Environmental influences on gene deployment during trait
development
Phenotypic plasticity refers to the ability of organisms to alter their
phenotype in response to changes in the environment. Recently,
phenotypic plasticity has been a hot topic in discussions of
evolutionary potential, but a complete understanding of the genetic
basis of plasticity is lacking. Dr Albertson and his team investigated the
genetic basis of phenotypic plasticity in cichlid fishes. They crossed
two divergent species to generate a genetic mapping population.
These hybrid families, at early juvenile stages, were split and raised
in alternate foraging environments that mimicked benthic/scraping
or limnetic/sucking modes of feeding. The different environments
produced variations in morphology that were largely comparable to
the major axis of divergence among the cichlids, which supported
the flexible stem theory of adaptive radiation – a theory proposing
‘While we know a lot about
how the head develops, we
know much less about how
the complex geometry of
the skull is determined’
that patterns of plasticity in a population will shape future patterns
of morphological evolution. In addition, the study revealed that the
genetic architecture of nearly every morphological trait examined was
highly sensitive to the environment. In other words, different genes
underlie shape variation in different environments. These results
have major implications for research aimed at identifying the genes
that underlie species divergence. They suggest that results could
be misleading if the experiments are not performed in the ‘correct’
environment, which is to say the environment in which evolutionary
divergence occurred.
Natural selection acts upon phenotypic variation. This notion has not
changed since the time of Darwin. Recent work in Dr Albertson’s lab
highlights the importance of genes in generating variation, however it
also underscores the immense significance of the environment. As the
field moves forward, it will be necessary to incorporate both variables
(genes and environment) into a common framework in order to gain a
comprehensive and mechanistic understanding of how species evolve.
W W W .S C I E NT I AP U BL I C AT I O NS . C O M
Dr Craig Albertson
Associate Professor
Department of Biology
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
USA
Dr Craig Albertson is an Associate Professor in the Department of
Biology at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst, where he studies
the development and evolution of complex morphologies, using the
craniofacial skeleton in bony fishes as his main experimental model. Dr
Albertson was awarded his PhD from the University of New Hampshire
in 2002 for a thesis entitled Genetic Basis of Adaptive Morphological
Radiation in East African Cichlid Fishes. He then went on to carry out
postdoctoral research on zebrafish developmental genetics at the
Forsyth Institute and Harvard School of Dental Medicine. He won
the Ernst Mayr Award in Evolutionary Biology, is an NSF CAREER
award grantee, has been the Keynote or Invited speaker at numerous
conferences, and received the College of Natural Sciences Outstanding
Teacher Award in 2016.
CONTACT
E: [email protected]
T: (+1) 413 545 2902
W: https://www.bio.umass.edu/biology/about/directories/faculty/rcraig-albertson
LAB MEMBERS
Jim Cooper (Postdoctoral Fellow 2007–2011, now Assistant Professor,
Washington State University, Tri Cities)
Nicole (Jacobes) McDaniels (PhD student 2007–2011, now Assistant
Professor, SUNY Herkimer)
FUNDING
NSF
NIH
KEY REFERENCES
KJ Parsons, M Conith, D Navon, J Wang, I Ea, K Groveas, C Campbell, RC
Albertson, Foraging environment determines the genetic architecture
and evolutionary potential of trophic morphology in cichlid fishes, Mol
Ecol., 2016, In press. DOI: 10.1111/mec.13801.
Y Hu, RC Albertson, Hedgehog signaling mediates adaptive variation in
a dynamic functional system in the cichlid skull, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA, 2014, 111, 8530–8534.
KE Powder, H Cousin, G McLinden, RC Albertson, A non-synonymous
mutation in the transcriptional regulator lbh is associated with cichlid
craniofacial adaptation and neural crest cell development, Mol Biol
Evol., 2014, 31, 3113–24.
Moira Concannon (OEB PhD student)
Dina Navon (OEB PhD student)
Douglas Calenda (MCB MS student)
KJ Parsons, AT Taylor, KE Powder, RC Albertson, Wnt signalling
underlies the evolution of new phenotypes and craniofacial variability
in Lake Malawi cichlids, Nat Commun., 2014, 5, 3629.
LAB ALUMNI
Kara Powder (Postdoctoral Fellow 2011–2016, now Assistant Professor,
Clemson University)
Yinan Hu (OEB PhD student 2009–2015, now Postdoctoral Fellow, URI)
Kevin Parsons (Postdoctoral Fellow 2009–2012, now Lecturer, University
of Glasgow)
KJ Parsons, RC Albertson, Unifying and generalizing evo-devo’s two
strands, Trends Ecol Evol., 2013, 28, 584–591.
W W W . SCI EN TI A P UBLI CA TI ON S. C OM